The Bell Curve: Race, Socioeconomic Status, and Social Work.
Gorey, Kevin M. ; Cryns, Arthur G.
The ongoing debate on the interrelationship among race, socioeconomic
status, and social problems has again been brought to the fore by the
recent publication of books by biological determinists, most notably The
Bell Curve: Intelligence and Class Structure in American Life
(Herrnstein & Murray, 1994) and Race, Evolution, and Behavior
(Rushton, 1997). These works are based on two central premises of what
we consider dubious empirical merit: (1) that one's ultimate social
position is, to a large extent, the consequence of the inherited
characteristic of intelligence; and (2) that the major
"racial" groups differ significantly on this largely
predetermined characteristic. It and other inherited differences among
racial groups are then put forth as explanations for black-white
differences on such disparate personal and social problems as
criminality, parental investment, illegitimacy, welfare dependency, and
AIDS. Moreover, one of the unstated assumptions of this deterministic
paradigm is that race is predominantly a biologic, rather that a social
construction; it does not account fur life experiences in any compelling
way (Muntaner, Nieto, & O'Campo, 1996). Such theorizing has
ominous implications for social welfare policy as well as for social
work practice. Therefore, we believe that the profession's voice
ought to be heard in this debate.
Social Worker's Contribution to the Debate
Recently, critiques of The Bell Curve have been presented in four
edited books (Devlin, Fienberg, Resnick, & Roeder, 1997; Fraser,
1995; Jacoby & Glauberman, 1995; Kincheloe, Steinberg, &
Gresson, 1996) and two monographs (Ceci, 1996; Fischer, Hout, Jankowski,
Swidler, & Voss, 1996). From among the 154 substantive arguments
made, primarily by academics (61 percent) and journalists (37 percent),
only one was authored by a social worker (Vidal, 1996). This finding
echoes an earlier lament by Gorey (1995) that despite expectations that
social workers, committed to a person-in-environment framework, would
lead in building knowledge on the interrelationship of socioeconomic
factors, race--ethnicity, and personal and social problems, very little
of the empirical research has been reported in the profession's
peer-reviewed publications.
Being the one exception, Vidal (1996) criticized The Bell Curve on
two related epistemologic grounds: (1) Its analyses grossly
oversimplified very complex issues; and (2) its review of knowledge was
highly biased; it ignored the rich social science literature on poverty,
AFDC use, and reform evaluations. Specifically, Vidal noted an array of
important variables that were ignored in The Bell Curve's analysis
of intelligence and welfare dependency: gender, the consequences of low
pay in low-skilled jobs, relative opportunity for education and more
rewarding jobs, and relative availability of affordable child care and
health care. Vidal clearly showed that social work's more systemic
and structural theorizing provides the basis for more comprehensive and
practically valid theoretical models to guide research on social
problems.
Practical versus Mere Statistical Significance
The ethical imperative for social work's involvement in this
debate is perhaps most cogently underscored when The Bell Curve's
authors and other similarly oriented analysts report multiple and
consistently "significant" relationships among racial group
status (most notably black and white) and intelligence, and other
clearly social phenomena such as welfare dependency, crime, parental
investment, familial abuse, and others, with black people recording
significantly more deleterious outcomes in comparison with whites.
Typically, their statistically significant findings are reported without
benefit of even the most elementary effect size metrics. Such reports
give no clear view of a purported relationship's practical,
clinical, or policy significance. Yet, Herrnstein and Murray (1994), as
well as Rushton (1997) and others, typically, make rather strong causal
claims and then make concomitant sweeping social policy statements,
which, not surprisingly, unequivocally support an extremely conservative
agenda.
It may be illustrative here to focus on the single largest effect
reported in The Bell Carve, that is, a one standard deviation difference
on mean intelligence between black and white people in the United
States. This finding, if accepted, may be interpreted to mean that 20
percent of the nation's variability in intelligence could be
accounted for by racial group status (d 1.00, converts to [r.sup.2] =
.20; Cohen, 1988). The remaining 80 percent of that variability remains
unaccounted for and is probably associated with other variables.
Moreover, contrary to determinists' interpretations of such
findings, an exclusively genetic explanation is nut necessarily
warranted; a social explanation is as plausible, indeed, probably more
so. We believe that the environmental alternative explanation is far
more compelling for the following reasons: Most of Herrnstein and
Murray's models of intelligence and social problems account for
only 5 percent to 15 percent of the criterion's variability, the
majority for less than 10 percent (Gould, 1995); secondary analysis of
their data reveals further that the race-intelligence association has
diminished by 35 percent to 50 percent over the past two decades: and
after any adjustment (matching, sample restriction, mathematical
modeling for any socioeconomic factors, no race-personal or social
problem associations are observed, In fact, the socioeconomic-adjusted
race intelligence association indicates that the main effect of race
accounts for less than one half of one percent of the explanation
([r.sup.2] = .004); social-environmental main effects and interaction
effects probably account for the remaining more than 99.5 percent (Cryns
& Gorey, 1999; Gorey & Cryns, 1995). The Bell Curve's
extremely simplistic models leave virtually all of social reality
unexplained, and so, we believe strongly, that policymakers ought to
reject them as the basis for making any social welfare or other policy
decisions.
The Need For More Complex Models
Transcendence of the simplistic and very typically atheoretical (Muntaner et al., 1996) black-white dichotomization of "race"
is a needed first step toward making the research in this field
practically useful. A search of research literature indexes between 1975
and 1998 provides evidence suggestive of social work's lead on this
score. We compared Social Work Abstracts with Psychological Abstracts,
Sociological Abstracts, and Medline on their use of more diverse ethnic
or culturally sensitive key word terms (for example, ethnicity; Afro,
African, Asian or Native American; aboriginal; Hispanic; Latino) versus
the mere use of race or a simplistic black-white dichotomy. Twice as
many social work citations (51 percent) used more ethnically-culturally
specific language compared with psychology, sociology, and medicine (26
percent, p < .001). It would seem that social work is more aware that
theories that enable understanding about the interrelationship of
race-ethnicity, socioeconomic status, and social problems will need to
account for the multitude of life experiences of diverse people of
color.
Social work researchers, although we traditionally have called for
systemic assessment of the transaction of people and environments,
acknowledging, at least in theory, the importance of multiple factors in
the development of both personal and social problems, also have tended
to analyze race or ethnicity simplistically in a vacuum of sorts, as if
it did not indeed interact with many other personal and social factors.
For example, a search of Social Work Abstracts on CD-ROM retrieved 8,923
citations on the key words intelligence or personal or social problems
representative of The Bell Curve's analyses, and nearly three times
as many were indexed under the race-ethnicity key words listed above
(678) as were indexed under socioeconomic-relevant key words (250) (for
example, poverty, income, education, social class, socioeconomic
status). None included both race and socioeconomic parameters, and so,
none could have observed any of their possible interactions. Such
simplistic analyses may unwittingly lend support to biological
determinism, as the interpretations of atheoretical racial group
comparisons are left to readers' implicit theory of causality
(Muntaner et al., 1996; Nisbett & Ross, 1980). Biological
determinists play this analytic game well. For example, of The Bell
Curve's numerous analyses of National Longitudinal Survey of Youth
data, its central analytic plan, none presented both racial and
socioeconomic factors so that the practical strength of their main
effects and their interactions could be understood. This makes it much
easier for readers to infer that the reported racial group differences
on crime, illegitimacy, and welfare dependency must result largely, if
not completely, from some innate personal, genetic factor.
Conclusion
The research and practice experience of ourselves and others
convinces us of the following: That race is primarily a social-cultural
construct; that socioeconomic factors are much better predictors of
various social and health statuses than is race; many diverse problems
are more prevalent among poor people, who are disproportionately
represented by children, women, and people of color (Wilson, 1987); and
that interaction effects of these and other diverse factors are nearly
always more important than simple main effects are (Armour, Anttinen,
May, & Paabo, 1996; Devlin, Daniels, & Roeder, 1997; Diamond,
1997; Gorey et al., 1997; Gould, 1981; Muntaner et al., 1996; Stringer
& McKie, 1997; von Haeseler, Sajantila, & Paabo, 1996). To make
the research in this field more useful to the practitioner and
policymaker, the simplistic and scientifically unsupported
differentiation of "race" into "black,"
"white," and "others" groups needs to be abandoned
in favor of a more diverse conceptualization that endeavors to
understand people and communities in systemic transactional terms. We
believe that the practice wisdom and knowledge of our social work
colleagues will confirm these notions and so implore them to bring their
voices to the important ongoing debate on race-ethnicity, socioeconomic
status, and social inequalities.
References
Armour, J. A., Anttinen, T., May, C. A., & Paabo, S. (1996).
Minisatellite diversity supports a recent African origin for modern
humans. Nature and Genetics, 13, 154-160.
Ceci, S. J. (1996). On intelligence: A bioecological treatise on
intellectual development (2nd ed.). Cambridge, MA: Harvard University
Press.
Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral
sciences (2nd ed.). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Cryns, A. G., & Gorey, K. M. (1999). Rushton's impermeable belief in the validity of his own evolutionary paradigm may prevent his
consideration of alternatives: A rejoinder to Rushton and Ankney.
Manuscript submitted for publication.
Devlin, B., Daniels, M., & Roeder, K. (1997). The heritability of
IQ. Nature, 388, 468-71.
Devlin, B., Fienberg, S. E., Resnick, D. P., & Roeder, K. (Eds.)
(1997). Intelligence, genes, and success: Scientists respond to The Bell
Curve. New York: Springer-Verlag.
Diamond, J. (1997). Guns, gems, and steel: The fates of human
societies. New York: W. W. Norton.
Fischer, C. S., Hout, M., Jankowski, M. S., Swidler, A., & Voss,
K. (1996). Inequality by design: Cracking the bell curve myth.
Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.
Fraser, S. (Ed.) (1995). The bell curve wars: Race, intelligence, and
the future of America. New York: Basic Books.
Gorey, K. M. (1995). Environmental health: Race and socioeconomic
factors. In R. L. Edwards (Ed. in-Chief), Encyclopedia of social work
(19th ed., Vol. 1, pp. 868-872). Washington, DC: NASW Press.
Gorey, K. M., & Cryns, A. G. (1995). Lack of racial differences
in behavior: A quantitative replication of Rushton's (1988) review
and an independent meta-analysis. Personality and Individual
Differences, 19, 345-353.
Gorey, K. M., Holowaty, E. J., Fehringer, G., Laukkanen, E., Webster,
D. J., Moskowitz, A., & Richter, N. I,. (1997). An international
comparison of cancer survival: Toronto, Ontario and Detroit, Michigan
metropolitan areas. American Journal of Public Health, 87, 1156-1163.
Gould, S. J. (1981). The mismeasure of man. New York: W. W. Norton.
Gould, S. J. (1995). Mismeasure by any measure. In R. Jacoby & N.
Glauberman (Eds.), The bell curve debate: History, documents, opinions
(pp. 3-13). New York: Times Books.
Herrnstein, R. J., & Murray, C. (1994). The bell curve:
Intelligence and class structure in American life. New York: Free Press.
Jacoby, R., & Glauberman, N. (Eds.). (1995). The bell curve
debate: History, documents, opinions. New York: Times Books.
Kincheloe, J. L., Steinberg, S. R., & Gresson, A. D. III, (Eds.).
(1996). Measured lies: The bell curve examined. New York: St.
Martin's Press.
Muntaner, C., Nieto, F. J., & O'Campo, P. (1996). The bell
curve: On race, social class, and epidemiologic research. American
Journal of Epidemiology, 144, 531-536.
Nisbett, R., & Ross, L. (1980). Human inference: Strategies and
shortcomings of social judgment. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.
Rushton, J. P. (1997). Race, evolution, and behavior: A life history
perspective (rev. ed.). New Brunswick, NJ: Transaction.
Stringer, C., & McKie, R. (1997). African exodus: The origins of
modern humanity. New York: Henry Holt.
Vidal, M. R. (1996). Genetic rationalizations and public policy:
Herrnstein and Murray on intelligence and welfare dependency. In J. L.
Kincheloe, S. R. Steinberg, & A. D. Gresson (Eds.), Measured lies:
The bell curve examined (pp. 219-226). New York: St. Martin's
Press.
von Haeseler, A., Sajantila, A., & Paabo, S. (1996). The genetic
archeology of the human genome. Nature and Genetics, 14, 135-140.
Wilson, W. J. (1987). The truly disadvantaged: The inner city, the
underclass, and public policy. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Kevin M. Gorey, MSW, PhD, is associate professor, Social Work
Program, University of Windsor, 401 Sunset Avenue, Windsor, Ontario, N9B
3P4, Canada; e-mail: gorey@server.uwindsor.ca.
Arthur G. Cryns, PhD, is professor emeritus, School of Social Work,
and adjunct professor, Department of Social and Preventive Medicine,
School of Medicine and Biomedical Sciences, State University of New York at Buffalo.