Social work practitioners and the human--companion animal bond: a national study.
Risley-Curtiss, Christina
A growing body of research supports the powerful relationships
between humans and companion and other animals: both positive and
negative. Companion animals may assist children and adults in feeling a
sense of security and unconditional love (Risley-Curtiss, Holley,
Cruickshank, et al., 2006), contribute to a child's cognitive and
language development (Melson, 2001), and contribute to an elderly
person's ability to carry out daffy activities (Raina,
Waltner-Toews, Bonnett, Woodward, & Abernathy, 1999). Service
animals enhance independence and quality of life for children and adults
who have ambulatory and other kinds of challenges (Duncan & Allen,
2000). Companion animals, however, are also victims of human cruelty,
and there is growing evidence of a link between animal cruelty, child
maltreatment, domestic violence, and increased criminality (Ascione,
2005).
Evidence of the powerful relationships between humans and companion
animals, as well as the fact that the majority of people with such
animals consider them to be part of their family, supports the premise
that the social work profession should be informed about these
relationships and skilled in including companion animals in their
practice. A review of the social work literature, including major
textbooks, and experience in social work education and practice
indicates that companion animals have not traditionally been included as
significant others in clients' environments. Recent related
research further suggests that integration of companion animals into
current social work may not be happening. A study of cross-reporting
between child welfare workers and humane society workers found that a
number of child welfare workers thought cross-reporting was unimportant,
were resistant to including animal welfare in their assessments, and
underreported concern for animal well-being (Zilney & Zilney, 2005).
Risley-Curtiss (2004) found that only seven out of 230 schools of social
work that responded to a survey included much content on the
human-companion animal bond (HCAB) in their courses and that what was
offered was mostly about animal-assisted therapy. Finally, Ascione
(2005) asserted that "developmental psychology and related
disciplines have virtually ignored the positive role that pets and other
animals may play in the lives of children" (p. 5). Social work is
one such related discipline. The purpose of this study is to examine
what social work practitioners know about the HCAB and whether they are
including such relationships in their practice.
SOCIAL WORK AND COMPANION ANIMALS
Companion animals should be integrated into social work research,
education, and practice because of their interconnectedness with humans.
This interrelatedness plays out three ways that are essential for the
social work profession include: (1) Companion animals are usually
considered to be family members and, thus, part of family systems; (2)
animal cruelty by children or adults is very deviant behavior that is
commonly correlated with a dysfunctional home life, indicates a need for
mental health services, and is related to many forms of human oppression
(for example, violence against women and children); and (3) companion
animals can have a therapeutic impact on the functioning of people of
all ages. Although these areas are discussed in the next sections, they
are not discrete categories but are very much intertwined. For example,
abused children may be more likely than nonabused children to talk to
their companion animals regarding their troubles and to see them as a
means for overcoming loneliness (Robin, ten Bensel, Quigley, &
Anderson, 1984).
Companion Animals as Family
In the United States, 62 percent of households have a companion
animal (American Pet Products Manufacturers Association, 2003). The
majority of those with companion animals consider them family.
Risley-Curtiss and colleagues, in two different studies on ethnicity and
companion animals, found that 97 percent (Risley-Curtiss, Holley, &
Wolf, 2006) and 87 percent (Risley-Curtiss, Holley, Cruickshank, et al.,
2006) of participants agreed that their pets are members of their
families. The Pew Research Center (2006) found that 85 percent of dog
owners and 78 percent of cat owners felt the same way.
Considering companion animals as family members means that they are
one of the subsystems within the complex family system and, as such,
both influence and are influenced by every other family system (Melson,
2001). Family animal-human interactions can result in such behaviors as
companion animals sleeping with family members; sharing family
members' food; being confided in and read to, and having their
birthdays celebrated. "We often overlook the fact that pets are
important not only for children but for every member of the family"
(Levinson, 1997, p. 122). Albert and Anderson (1997) found that women
talked about how their companion animals raised family morale. Cain
(1983) found, in her study of the characteristics of pet relationships
in 60 families, that 81 percent felt that their pets were sensitive to
the moods of other family members, and some related that when their
family was stressed or in conflict, their pet manifested physical
symptoms such as loss of appetite and diarrhea. Thus, companion animals
may mirror family tensions and critical situations (Levinson, 1997). In
a study of 896 military families, Catanzaro (1984) found companion
animals to be very important during the temporary absence of a spouse or
child, childhood and adolescence, lonely or depressed times, crises such
as the illness or death of other family members, or relocation and
unemployment. Companion animals can act as stabilizers in these
situations because they offer love, affection, and unconditional
acceptance. Companion animals also help families learn about certain
life experiences such as responsibility, caregiving, and loss and death.
Moreover, companion animals may sacrifice their own health or give their
lives for family members by "functioning as sentinels of unsafe
environmental conditions" (Jalongo, Stanek, & Fennimore, 2004,
p. 54).
Animal Cruelty
Animal cruelty by children or adults is considered to be a very
serious, alarming behavior. For children it may well be one of the early
manifestations of conduct problems associated with "low empathy and
callous disregard" (Dadds, Whiting, & Hawes, 2006) and should
be viewed as needing intervention (Merz-Perez & Heide, 2004). In
addition, animal cruelty, in some form, is illegal in every state. A
substantial body of research suggests a correlation between animal
cruelty and antisocial behaviors (see Merz-Perez & Heide, 2004),
including oppression and persecution ofmarginalized subgroups (Adams
& Donovan, 1995; Wolf, 2000). Merz-Perez and Heide suggested that
cruelty to companion and other animals may be an indicator that
individuals are at risk themselves of having violence committed against
them. A number of participants in Merz-Perez and Heide's study of
offenders had observed their parents committing abusive acts against the
offenders' beloved pets .The pet abuse committed was also cruelty
against these offenders in psychological form. In the case of battered
women, Adams (1995) described pet abuse as a unique form of emotional
battering (see also Faver & Strand, 2007). Women whose pets are
threatened, harmed, or killed experience fear for themselves and their
animals. They may decide they have to give up their pets to a shelter
(where they may be killed) or others to avoid harm. In these cases the
women can experience profound grief over the loss of their pets and the
relationship with their pets. If the women have children who also
experience this loss, then the children suffer this loss with them. In a
study by Ascione, Weber, and Wood (1997), 39 children of battered
mothers were interviewed: 66.7 percent had witnessed pets being hurt by,
among other things, strangulation, poisoning, and shooting. More than
half(51.4 percent) said they had protected a pet from a perpetrator.
A link between performing acts of animal cruelty and having
observed such acts is also suggested in studies involving animal cruelty
and family violence. Ascione et al. (1997) found, in their study of
companion animal abuse experiences of abused and nonabused women, that
61.5 percent of the abused women reported their children witnessing pet
abuse, in contrast to 3.3 percent of the nonabused women. More than 13
percent of the children who had witnessed such abuse reported that they
themselves had hurt a pet by, among other things, throwing, hitting, or
stepping on the animal. Similar evidence suggests that for social
workers, animal abuse may well be a means of identifying parallel
dynamics within the larger family group (Hutton, 1998). Research also
supports links between animal, child, and elder abuse. For example,
children who have been sexually or physically abused are more likely
than nonabused children to abuse animals (Ascione, 2005).
Therapeutic Impact of Companion and Other Animals on Humans
The literature, both professional and popular, is replete with
evidence of the positive effects that animals can have on humans--more
than can be adequately reviewed here. However, examples include
increased length of walking time and significantly lower serum
triglycerides (Dembicki & Anderson, 1996). Risley-Curtiss, Holley,
Cruickshank, et al. (2006) found that the women in their study
identified receiving friendship, fun, love, comfort, and
constancy--either for themselves, their children, or both--and
protection from their animal companions. Because of the powerful
connections that humans can have with companion animals, animals can
also be positive adjuncts in treatment of clients (Fine, 2000; Levinson,
1997).
This positive impact has been recognized as far back as the middle
of the 18th century with the planned introduction of pets into the care
of people with mental illnesses at "The York Retreat" in
England (Levinson, 1997). In 1969 Boris Levinson described, in his
seminal book Pet-oriented Child Psychotherapy, how companion animals
could hasten the development of rapport between therapist and patient,
thereby increasing the likelihood of patient motivation, and how the
inclusion of animals could be helpful in psychological assessment,
psychotherapy, and pet-oriented therapy in residential settings; in
working to motivate the exceptional child for learning; and in family
therapy (Levinson, 1997). In 1988 Cusack reviewed research on the
positive connection between mental health and pets specifically related
to depression, stress and anxiety, psychiatric patients, children,
adolescents, family, elderly people, people who are physically
challenged, and people in prison. At the same time the National
Institutes of Health (1988) convened a workshop on the human health
benefits of pets. Beck and Glickman (1987) ended the workshop by
proposing that all future studies of human health should include the
presence or absence of companion animals in humans' lives and,
where present, the nature of this relationship as a significant
variable.
More recently, Garrity and Stallones (1998) cautiously concluded
that benefits from companion animal association occur on the
psychological, physical, social, and behavioral levels and are probably
both a direct benefit to humans and a protective or buffering factor
when humans face life crises. Melson (2001) wrote, "the study of
children has been largely 'humancentric,' assuming that only
human relationships ... are consequential for development" (p. 5),
yet "the ties that children forge with their pets are often among
the most significant bonds of childhood, as deeply affecting as those
with parents, sibling, and friends" (p. 16).
Given the importance of the bond between companion animals and
humans, the purpose of this study was to explore what social work
practitioners know and are doing in relation to the HCAB. The three
major questions guiding the research were as follows: (1) Do social work
practitioners have exposure to and knowledge of the HCAB in relation to
social work issues (for example, health, family, children)? (2) Are
social work practitioners including companion animals in their
assessment and treatment of clients? (3) Are social workers receiving
professional education and/or training on how to include companion or
other animals in their practice?
METHOD
Sampling
A random sample of 5,012 NASW members who identified as
clinical-direct practitioners with BSW, MSW, or PhD degrees was drawn
from the 2004-2005 NASW membership mailing list. The participants were
surveyed by mail between June and December 2005. Eighteen participants
were dropped after the second mailing because no mail was being posted
to New Orleans following Hurricane Katrina. Three mailings were returned
because of incorrect addresses. To increase response rates, a small
lottery incentive involving four Amazon.com gift certificates was
included. Three survey mailings were completed, with the first and third
including a cover letter and hard-copy survey and the second, a postcard
reminder (Dillman, 2000). Participants also had the option to complete
the survey online using Monkeysurvey.com. The final sample consisted of
1,649 respondents (of 4,991), for a 33 percent response rate.
Measures
The research questions were operationalized as 38 questions
designed to obtain information regarding exposure to information on the
HCAB and knowledge and integration of the HCAB into social work
practice. Content validity was established through a review of the
literature and through a review by two international experts in the
human-other animal bond field. It was also pilot tested with a group of
students.
Exposure to Information/Knowledge of Animal--Human Relations. Two
strategies were used to ascertain the level of knowledge participants
had about the human-other animal bond. First, participants were asked to
rate, on a three-point scale (1 = very little/none, 2 = some, and 3 = a
lot), how much they had heard or read about human-other animal
relationships. A set of nine topics covered the link between animal
abuse and other forms of violence, the positive influence of companion
and other animals on various age groups, and treatment of clients who
have abused companion and other animals or had experienced the loss of a
companion animal. Second, participants were asked whether they agreed or
disagreed with five statements (for example, "The elderly are the
population least likely to benefit from animals in their lives"). A
five-point scale (1 = strongly agree, 2 = agree, 3 = disagree, 4 =
strongly disagree, 5 = don't know) was used to avoid the test
anxiety that a simple yes/no response might engender. Categories for
exposure were collapsed in the analysis to two: very little/none and
some/a lot. Knowledge categories were collapsed to three: strongly
agree/agree, strongly disagree/disagree, and don't know. Internal
consistency for the nine exposure items was measured using
Cronbach's alpha and was an acceptable .82. Internal consistency
was not measured for the knowledge questions, as they were intended to
measure different concepts, not a single construct.
Inclusion of Companion and Other Animals in Assessment and
Treatment. Another series of questions asked if participants ask
questions about companion and other animals in their intake assessments
(yes/no) and, if so, what kinds of questions (for example, do they have
companion animals, has anyone in their family hurt their animals) and
whether they include animals in their treatment of clients (yes/no) and,
if so, what type of animals and why, and if not, why not. Finally, they
were asked whether they treat clients for animal loss or cruelty
(yes/no).
Education and Training. Practitioners were asked whether they have
any special training in including animals in social work practice
(yes/no); if yes, what kind of training; and whether their social work
coursework included information on any of the following: animal cruelty
and/or abuse, animal--assisted activities/therapy, or the positive
effects of animals in people's lives, with each category coded as a
dichotomous variable. Participants were also asked if they would like to
learn more about the human-animal bond (yes/no).
Demographics. Gender was measured as a dichotomous variable, age in
total years, income as current annual income in thousands, and ethnic
identify as their "primary" ethnic identity using NASW's
mailing list categories. Questions and response categories on the
participants' social work practice (for example, primary work
setting, major area of practice, primary work function, client
population) replicated those used from NASW membership mailing list.
RESULTS
These data analyses rely on descriptive statistics such as
frequencies and means.
Sample Description
Almost 80 percent (n = 1,621) of the participants were female and
white (92 percent, n = 1,612), with a mean age of 53 years (SD = 7.8).
Mean annual income was $60,393 (n = 1,358, SD = $37,461). Participants
had an average of 21 years of post-BSW/ MSW experience (SD = 7.9).This
sample is similar to that for the 2002 NASW Practice Research Network
(PRN) (2003) survey, which found that most NASW regular members were
female (79 percent) and white (87 percent), with a median age of 50.
This study sample, however, had more years of practice than the mean 16
years for the NASW PRN survey.
In this study sample, 95.7 percent (n = 1,630) had MSWs, 59.3
percent (n = 1,584) practiced in the area of mental health, and 35.3
percent of those stated they work in private practice. The majority of
participants (78.1 percent, n = 1,580) identified their primary work
function as clinical-direct practice, with 51.9 (n = 1,516) percent
serving primarily nonelderly adults and 44.1 percent (n = 1,539)
focusing on individual problems.
Exposure to Information on Animals
Participants were asked how much they had heard or read about the
link between animal abuse and human violence, the positive influence of
companion animals on humans, and treatment of clients who abuse animals
and who have experienced the loss of a companion animal.The majority of
those responding had read or heard some/a lot about the link between
animal and child abuse (78.1 percent), domestic violence (69.8 percent),
and criminal behavior (85.2 percent). Even more participants had heard
or read some/a lot about the positive impact of animals on adults (97.8
percent), children (92.1 percent), and elderly people (97.9 percent).
Most (69.7 percent) had not heard much about treatment of clients who
abuse animals but had heard about treatment for loss of a companion
animal (71.2 percent).
Knowledge of the Animal-Human Bond
Participants were asked to respond to five statements about humans
and nonhuman animals. Two statements were worded to be correct, and
three were incorrect. Participants ranked the statements from strongly
disagree to strongly agree. Categories were collapsed from five to three
(strongly disagree/ disgree, agree/strongly agree, don't know).
Most participants (84.9 percent) agreed or strongly agreed that
"More than half of U.S. households have at least one dog or cat,
"whereas 15.1 percent did not know the correct response. Over 68
percent did not know the correct response to "One of the two least
common fears of children is of animals"; 31.4 percent correctly
disagreed. Most participants (83 percent) disagreed correctly that
"Bonds with companion animals are simply substitutes for human
relationships." Almost 88 percent of participants correctly agreed
that "It has been demonstrated that people who repeatedly and
intentionally harm animals are more likely to show violence towards
people," and 95.3 percent correctly disagreed that "The
elderly are the population least likely to benefit from animals in their
lives."
Animals in Assessment and Treatment
Two-thirds (n = 1,091) of participants reported that they do not
include questions about companion or other animals in their intake
assessments. Even fewer reported including animals as part of their
interventions in social work practice (23.2 percent, n = 381). Of those
who do include questions about nonhuman animals in their assessments,
508 ask if their clients have pets, 145 ask if they have other animals
(for example, farm animals), 202 ask if anyone in the family has hurt
their animals, and 289 ask about what place the animals have in the
client family. The most common responses in the "other"
category had to do with loss of a pet (39),who cares for their pets
(13), who will care for the pet in case of death or illness (6), and
names of pets (8).
Of the 381 who included animals in their interventions, 86 include
animal-assisted activities such as visiting elderly people, 143 do
animal-assisted therapy (that is, animal is part of treatment plan), and
49 include animals in inpatient residences. The animals most commonly
included in participants' practices were dogs (n = 320) and cats (n
= 167). However, a broad variety of other animals were also included,
such as birds, "pocket pets" (for example, hamsters, rats,
guinea pigs), horses, farm animals (cows, goats, sheep, and so forth),
fish, reptiles, and rabbits. Qualitative responses to how participants
involved animals in practice included 79 having an animal of their own
present in therapy sessions, 26 asking or allowing clients to bring
their own animals to sessions, 30 recommending getting a pet to clients,
and seven discussing the benefits of pets with clients. Fifty-eight
percent of respondents thought the best reason for including other
animals in their interventions was because clients care about animals
(emotional health); 31.2 percent thought that clients open up more in
therapy. The five most common reasons for not including animals in their
practice were (1) against agency policy; (2) client allergies; (3) fear
of liability fear of an animal hurting a client, or clients' fears
of animals; (4) had not thought of doing so, and (5) lacked training.
Almost 93.6 percent of those who responded (n = 512) do not treat
clients for animal cruelty; 57.7 percent do treat clients for companion
animal loss and grief.
Education and Training
The vast majority of the 1,621 who responded (95.7 percent) said
that they have not had any special training in including companion or
other animals in their practice. Furthermore, 82.2 percent of those who
include animals in practice reported having no special training to do
so. Almost 63 percent said they had no social work course content
regarding animals or did not remember such content. For those who did
have course content, 22.4 percent said they had information on animal
cruelty/abuse, 25.7 percent on the positive effects of animals on
people, and 12.6 percent on animal-assisted activities or therapy.
Finally, 79.3 percent said they would like to know more about the
human-animal bond.
Limitations
Several limitations are important to consider. First,
generalizability may be limited due to a lower response rate than
desired, despite using multiple methods. We have no information on why
people did not respond or whether nonresponders are significantly
different from responders. Nonetheless, the sample demographics appear
to be similar to NASW's own, and the sample does not appear to be
biased in favor of those most interested in animals, as might be
anticipated. There is also some evidence that lower response rates may
not harm data quality as much as feared. For example, Keeter, Miller,
Kohut, Groves, and Presser (2000) found very few differences in
responses between a five-day phone survey with a 36 percent response
rate and an eight-week study with a 60.6 percent response rate. Second,
the measures of knowledge (exposure to and agreement or disagreement
vAth statements) are proxy measures, not true measures. Concern for
"test anxiety" led to not explicitly testing knowledge. The
measures used are believed to provide some preliminary indication of how
much participants know about different but critical aspects of the HCAB.
DISCUSSION AND IMPLICATIONS
Although it appears that the participants in this study have some
knowledge of both the negative and positive aspects of the HCAB, the
vast majority of social work practitioners in this study are not
including companion animals in their practice. Especially concerning are
the findings that only one-third ask about animals at all in assessment
and that only 12 percent of the whole sample (n = 1,649) ask clients
about animal cruelty. Many are not including companion animals in their
practice because they have not been educated or trained to do so.
An alarming finding is that of those who are including animals in
their practice, most are doing so without the necessary training or
education. The reasons participants gave for not including animals in
practice showed the lack of education and training about the benefits
and various ways animals can be included in practice, a lack of
understanding of the importance and usefulness of identifying animal
cruelty in clients, and a lack of information on the vast numbers and
types of agencies and organizations including animal-facilitated work.
For example, some participants stated that they work in a hospital
setting and were concerned about liability. Although these are
legitimate concerns, education would inform practitioners of the many
hospitals that now have animal-assisted therapy programs (for example,
Banner Health Hospital Systems has such programs in most of their
hospitals), and one well-known organization, the Delta Society (www.deltasociety. org),includes a $1 million insurance policy in their
"pet partners" certification. Thus, these concerns can be
overcome, as evidenced by the proliferation of animal-assisted therapy
programs throughout the United States, including the Eden Alternative
for long-term residential living (www.edenalt.com); Green Chimneys, a
residential facility for at-risk children (www.greenchimneys.org); and
equine-assisted therapy (www.eagala.org).
This lack of preparation in human--companion animal relationships
suggests that although the social work profession values diversity, it
is a "humancentric" or "speciesist" (Wolf, 2000)
diversity. A serious consequence of disregarding human-companion animal
relationships is that it shortchanges our ability to help our clients.
For example, social workers are increasingly providing home-based
services as the core of their service provision. This affords them an
opportunity to repeatedly interact with companion and other animals in a
nonthreatening manner. With appropriate knowledge and training, social
work professionals can be in a position to do much to help people
enhance their own lives by assisting with their companion animals. They
can link clients to low-cost veterinary services and to food banks that
provide animal food. They can help families understand the need to spay or neuter their animals and direct them to affordable spay or neuter
services. Through budgeting, social workers can even assist individuals
(for example, the foster child aging out of the system) and families in
deciding whether they can afford a companion animal. They can validate
the importance of the nonhuman family member to their client families
and maximize their work with those families by drawing on the positive
impact such animals can have for family members. Finally, they can
identify and treat clients with animal abuse histories. Identifying and
treating animal abuse early may help clients avoid related troubles in
the future.
Given the ever-growing body of evidence supporting the importance
of human-other animal relationships in early identification of potential
problems and regarding the potential for companion animals to help
individuals and families build resiliency, it is incumbent on the social
work profession to join other professions and disciplines in efforts to
delve into, and build on this bond. If social work practice is to be
truly anti-oppressive and ecologically grounded (which requires one to
see humans in the context of their environments and as constantly in
reciprocal interaction with significant others), then the inclusion of
the HCAB is essential. However, for practitioners to include
human-companion animal relationships, they need to be informed by
research through education and training. The findings of this study
suggest that this has not happened. One reason is the lack of social
work research on human-companion animal relationships. Previous research
shows that companion animals are a part of clients' ecologies;
thus, to truly conduct research from an ecological perspective, the
presence or absence of companion and other animals must be considered.
Hence, a major implication for social work research is the need to
expand the lense to include the presence or absence of companion
animals, and the nature of those relationships, in research on the
well-being and treatment of individuals and families with regard to
issues such as oppression, health, social support, and violence. A
wealth of valuable information, and for some, a critical aspect of their
life is missed, when, for instance, longitudinal studies of families or
children do not include any questions on the presence and relationship
of animals to the human subjects. In addition, animal-assisted
activities and therapy are also being used by some social work
practitioners and by many of the agencies within which social workers
practice (Fine, 2000). Given an increasing emphasis on evidence-based
practice, this suggests that social work researchers should also join in
efforts to evaluate such activities and programs.
The findings that most of the participants have received neither
education nor training on the HCAB even when including animals in
practice and that a majority would like to know more about this bond
have implications for social work education and training. Information on
the HCAB needs to be integrated into the curriculum at all levels of
social work education. Although social work education is already
struggling to be inclusive of many important social-environmental
topics, the integration of nonhuman animals can be done with minimal
effort and time and is related to racism (see Risley-Curtiss, Holley,
Cruickshank et al., 2006; Risley--Curtiss, Holley, & Wolf, 2006),
sexism and oppression (Adams & Donovan, 1995), homelessness (many
homeless people will not go to shelters because they cannot take their
pets), economics (44 percent of low-income participants in
Risley-Curtiss, Holley, & Wolf, 2006, had companion animals), and
health disparities. Simply modeling the inclusion of companion animals
in genograms; in ecomapping; and in definitions of family, support
systems, and environment can raise awareness and legitimize the need to
ask clients about companion animals. Identifying and using texts and
articles that include companion animal issues (for example, see Ashford,
LeCroy, Lortie, & Brougham's, 2006, Instructor's Manual
with Test Bank [Perez family case study]), such as the link between
animal cruelty and other forms of family violence and how to assess for
other animal relationships, is critical for courses in human behavior,
social work practice, families and children, domestic violence, and
child welfare. The therapeutic impact that companion and other animals
can have for children, families, and the elderly population, as well as
such diagnostic information as the typical age that children might begin
abusing animals and motivations for doing so, can be easily woven into
human behavior or mental health courses (animal abuse is one criterion
in conduct disorders). Courses that focus on evidence-based individual
and agencywide interventions should also include information on
animal-assisted interventions, their efficacy, and their potential
usefulness for many different difficulties and age groups. For instance,
having animals is especially helpful for the elderly population as a
source of social support and as an aid to physical and emotional health.
Cherished companion animals can give elderly people a reason to get up
in the morning and to "keep living." This same information
should be provided through continuing education workshops for
postgraduate social workers. These workshops should be planful and
repeated to ensure that all practitioners are aware of the importance of
companion animals in the lives of clients. Education and training must
address the issue through discussion that it does not matter whether
practitioners like, dislike, are afraid of, or are even interested in
companion animals, they must critically assess the part companion
animals may or may not have in their clients' lives and the
potential for therapeutic interventions facilitated by companion and
other animals. Although some clients may also dislike or fear animals,
and attitudes and practices regarding animals may vary by culture, these
beliefs and behaviors cannot be determined if practitioners do not ask.
This study indicates that some social workers are including animals
in their practices. These numbers need to he greatly expanded because
practitioners can significantly improve their client service with a
thorough understanding of the impact of companion animals on individuals
and families. All practitioners should be asking about the presence of
animals in client lives and, if found, about the relationships with such
animals. In addition, although not all practitioners need to do
animal-assisted work, they should understand the potential benefits of
and differences between animal-assisted activities and therapy and
should consider referrals to programs that do include animals (for
example, hippotherapy, equine-assisted psychotherapy).
The social work profession's global mission is to help people
enhance their well-being. Without expanding to include companion animals
in social work research, education, and practice, the profession fails
to maximize its potential to do so.
REFERENCES
Adams, C.J. (1995).Woman-battering and harm to animals. In C.J.
Adams & J. Donovan (Eds.), Animals & women: Feminist theoretical
explorations (pp. 55-84). Durham, NC: Duke University Press.
Adams, C.J., & Donovan, J. (Eds.). (1995). Animals & women:
Feminists theoretical explorations. Durham, NC: Duke University Press.
Albert, A., & Anderson, M. (1997). Dogs, cats and morale
maintenance: Some preliminary data. Anthrozoos, 10, 121-124.
American Pet Products Manufacturers Association. (2003, April 14).
Leading trade organization releases study finding pets owned by more
than half of all U.S. households. Retrieved January 29, 2004, from
www.appma.org/ press/press_releases/2003/nr_04-14-03.asp
Ascione, F. R. (2005). Children and animals: Exploring the roots of
kindness and cruelty. West Lafayette, IN: Purdue University Press.
Ascione, F. R., Weber, C.V., & Wood, D. S. (1997, April 25).
Final report on the project entitled: Animal welfare and domestic
violence. Logan, UT: Author.
Ashford, J., LeCroy, C.W, Lortie, K. L., & Brougham, J. (2006).
Human behavior in the social environment: Instructor's manual with
test bank. Belmont, CA: Thomson Brooks/Cole.
Beck, A. M., & Glickman, L.T. (1987, September 10-11). Future
research on pet facilitated therapy: A plea for comprehension before
intervention. Paper presented at the NIH Technology Assessment Workshop:
Health Benefits of Pets, Washington, DC.
Cain, A.O. (1983).A study of pets in the family system. In A. H.
Katcher & A. M. Beck (Eds.), New perspectives on our lives with
companion animals (pp. 72-81). New York: Haworth Press.
Catanzaro, T. E. (1984).The human-animal bond in military
communities. In R. K. Anderson, B. L. Hart, & L. A. Hart (Eds.), The
pet connection (pp. 341-347). Philadelphia: University of Pennsylvania
Press.
Cusack, O. (1988). Pets and mental health. New York: Haworth Press.
Dadds, M. R., Whiting, C., & Hawes, D.J. (2006). Associations
among cruelty to animals, family conflict, and psychopathic traits in
childhood. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 21, 411-429.
Dembicki, D., & Anderson, J. (1996). Pet ownership may be a
factor in improved health of the elderly. Journal of Nutrition for the
Elderly, 15(3), 15-31.
Dillman, D. A. (2000). Mail and Internet surveys: The tailored
design method (2nd ed.). New York: John Wiley & Sons.
Duncan, S. L., & Allen, K. (2000). Service animals and their
role in enhancing independence, quality of life and employment for
people with disabilities. In A. Fine (Ed.), Handbook of animal-assisted
therapy (pp. 303-323). San Diego: Academic Press.
Faver, C.A., & Strand, E. B. (2007). Fear, guilt, and grief:
Harm to pets and the emotional abuse of women. Journal of Emotional
Abuse, 7, 51-70.
Fine, A. (2000). Handbook of animal-assisted therapy. San Diego:
Academic Press.
Garrity, T. E, & Stallones, L. (1998). Effects of pet contact
on human well-being. In C. C. Wilson & D. C. Turner (Eds.),
Companion animals in human health (pp. 3-22). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage
Publications.
Hutton, J. S. (1998). Animal abuse as a diagnostic approach in
social work: A pilot study. In R. Lockwood & E R. Ascione (Eds.),
Cruelty to animals and interpersonal violence (pp. 415-418). West
Lafayette, IN: Purdue University Press.
Jalongo, M. R., Stanek, M. L., & Fennimore, B. S. (2004).
Companion animals at home:What children learn from families. In M. R.
Jalongo (Ed.), The world's children and their companion animals:
Developmental and educational significance of the child/pet bond (pp.
47-60). Olney, MD: Association for Childhood Education International.
Keeter, S., Miller, C., Kohut, A., Groves, R. M., & Presser, S.
(2000). Consequences of reducing nonresponse in a national telephone
survey. Public Opinion Quarterly, 64, 125-148.
Levinson, B. M. (1997). Pet-oriented child psychotherapy (2nd ed.).
Springfield, IL: Charles C Thomas.
Melson, G. F. (2001). Why the wild things are: Animals in the lives
of children. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Merz-Perez, L., & Heide, K. M. (2004). Animal cruelty: Pathway
to violence against people. NewYork: Altamira Press.
National Association of Social Workers Practice Research Network.
(2003). Demographics. Retrieved September 9, 2009, from
http://www.socialworkers.org/ naswprn/survey/Two/Datagram2.pdf
National Institutes of Health. (1988). Health benefits of pets:
Summary of working group. Washington, DC: U.S. Department of Health and
Human Services.
Pew Research Center. (2006, March 7). Gauging family intimacy: Dogs
edge cats (dads trail both): A social trends report. Washington, DC:
Author.
Raina, P., Waltner-Toews, D., Bonnett, B. G., Woodward, C., &
Abernathy, T. (1999). Influence of companion animals on the physical and
psychological health of older people: An analysis of a one-year
longitudinal study. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society, 47,
323-329.
Risley-Curtiss, C. (2004). Human--other animal bond related content
and programs in schools of social work. Unpublished study, Arizona State
University, Tempe.
Risley-Curtiss, C., Holley, L. C., Cruickshank, T., Porcelli, J.,
Rhoads, C., Bacchus, D., et al. (2006). "She was family":
Women of color and their animal-human connections. AFFILIA, 21, 433-447.
Risley-Curtiss, C., Holley, L. C., & Wolf, S. (2006). The
animal-human bond and ethnic diversity. Social Work, 51,257-268.
Robin, M., ten Bensel, R.W., Quigley, J., & Anderson, R. K.
(1984).Abused children and their pets. In R. K. Anderson, B. L. Hart,
& L.A. Hart (Eds.), The pet connection (pp. 111-130). Minneapolis:
University of Minnesota.
Wolf, D. B. (2000). Social work and speciesism [Commentaw]. Social
Work, 45, 88-93.
Zilney, L. A., & Zilney, M. (2005). Reunification of child and
animal welfare agencies: Cross-reporting of abuse in Wellington County,
Ontario. Child Welfare, 84, 47-66.
Christina Risley-Curtiss, PhD, MSSW,, is associate professor,
School of Social Work, Arizona State University, Department of Public
Programs, Phoenix, AZ; e-mail: risley.curtiss@asu.edu. The author thanks
the William and Charlotte Parks Foundation for Animal Welfare for their
financial support in this endeavor, Dr. Robert Leighninger for his
helpful comments regarding the manuscript, and Dr. Tim Tyrell for his
statistical consultation. Technically, the correct terms for what we
commonly call animals is "other animals" or "nonhuman
animals"(which is hierarchical), as humans are not separate from
animals but are one species of animal. Although the author prefers the
term "other animals," the more commonly used terms--"pets
"or "companion animals"-have been primarily used. The
term "other animals" has been used in reference to farm and
wild animals. The author lives with 20 companion animals: 18 cats, one
dog, and one horse.
Original manuscript received December 7, 2006
Final manuscript received March 1, 2008
Accepted March 6, 2008