The animal--human bond and ethnic diversity.
Risley-Curtiss, Christina ; Holley, Lynn C. ; Wolf, Shapard 等
Keeping companion animals (that is, pets) is a universal cultural
phenomenon (Brown, 002). In the United States, dogs or cats or both are
found in at least one-third of households: Approximately 64 million
households have a companion animal, including 77.7 million cats and 65
million dogs (American Pet Products Manufacturers Association, 2003).
An ever-increasing body of literature suggests that affectionate
relationships with animal companions have health-enhancing effects on
people and enrich their quality of life (see Lago, Delaney, Miller,
& Grill, 1989; Netting, Wilson, & New, 1987; Sable, 1995).
Research has also suggested that these relationships are complex and
vary depending on a number of study population characteristics (Wilson
& Netting, 1987). It stands to reason that ethnic and cultural
differences may be among the factors that either enhance or inhibit such
effects and that an understanding of the effect of ethnicity on
relationships with companion animals is essential. Unfortunately,
although there is a growing body of research on the animal--human bond,
there appears to be little attention paid in the broad professional
literature to ethnic and cultural beliefs, values, and practices
regarding companion animals (Kaufman, 1999), and there is essentially
none in the social work literature.
THE ANIMAL--HUMAN BOND AND SOCIAL WORK
The health and well-being of companion animals and humans have long
been intertwined but have only recently been the subject of empirical
study. Fortunately, knowledge of this relationship is growing, and there
is much evidence attesting to the powerful connections between people
and their animals, both positive and negative (Faver & Strand, 2003;
Netting et al., 1987; Sable, 1995). Yet these animal--human connections
have received virtually no attention from social work scholars, despite
the fact that, for example, abuse and cruelty to companion animals in
families is somewhat commonplace and has disturbing consequences for
both humans and animals (see, for example, Deviney, Dickert, &
Lockwood, 1998; Faver & Strand; Flynn, 2000). In addition, social
work and other human service practitioners are increasingly including
animals as adjuncts to treatment without social work research to inform
and support them.
A recent Social Work Abstracts search of articles published since
1977 produced 25 references using the key word pet, with only 22
articles that addressed people's social relationships with animals.
The key word animal drew 47 references, 33 of which referred to such
things as animals in substance abuse or mental health research. Thus, in
27 years there have been no more than about 30 isolated articles on
animal companions and humans in the social work literature.
Existing research has largely been conducted by those in
psychology, psychiatry, and veterinary medicine and is often from an
individual psychopathogical perspective. In contrast, social work is
grounded in an ecological-systems perspective that requires one to view
humans within the context of their environments and as constantly in
reciprocal interaction with significant others (Hepworth, Rooney, &
Larsen, 2002). Sixty-two percent of U.S. households have reported owning
a companion animal (American Pet Products Manufacturers Association,
2003), which means that social workers are very likely to work with
families with companion animals. Furthermore, because 68 percent of
Americans consider companion animals as family (Brookman, 1999), the
inclusion of companion animals in both practice and research should be a
natural extension of social workers' work with humans and their
challenges, coping mechanisms, and resilience factors. Recent related
research, however, has suggested that this inclusion may not necessarily
be happening. A study of 203 psychologists found although 94 percent
believed animal abuse to be connected to other behavioral disturbances,
only 14 percent assessed for such abuse (Nelson, 2002). A study of
cross-reporting between child welfare workers and humane society workers
found that a number of child welfare workers thought cross-reporting was
unimportant and were resistant to including animal welfare in their
assessments. These child welfare workers also underreported concern for
animal well-being (Zilney & Zilney, 2005). Finally, an unpublished
study by Risley-Curtiss (2004) found that only seven of 230 schools of
social work that responded to a survey included much content on the
human--animal bond in their courses, and what was offered most often
dealt with animal-assisted therapy. Recognizing the potential impact of
companion animals on people's lives could significantly affect
social workers' abilities to help their clients. With appropriate
knowledge and training, social work professionals are in a position to
do much to enhance the lives of both people and their companion animals.
LITERATURE REVIEW
As described earlier, there is a dearth of literature on the effect
of ethnic and cultural diversity on the animal--human bond in the United
States. The 2002 edition of the U.S. Pet Ownership and Demographics Sourcebook, published by the American Veterinary Medical Association,
does not include ethnicity in its companion animal owner demographics.
In a study on the link between corporal punishment and children'
cruelty to animals, Flynn (1999) incidentally noted that white people
were somewhat more likely to commit animal abuse than people of color.
Unfortunately, even with a sample about one-fifth African American,
Flynn failed to take the opportunity to explore these ethnic differences
in this and other articles drawing on the same data (for example, see
Flynn, 2000). As noted by Kaufman (1999): "Rarely has animal
protection in general, and the violence link specifically, been
discussed in the context of a multicultural society" (p. 260).
Some ethnic differences have been found in companion animal
ownership and attachment. Marx and colleagues (1988) examined
demographics of companion animal ownership among U.S. adults ages 21 to
64 and found that white people were statistically more likely than
people of color to have companion animals. Nonetheless, although race
was identified as a significant factor, there was no discussion or
examination of this finding. In a more in-depth examination, Siegel
(1995) interviewed 877 urban non-Latina/Latino white, Latina/Latino,
African American, Asian, and "other" adolescents between the
ages of 12 and 17. The author found distinct racial or ethnic
differences in companion animal ownership and attachment. White
adolescents were most likely to have companion animals in their homes,
followed by Latinos/Latinas and those of Asian descent. African
Americans were least likely to have such animals in their homes. White
youths also rated their companion animals as significantly more
important to them than did Latina/ Latino youths, whereas African
American and Asian youths reported intermediate levels of attachment but
were not significantly different from any other group. Having sole
responsibility for the care of a companion animal was also related to
race and ethnicity, with African American youths most likely to have
sole responsibility, followed by Latinas/Latinos, white, and Asian
youths. Brown (2002) examined racial variations in pet attachment among
76 African American and 57 white veterinary students at one school.
Among her findings was that more white students had companion animals
(100 percent compared with 86 percent); they also had a larger number of
such animals and were more likely to sleep with their companion animals
than were African American students (70 percent compared with 53
percent). White students also had significantly higher scores on a pet
attachment questionnaire than did African Americans. For example, there
were differences in responses to such attachment statements as "No
family is complete without a pet" and to questions about taking
pets to visit friends and relatives. Brown concluded that how attachment
to animals is shown may vary from culture to culture. She also noted
that differences in housing, socioeconomic factors, and urban-rural
background may aid in explaining the racial differences she found.
Johnson and Meadows (2002) examined 24 community-dwelling
Latinas/Latinos age 50 and older regarding their relationships with
their companion animals (dogs) and whether attachment to a companion
animal was related to self-perceived health and functional ability. The
majority of participants viewed their dogs as equals (54 percent) and as
members of their family (79 percent). Sixty-seven percent stated that
their dog was the reason they got up in the morning, and 62 percent
stated that their dog comforted them. Although the study results did not
support a relationship between animal companion relationship and
participant's health or functional ability, the authors concluded
that for these Latinos/Latinas, dogs were as important as they have been
reported to be among white people and that dog companions were
considered, among these elderly Latinas/Latinos to be valued members of
their families.
Finally, the nonprofessional Best Friends magazine recently
published the first in a planned series of articles on the lack of
people of color in the animal welfare field (Richard, 2004). This first
report is on African Americans and is anecdotal, but it makes an
important observation regarding the lack of people of color in animal
welfare work. Richard noted that "White people often believe that
minority communities simply don't care about animals" (p. 14).
This attitude is implied throughout the animal bond literature by the
treatment of all companion animal owners as one and by the apparent lack
of interest in studying populations of color (Kaufman, 1999).
Richard (2004) and Kaufman (1999) clearly indicated a need for more
information on the possible effect of race and ethnicity in
understanding people's relationships with companion animals. Do
people from various racial and ethnic groups perceive that they derive
similar benefits from their companion animals? Do companion animals play
similar roles in the lives of people from various ethnic groups? Answers
to these questions will allow social workers to conduct more complete,
culturally relevant assessments of individual and family well-being; to
better understand whether and when different types of animals have the
potential to assist individuals and families in healing from trauma or
in enriching their quality of life; and to assist social workers in
group and other facilities in assessing the appropriateness of
incorporating animal-assisted activities and therapy into their
interventions. Social work, as a profession, needs to join the ranks of
other disciplines in building its knowledge base about animal--human
connections.
THE STUDY
The Setting
The survey was conducted by a survey research laboratory (SRL) in a
large southwestern metropolitan county. It was the pilot for a proposed
ongoing survey that will be carried out on an annual or semiannual
basis. The survey was done by telephone between July 10, 2003, and
August 6, 2003. A total of 587 interviews were completed.
Interviewers who carried out the survey were paid staff of the SRL
with a minimum of 30 hours of training. Calls were made from the SRL
phone bank, where shift supervisors monitored interviewers and provided
feedback for each shift on standardized interviewing practices. Data
were collected using a computer-assisted telephone interviewing system
that administered the sample, ensured proper skip sequences, and allowed
only valid punches.
Sampling
The sample was drawn as a list-assisted random digit dial, using
the Genesys System. The Genesys database contains current listings of
all active area codes and central offices in the United States, along
with a database of all listed telephone numbers. The county area codes
were specified. Within the 584 working exchanges, numbers were drawn
from 100 blocks that had at least three listed numbers. This improves
the efficiency of sampling by avoiding 100 blocks that have been
assigned but not populated with residential phones, while still giving
full probability of inclusion to new construction, people who have
recently moved and are not listed, and people who have chosen not to be
listed in telephone directories.
A total of 4,790 telephone numbers were called. Because the person
in the household who answers the phone is not a random distribution, the
most recent birthday selection method (Lavrakas, Stasny, & Harpuder,
2000) was used to select a random respondent. This technique has the
advantage of not requiring a detailed enumeration at the beginning of
the interview.
Each number was called a minimum of 10 times, filling a grid of
morning, afternoon, evening, weekend, and weekday. If no contact was
made after 10 calls (all results were continuous busy signals or rings
with no answer), no further attempts were made. If any of the 10
attempts resulted in an indication that the number might have been a
residential household, calls continued up to a maximum of 20 attempts.
Once a residential household was reached and a respondent chosen, no
artificial limit was set on the number of callbacks. A total of 20,037
phone calls were made over the course of the project to the 4,790 sample
numbers.
The response rate was calculated using the American Association for
Public Opinion Research (AAPOR) Standard Definitions: Final Dispositions
of Case Codes and Outcome Rates for Surveys (AAPOR, 2004). Using
Response Rate 3, which estimates the proportion of cases of unknown
eligibility that are actually eligible, the response rate was 33.4
percent. Although this response rate is lower than we might like, there
is some recent evidence that lower rates may not harm data quality as
much as feared. For example, Keeter and colleagues (2000) found very few
differences in responses between a five-day phone survey with a 36
percent response rate and an eight-week study with a 60.6 percent
response rate.
Measures
The data for this study were a subset of the data from a larger
survey that consisted of 121 questions covering a variety of topics,
including how participants liked their environment, their views on
international adoption, and demographics such as gender, ethnicity, and
income. The independent demographic variables used in our subset of data
were racial/ethnic identities measured dichotomously. To avoid forcing
multiethnic participants to choose only one racial or ethnic identity,
race or ethnicity was measured in a series of separate categorical variables. Participants were asked if they were of Hispanic/Spanish
origin (yes/no) and if they described themselves as white (yes/no),
black/ African American (yes/no), American Indian (yes/ no), Asian
(yes/no), or Pacific Islander (yes/no). Note that although the survey
used the terms Hispanic/Spanish origin and American Indian, we are aware
of important critiques of these terms and therefore prefer the terms
Latina/Latino and indigenous peoples.
The dependent variables were selected from the 37 questions that
asked specifically about pet ownership and care and beliefs regarding
pets. Although we prefer the use of the more equitable term
"companion animal," we recognize that common usage continues
to be pet. Therefore, the word pet was used in the survey. Having a pet
was measured as a dichotomous variable (yes/no). What kind of pet
respondents had was measured in a series of dichotomous variables
(yes/no) that covered eight types: cat, dog, fish, horse, rabbit,
turtle, small rodent, and bird. All those having pets were asked whether
they agreed or disagreed with the statements "It's okay to
declaw cats" and "My pet is a member of my family." Those
with dogs, cats, or both were also asked what their pet offered them,
such as unconditional love (yes/no) or sense of personal safety
(yes/no), as well as questions regarding care of their animal (for
example, "Is your dog/cat spayed or neutered" [yes/no]).
Finally all participants (with and without pets) were asked whether they
agreed with belief statements such as "Animals do not feel
pain" and "Animals can feel sadness, fear, happiness, and
love."
Analyses
Data analyses rely on descriptive statistics such as frequencies,
chi-square tests, and forward stepwise regression. All the dependent
variables were categorical, with most, but not all, measured
dichotomously. Ethnicity, the independent variable, was measured as
multiple dichotomous variables (Hispanic/Spanish origin, white,
black/African American, American Indian, Asian and Pacific Islander).
Therefore we used chi-square to test for statistical significance
between the multicategorical dependent variables and the dichotomous
independent variables. Forward stepwise logistic regression was used to
examine the relationship between various ethnic identities and
dichotomous dependent variables. Forward stepwise regression starts with
only the constant in the equation (that is, no variables). At each step,
the variable with the smallest significance level for the score
statistic below the chosen cutoff value (.05) is entered into the model.
RESULTS
Sample Description
The majority of the 587 participants identified themselves as
white, not Hispanic (78.4 percent; n = 460) and female (60.1 percent; n
= 353) (see Table 1, Total Study Sample column) with a mean age of 46
years (SD = 16.5; range = 18-92). Another 12.3 percent identified
themselves as of Hispanic/Spanish origin. Of these 72 participants, 35
identified as Mexican American, 14 as Mexican, three as Puerto Rican,
two as Cuban, and 13 as "other"; five did not categorize their
Hispanic/ Spanish origin. Almost 75 percent of participants had some
education beyond high school, with 40.4 percent having attended
technical or trade school or some college; 21.1 percent having graduated
from a four-year college, and 16.1 percent having attended or completed
graduate school. Almost 63 percent of the sample were married or living
together as married, and 61.9 percent were working for pay. The median
annual household income ranged from $50,000 to less than $60,000, and
almost 60 percent made between $30,000 and $79,999 annually. The average
household size was 2.76 (SD = 1.4; range = 1-8), with 16 percent of
participants riving alone and 54.8 percent living with one or two other
people.
The study sample differs somewhat from the county population from
which it was drawn. The study sample included more women than in the
general population and fewer people between the ages of 18 and 24 (Table
1). However, 59.3 percent of the county population and 61.1 percent of
the study sample were 25 to 54 years old. The study sample was similar
to the county on income in the lower and highest income categories but
differed somewhat in the $50,000-$99,000 category, with more of the
study sample falling in this category. The study sample was also a
better-educated group, with high school nongraduates underrepresented.
Finally, Latinas/Latinos were underrepresented and indigenous peoples
were overrepresented in the sample population.
Companion Animal Ownership and Care
Almost 63 percent (n = 368) of the participants had pets at the
time of the survey. Dogs were the most common pet by a wide range, with
79.1 percent of participants with animals having dogs, 21.2 percent
having fish (see Table 2). Slightly more than 13 percent had both cats
and dogs.
The majority of participants with companion animals again were
white, not Hispanic (77.2 percent), female (60.6 percent), married or
living together as married (65.8 percent), and working for pay (67.3
percent). Similar to the entire sample, their mean age was 44 years old
(range = 18-92; SD = 14.6), 77.6 percent had some kind of education
beyond high school, and medium income was $50,000 to less than $60,000
annually.
In regard to the care of their animals, the most common residence
of cats and dogs was both inside and outside (55.7 percent), followed by
inside only/never going outside (35.2 percent). Slightly more than 85
percent (n = 300) said that their dog or cat was spayed or neutered, and
78.3 percent said they had a veterinarian for their dog or cat. The most
common reason for not spaying or neutering the dog or cat was
"planning to breed it" (18 percent) followed by "It is
not old enough" (12 percent). Three participants said they could
not afford it, and four did not believe that it is right to spay or
neuter animals. Slightly less than 30 percent (n = 103) obtained their
dog or cat from family or friends, 20.5 percent got their dog or cat
from a breeder, and 21.3 percent got their dog or cat from some sort of
shelter. About 11 percent said they obtained their animals from an
"other" source. We are unsure what those other sources were.
Finally, more than 97 percent of all pet owners agreed with the
statement "My pet is a member of my family."
Ethnic Differences Regarding Companion Animals
Companion Animal Ownership. Participants who identified themselves
as American Indian were most likely to have companion animals (73.5
percent; n = 25/34), followed by white people (65 percent; n = 319/491),
people of Hispanic/Spanish heritage (56.9 percent; n = 41/72), African
Americans (40.9 percent; n = 9/22), Pacific Islanders (40 percent; n =
2/5), and Asians (37.5 percent; n = 6/16). Logistic regression analysis
was done with the six ethnicity variables entered as independent
variables and pet ownership (yes/no) as the dependent variable. The
overall regression model was significant, with a model chi-square of
8.07 (p = .004). Two variables met the entry criteria: describing
oneself as white or as American Indian. Describing oneself as white
entered first. The odds of those identifying themselves as white having
pets increased by 48 percent compared with those not describing
themselves as white. After controlling for those identifying as white,
the odds of those identifying themselves as American Indian having pets
increased by 58 percent compared with those who did not so identify
themselves.
To explore whether there is a significant relationship between
specific types of companion animals and ethnic identity or identities,
we ran a series of eight logistic regressions. For each equation, all
six race and ethnicity variables were entered as independent variables
and one type of companion animal was entered as the dichotomous
dependent variable (that is, cat, dog, bird, rabbit, turtle, fish,
horse, or small animals such as mice, rats, and hamsters) .These
analyses revealed that identifying oneself as being of Hispanic/Spanish
origin significantly reduced the odds of having cats by 59 percent
(model [chi square] = 14.261,p = .01) and of having birds by 68 percent
(model [chi square] = 7.23, p = .007). Describing oneself as American
Indian increased the odds of having fish by 65 percent (model [chi
square] = 5.34, p = .02). No other significant relationships were found
between racial or ethnic group membership and type of companion animals.
Care of Companion Animals. Four questions were asked of those with
a cat, dog, or both regarding the care of these animals: (1) whether
their cat or dog was spayed or neutered; (2) what their reasons were for
not spaying or neutering; (3) whether their dog or cat resided inside
only, inside and outside, or outside only; and (4) whether they had a
veterinarian for their dog or cat. These questions were used as the
dependent variables in a series of logistic regressions with racial and
ethnic identities as the independent variables. Those who identified as
being of Hispanic/Spanish origin were 3.41 times more likely not to have
their animal spayed or neutered than those who did not identify as of
Hispanic/ Spanish origin (model [chi square] = 8.77, p = .003). We also
used the chi-square test of independence for two multicategorical
dependent variables: reasons for not spaying or neutering their cat or
dog and residence of dog or cat (inside only, inside and outside,
outside only). Given the high frequency of cells with expected counts of
less than five, we were unable to test for statistical significance
between ethnic identity and reasons given for not spaying or neutering
their dog or cat. When we collapsed the residence variable to two
categories (inside and outside versus outside only), the nonparametric
Fisher's Exact Test was significant for identifying as white (p =
.04). However when identifying as white was entered into a logistic
regression along with the other ethnicities as independent variables,
the model was not significant. Finally, cat and dog owners were asked
whether they had a veterinarian for their pet. Hispanic/Spanish origin
was the only ethnicity to enter the model with having a veterinarian for
their pet as the dependent variable (model [chi square] = 6.49,p = .01).
Those identifying themselves as of Hispanic/Spanish origin were 73
percent more likely not to have a veterinarian for their pet than were
those who did not identify themselves as of Hispanic/Spanish origin.
Relationships and Beliefs. Cat and dog owners were also asked a
series of questions regarding their relationships with their companion
animals, including what their animal offered them. Specifically they
were asked if their pets offered them emotional support, unconditional
love, companionship, a sense of personal safety, income from breeding,
and a chance to teach their child responsibility. In addition, all pet
owners were asked if they agreed to such statements as "My pet is a
member of my family" (agree/disagree), and "It is okay to
declaw a cat" (agree/disagree). These variables were used as
dependent variables with all ethnic identities as independent variables
in logistic regression analyses. No statistically significant
differences were found with regard to racial and ethnic identity and
emotional support, unconditional love from pet, companionship from pet,
and pets are family members. Those identifying themselves as Hispanic/
Spanish origin were 55 percent more likely to get a sense of personal
safety from their pet than were those not identifying themselves as
Hispanic/ Spanish origin (model [chi square] = 4.36, p = .04). Those
identifying themselves as Asian were 93 percent more likely to say they
got income from breeding than were those not identifying as Asian (model
[chi square] = 18.91, p = .000). It should be noted, however, that there
was a very small number of Asian Americans with pets (n = 6), and only
two (33 percent) said they got income from breeding. Those identifying
themselves as white were 2.44 times more likely not to feel that their
pets taught their children responsibility than were those not
identifying themselves as white (model [chi-square] = 5.10, p = .02).
Finally, the odds of those identifying themselves as white agreeing that
it is okay to declaw cats increased by 64 percent when compared with
those not identifying themselves as white (model [chi square] 8.56, p =
.003).
Participants were asked whether they agreed with another series of
statements, whether they had pets or not. These included "Pets
should be spayed/neutered early in life," "It's okay to
hit a pet if it misbehaves," "Animals do not feel pain,"
"Animals can feel sadness, fear, happiness, and love," and
"There are too many unwanted animals in the world. "All were
measured dichotomously as agree/disagree. Frequencies by ethnicity are
presented in Table 3. Statistical significance was found only in the
logistic regression model for "pets should be spayed/ neutered
early in life" and for whether animals feel pain. Those who
identified themselves as white were 0.59 times more likely to agree that
animals should be spayed or neutered early in life than those not
identifying themselves as white (model [chi-square] = 7.57,p = .006).
Those who identified themselves as Asian were 81 percent more likely to
agree that animals do not feel pain (model [chi square] = 4.58, p =
.03). In addition, although the overall model was not statistically
significant, identifying oneself as Asian was associated with
disagreeing that there are too many unwanted animals in the world (p =
.05).
Limitations of the Study
Before we discuss the implications of this study, several
limitations are important to consider. First, although a random sampling
technique was used, people of color were underrepresented in the sample
in comparison to their representation in the population from which they
were drawn. This underrepresentation limits the study's
generalizability. Furthermore, the population from which the sample was
drawn is one with relatively few people of Asian, African American, or
Pacific Islander descent. This limitation prevented detection of
significant differences that may actually exist among these racial and
ethnic groups. Future studies might use a stratified random sampling
technique to address this problem. Second, because of the exploratory
nature of the study and the need to limit the number of questions asked,
relatively few questions regarding human-companion animal attachment
were asked, and no questions were asked about why participants did not
have companion animals. Future researchers might benefit from asking
additional questions about the animal-human relationship (for example,
"Does your pet sleep in your bed with you?") and about factors
that influence the decision to have or not have companion animals.
Other research has suggested that care practices and beliefs may
vary depending on an individual's attachment to a companion animal
(Wilson & Netting, 1987). Finally, the use of stepwise regression
has limitations, in that a model is selected to fit a particular sample.
Thus, there is no assurance that the same model would be selected if
another sample from the same population were selected. In addition, the
model always fits the sample better than the population from which it
was selected (Norusis, 1990). One, therefore, must use caution in
interpreting the results, especially with regard to other populations.
DISCUSSION
It is interesting to note that the percentage of pet owners in our
study is almost exactly the same (62 percent versus 63 percent) as that
found in the 2002 American Pet Products Manufacturers Association survey
(American Pet Products Manufacturers Association, 2003). In addition,
this study's findings offer support that there are some ethnic
differences in whether people have animal companions and in the care of
and beliefs regarding companion animals. A higher percentage of
indigenous people indicated that they have pets; those who identified as
white were next most likely to have pets. This finding is particularly
important because no earlier studies have included indigenous people in
their study samples.
Furthermore, members of different racial or ethnic groups who do
have pets may prefer the companionship of different types of animals. In
this sample, those who identified as being Latina/Latino were
significantly less likely than others to have cats or birds, whereas
those identifying as indigenous were more likely to have fish. Although
no other statistically significant differences were found in types of
companion animals owned by members of various ethnic groups, it is
possible that a sample that includes larger numbers of people who
identify as African American, Asian, and Pacific Islander may reveal
other differences among groups.
Perhaps the most important finding is that among study participants
who had cats, dogs, or both, there were no significant racial or ethnic
differences in whether participants reported that they receive emotional
support, unconditional love, and companionship from their pets.
Furthermore, regardless of racial or ethnic group membership, almost all
companion animal owners described their animals as being family members.
This finding is consistent with earlier studies (Brookman, 1999) and
reinforces the importance of social workers' considering
clients' relationships with their companion animals in their work
with individuals and families (see Implications for Social Work Practice
section, below). Of course, companionship, emotional support, and so
forth may mean different things to different people, so further research
is needed to better understand these findings (see Implications for
Social Work Research section).
The finding that Latinas/Latinos report that their dogs or cats
give them a sense of personal safety is also critical to consider.
Because members of this group are less likely to have cats, it may be
assumed that they considered their dogs as providing this sense of
safety. Although further research is certainly needed to understand this
finding, it is not surprising that a social group that experiences
discrimination and other forms of oppression might gain a sense of
safety from having a dog companion.
This study found that those who identified themselves as white were
significantly different from others on three outcome variables. White
people were significantly more likely than others to say that pets
should be spayed or neutered early in life and that it is okay to declaw
cats and least likely to say that having a cat or dog teaches children
to be responsible. Further research is needed to learn whether this
first difference is related to socioeconomic status. The first
difference suggests a concern for the nonhuman animal world, whereas the
second and third differences appear to be more focused on humans'
needs or preferences. Further research is needed to better understand
these apparently complicated attitudes about companion animals.
IMPLICATIONS FOR SOCIAL WORK RESEARCH
Findings from this exploratory study have several implications for
future research. First and most critical, the finding that the vast
majority of participants with companion animals see them as family
members strongly indicates that social work researchers who are studying
individual and families must include, at a minimum, the presence of
companion animals--and their meaning to human participants from various
ethnic, gender, age, and other social groups--in their research.
Research from other disciplines has already suggested that animals can
play an important role in violent families experiencing domestic
violence or child abuse (for example, see Flynn 1999, 2000) in the
development of children (Melson, 2001), and in enhancing the fives of
elderly people (Baun & McCabe, 2000). By including animals in their
studies, social work researchers can add to this important, growing body
of knowledge and assist social work practitioners in implementing a more
inclusive ecological approach to work with clients.
In relation to ethnicity, additional quantitative studies need to
be conducted with samples that include larger numbers of people from
various racial and ethnic groups, including Latinas/Latinos who are not
of Mexican descent. Studies might also include measures of the strength
of ethnic identity (for example, see Cuellar, Arnold, & Gonzalez,
1995; Klonoff & Landrine, 1999; Resnicow, Soler, Braithwaite,
Selassie, & Smith, 1997; Roberts et al., 1999). Studies in other
areas have found that strength of ethnic identity, rather than simply
ethnic group membership, may affect attitudes and behaviors (for
example, see Lorenzo-Hernandez & Ouellette, 1998; Phinney, Cantu,
& Kurtz, 1997). Such studies also need to examine whether there are
differences in attitudes toward and relationships with animals among
people from various geographic regions, ages, genders, social classes,
and urban, rural, and suburban residences and those with different
amounts of leisure time and varying personal experience with companion
animals. Future studies might include additional questions that will
allow researchers to better understand people's relationships with
their companion animals. For example, are there ethnic differences in
whether it is appropriate to allow one's dog to sleep in one's
bed? Are there differences in how much time one spends with one's
cats? This study found that people obtain unconditional love, emotional
support, and companionship from their cats and dogs. Additional research
is needed to understand human relationships with other types of
companion animals. How would people from various ethnic, gender, and age
groups rate the importance of the benefits of having different types of
companion animals?
Qualitative studies are needed to assist in understanding what
these relationships mean to people. For example, when people say that
they get emotional support from their companion animals, what do they
mean? When do they turn to their animal for this emotional support? Are
there instances in which this emotional support is more important than
what one might obtain from a human companion--or from a social worker?
Can an animal who is not a long-term part of one's family of origin
or choice (for example, a horse in an outdoor program for youths; a cat
in a residential care facility) provide the same level of companionship,
unconditional love, and emotional support as a family's companion
animal? Does such an animal become part of an individual's family,
as study participants described? Are there ethnic, age, or other social
group differences and similarities in how people experience these
relationships?
Research is also needed to learn whether individuals living in
stressful environments (for example, higher crime areas, violent or
neglectful families) have different relationships with their companion
animals than do others. It is possible that people in such environments
may feel that the responsibilities of caring for an animal simply add
stress to their lives. Alternatively, such people may benefit from the
companionship, emotional support, and unconditional love of a companion
animal.
Unfortunately, it makes sense that some people who consider their
companion animals to be members of their families might abuse animals
just as they might abuse humans. More studies of the factors related to
animal abuse--particularly studies that consider whether there is a
relationship between animal abuse and human abuse are needed to help us
better understand both of these social problems.
Studies also need to explore the perspectives of the sizable minority of people who do not have companion animals. Is not having
companion animals related to living situation, income, formal
educational level, attitudes about animals, or something else?
Finally, available research suggests that many social workers may
not be including attention to animal--human relationships in their
practices. Additional studies are needed to understand if some social
workers are doing so and ways in which practice that includes attention
to such relationships affects the lives of clients.
IMPLICATIONS FOR SOCIAL WORK PRACTICE
The finding that the vast majority of dog and cat owners in this
study said that their animal companions provide them with emotional
support, unconditional love, and companionship and that they consider
their animals to be family members has tremendous implications for
social work practice, particularly in light of evidence suggesting that
attention to animals may currently be lacking (for example, see
Risley-Curtiss, 2004; Zilney & Zilney, 2005). First, these findings
support the practice of asking about the presence of animals and
including attention to individuals' relationships with their
companion animals, particularly cats and dogs, in biopsychosocial
assessments. If animals are present and not included, essential elements
of individuals' interactions with their environments will be
missing, and the interventions that follow may fail to draw on critical
resources for clients' health and well--being. In addition, if an
individual is strongly attached to her or his companion animal, an
adequate intervention may need to include providing resources for care
of a companion animal (for example, food; veterinary care, including
vaccinations; boarding services during hospitalization or other
out-of-home care of the person). Although such components are rarely
considered "social work interventions," they may indeed be
important for clients' well-being.
Findings also have implications for work with specific racial and
ethnic groups. For example, social workers who want to include animals
in their group care settings that serve Latinas/Latinos (at least those
who are of Mexican descent) may consider selecting dogs, rather than
cats or birds, for residence in their facilities. Findings also suggest
that facilities serving indigenous and white populations, in particular,
might improve their services by including animals in their programs.
Finally, findings suggest that adult group homes, shelters, and
other care facilities might consider the feasibility of allowing people
to bring their companion animals with them. Of course, there are many
difficult issues involved in such an arrangement (such as the potential
for an animal to harm another person or animal, or vice versa). Instead,
social workers might ensure that animals are well cared for when apart
from their human companions and arrange for visitation opportunities.
This could decrease individuals' worries about the well-being of
their animal companions, while continuing to provide benefits resulting
from the emotional support, companionship, and perceived unconditional
love of a companion animal.
Given the findings of this and other studies regarding the
importance of companion animals, social work researchers and
practitioners must pay attention to the presence of animals in the lives
of their clients. To give companion animals short shrift means we are
missing a potentially vital connection and are paying only lip service to our claims of an ecological approach.
Original manuscript received March 19, 2004 Final revision received
March 18, 2005 Accepted June 1, 2005
REFERENCES
American Association for Public Opinion Research. (2004). Standard
definitions: Final dispositions of case codes and outcome rates for
surveys (3rd ed.). Lexena, KS: Author.
American Pet Products Manufacturers Association. (2003, April 14).
Leading trade organization releases study finding pets owned by more
than half of all U.S. households. Retrieved January 29, 2004, from
www.appma.org/ press/press releases/2003/nr_04-14-03.asp
American Veterinary Medical Association. (2002). U.S. pet ownership
and demographics sourcebook. Schaumburg, IL: Author.
Baun, M. M., & McCabe, B.W. (2000).The role animals play in
enhancing quality of life for the elderly. In A. Fine (Ed.), Handbook of
animal-assisted therapy (pp. 237-251). New York: Academic Press.
Brookman, F. (1999, October 25).The things people do for pets!
Discount Store News. Retrieved January 29, 2004, from
www.findarticles.com/cf_dls/_m3092/ 20_38/57443535/p1/article.jhtml
Brown, S. E. (2002). Ethnic variations in pet attachment among
students at an American school of veterinary medicine. Society and
Animals, 10, 455-456.
Cuellar, I., Arnold, B., & Gonzalez, G. (1995). Acculturation Rating Scale for Mexican--Americans--II: A revision of the original
ARSMA scale. Hispanic Journal of Behavioral Sciences, 17, 275-304.
Deviney, E., Dickert, J., & Lockwood, R. (1998). The care of
pets within child abusing families. In R. Lockwood & F. Ascione
(Eds.), Cruelty to animals and interpersonal violence: Readings in
research and application (pp. 305-313). West Lafayette, IN: Purdue
University Press.
Faver, C. A., & Strand, E. B. (2003). Domestic violence and
animal cruelty: Untangling the web of abuse. Journal of Social Work
Education, 39, 237-253.
Flynn, C. P. (1999). Exploring the link between corporal punishment
and children's cruelty to animals. Journal of Marriage and the
Family, 61, 971-981.
Flynn, C. P. (2000). Why family professionals can no longer ignore
violence towards animals. Family Relations, 49, 87-95.
Hepworth, D. H., Rooney, R. H., & Larsen, J.A. (2002). Direct
social work practice (6th ed.). Pacific Grove, CA: Brooks/Cole.
Johnson, R.A., & Meadows, R. L. (2002). Older Latinos, pets and
health. Western Journal of Nursing Research, 24, 609-620.
Kaufman, M. E. (1999). The relevance of cultural competence to the
link between violence to animals and people. In F. R. Ascione & P.
Arkow (Eds.), Child abuse, domestic violence, and animal abuse (pp.
260-270). West Lafayette, IN: Purdue University Press.
Keeter, S., Miller, C., Kohut, A., Groves, R.M., & Presser, S.
(2000). Consequences of reducing nonresponse in a national telephone
survey. Public Opinion Quarterly, 64, 125-148.
Klonoff, E. A., & Landrine, H. (1999). Acculturation and
cigarette smoking among African Americans: Replication and implications
for prevention and cessation programs. Journal of Behavioral Medicine,
22, 195-204.
Lago, D., Delaney, M., Miller, M., & Grill, C. (1989).
Companion animals, attitudes towards pets, and health outcomes among the
elderly: A long-term follow-up. Anthozoos, 3, 25-34.
Lavrakas, P. J., Stasny, E. A., & Harpuder, B. (2000, May
18-21). A further investigation of the last-birthday respondent
selection method and within unit coverage error. Paper presented at the
55th Annual American Association for Public Opinion Research Convention,
Portland, OR.
Lorenzo-Hernandez, J., & Ouellette, S. C. (1998). Ethnic
identity, self esteem, and values in Dominicans, Puerto Ricans, and
African Americans. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 28, 2007-2024.
Marx, M. B., Stallones, L., Garrity, T. F., & Johnson, T. P.
(1988). Demographics of pet ownership among U.S. adults 21 to 64 years
of age. Anthrozoos, 2, 33-37.
Melson, G. F. (2001). Why the wild things are. Cambridge, MA:
Harvard University Press.
Nelson, P. (2002). The survey of psychologists' attitudes,
opinions, and clinical experiences with animal abuse (Doctoral
dissertation, The Wright Institute, 2002). Dissertation Abstracts
International, 63, 1040
Netting, F. E., Wilson, C. C., & New, J. C. (1987). The
human-animal bond: Implications for practice. Social Work, 32, 60-64.
Norusis, M. J. (1990). SPSS/PC+ advanced statistics 4. O. Chicago:
SPSS.
Phinney, J. S., Cantu, C. L., & Kurtz, D.A. (1997). Ethnic and
American identity as predictors of self-esteem among African American,
Latino, and White adolescents. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 26,
165-185.
Resnicow, K., Soler, R. E., Braithwaite, R. L., Selassie, M. B.,
& Smith, M. (1997). Development of a racial and ethnic identity
scale for African American adolescents: The survey of black life.
Journal of Black Psychology, 25, 171-188.
Richard, J. (2004, January/February). Minority report: part one:
African Americans. Best Friends, pp. 14-16.
Risley-Curtiss, C. (2004). [The animal--human bond and schools of
social work]. Unpublished raw data.
Roberts, R. E., Phinney, J. S., Masse, L. C., Chen, Y. R., Roberts,
C. R., & Romero, A. (1999).The structure of ethnic identity of young
adolescents from diverse ethnocultural groups. Journal of Early
Adolescence, 19, 301-322.
Sable, P. (1995). Pets, attachment, and well-being across the life
cycle. Social Work, 40, 334-341.
Siegel, J. M. (1995). Pet ownership and the importance of pets
among adolescents. Anthrozoos, 8, 217-223.
Wilson, C.C., & Netting, F. E. (1987). New directions:
Challenges for human-animal bond research and the elderly. Journal of
Applied Gerontology, 6, 189-200.
Zilney, L. A., & Zilney, M. (2005). Reunification of child and
animal welfare agencies: Cross-reporting of abuse in Wellington County,
Ontario. Child Welfare, 84, 47-66.
Christina Risley-Curtiss, PhD, MSSW, is associate professor, School
of Social Work, Arizona State University, Tempe, AZ 85287-1802; e-mail:
risley.curtiss@asu.edu. Lynn C. Holley, PhD, ACSW, is assistant
professor, School of Social Work, Arizona State University. Shapard
Wolf, MEd, is director and technology support analyst, principal, School
of Life Sciences, Arizona State University. This study was part of a
larger study conducted through the Arizona State University Survey
Research Laboratory of which Mr. Wolf was, at the time, the director.
Table 1: Demographic Characteristics of County Population,
Sample Participants, and Pet Owners, 78 Years and Older
County Total
Population Study Sample
(N = 2,244,146) (N = 587)
Characteristic % n % n
n = 2,244,146 n = 585
Female 50.5 1,132,745 60.1 353
Male 49.5 1,111,401 39.7 232
Race or ethnicity n = 2,244,146 n = 587
White, not Hispanic 71.0 1,593,872 78.4 460
Hispanic, any race 20.8 466,312 12.3 72
American Indian,
not Hispanic 1.4 30,317 4.9 29
Other race
and mixed race 6.9 153,645 8.5 50
Age n = 2,244,146 n = 572
18-24 14.0 313,365 9.3 53
25-34 21.8 488,329 19.0 109
35-44 21.2 475,907 21.5 123
45-54 16.3 366,464 20.5 117
55-64 10.7 241,102 13.1 75
65 & over 16.0 358,979 16.6 95
Household income n = 1,133,048 n = 514
Less than $10,000 6.8 77,072 6.2 32
$10,000-$49,999 48.0 544,576 42.2 217
$50,000-$99,999 31.9 361,254 40.3 207
$100,000 or more 13.3 150,146 11.3 58
Educational attainment n = 2,246,838 n = 584
Did not complete
high school 19.5 438,609 3.9 23
High school
diploma or GED 23.5 528,340 18.5 108
Some college/
technical school/
associate's degree 33.8 759,548 40.4 236
Study Sample:
Only Those
with Pets
(N = 368)
Characteristic % n
n = 366
Female 60.6 223
Male 39.1 143
Race or ethnicity n = 368
White, not Hispanic 82.1 302
Hispanic, any race 11.1 41
American Indian,
not Hispanic 6.3 23
Other race
and mixed race 5.4 20
Age n = 362
18-24 8.3 30
25-34 20.7 75
35-44 23.2 84
45-54 24.6 89
55-64 14.1 51
65 & over 9.1 33
Household income n = 329
Less than $10,000 3.0 10
$10,000-$49,999 40.7 1.34
$50,000-$99,999 43.2 142
$100,000 or more 13.1 43
Educational attainment n = 367
Did not complete
high school 3.3 12
High school
diploma or GED 19.1 70
Some college/
technical school/
associate's degree 40.6 149
Note: GED = general equivalency diploma.
The ns for race or ethnicity add up to more
than 587 because participants could identify
themselves as more than one race or ethnicity.
Table 2: Types and Frequencies of Companion
Animals by Ethnicity (n = 368)
Pet Owners Hispanic/ White,
with This Spanish not American
Animal Origin Hispanic Indian
Type of
Animal n % % (n = 41) (n = 302) (n = 25)
Cat 61 16.6 5 55 3
Dog 214 58.2 29 180 14
Bird 46 12.5 11 37 5
Rabbit 3 0.8 0 3 1
Fish 78 21.2 12 65 10
Small animals
(rats, etc.) 25 6.8 2 22 4
Horse 15 4.1 1 13 2
Turtle 14 3.8 4 13 1
African Pacific
American Asian Islander
Type of
Animal (n = 9) (n = 6) (n = 2)
Cat 0 2 0
Dog 9 4 2
Bird 0 2 0
Rabbit 0 0 0
Fish 0 1 0
Small animals
(rats, etc.) 0 0 0
Horse 0 0 0
Turtle 0 0 0
Note: The ns for race or ethnicity add up to more than
369 because participants could identify themselves as
more than one race or ethnicity. Percentages add up to
more than 100 because participants could have more than
one type of pet.
Table 3: Beliefs Regarding Companion Animals
by Ethnicity (n = 587)
Hispanic/ White,
Spanish not African
Origin Hispanic American
Belief Variable (n = 72) (n = 491) (n = 22)
Pets should be spayed or
neutered early in life.
Agree 55 419 14
Disagree 13 46 6
Don't know (a) 4 26 2
It's okay to hit a
pet if it misbehaves.
Agree 14 86 7
Disagree 58 390 14
Don't know (a) 0 15 1
Animals do
not feel pain.
Agree 5 21 1
Disagree 66 469 21
Don't know (a) 1 1 0
Animals can feel fear,
sadness, happiness,
and love.
Agree 70 484 20
Disagree 0 4 0
Don't know (a) 2 3 2
There are too many
unwanted animals
in the world.
Agree 64 447 20
Disagree 6 29 0
Don't know (a) 2 15 2
American Pacific
Indian Asian Islander
Belief Variable (n = 34) (n = 16) (n = 5)
Pets should be spayed or
neutered early in life.
Agree 27 10 4
Disagree 6 1 1
Don't know (a) 1 5 0
It's okay to hit a
pet if it misbehaves.
Agree 4 3 1
Disagree 29 11 4
Don't know (a) 1 2 0
Animals do
not feel pain.
Agree 3 3 1
Disagree 31 13 4
Don't know (a) 0 0 0
Animals can feel fear,
sadness, happiness,
and love.
Agree 33 16 5
Disagree 0 0 0
Don't know (a) 1 0 0
There are too many
unwanted animals
in the world.
Agree 29 13 5
Disagree 4 3 0
Don't know (a) 1 0 0
Note: The ns for ethnicity add up to more than 368
because participants could identify themselves as
more than one ethnicity. "Don't know" was coded
"don't know/refuse to answer."