The evidence game: home win or score draw?
Bullock, Roger
At a recent seminar on child development, one of the world's
leading experts came out with the comment, 'evidence usually wins
in the end'. Further elaborating, he argued that 'even if it
does not provide categorical answers, it sets a framework, indicates
probabilities, highlights important questions and closes lines of
enquiry'. This confident announcement set me thinking: 'Is
this true with regard to children in need?' The speaker's
background was in medicine where the supply of and demand for reliable
information are probably greater than in social care and, in some
circumstances, the number of potentially significant factors is easier
to handle.
Having spent 46 years in research, I have seen the respect given to
evidence fluctuate. When, as at present, resources are diminishing, it
becomes much sought after by agencies and project managers seeking to
prove their services' effectiveness, demonstrate value for money
and fend off cuts. In this competitive climate, all sorts of claims are
made about children and it is hard to know whether they are genuine
wishes for the wider good or appeals to strengthen the negotiating
position of those speaking out.
Two recently reported propositions about looked after children
illustrate this. One says that a high proportion of young men and women
entering prison custody have been in care, and the other wants more
children to be removed from home. From a research perspective, it is
difficult to know what to make of these statements, especially as it is
the bullet points that tend to attract publicity. With regard to prison
custody, the statistic that over one-third of entries are ex-care is
undoubtedly accurate but gives a misleading picture when considered in
isolation. Each year in England some 150 out of 25,000 care leavers (54%
of them boys) go directly to jail and so form part of the 2,000 (92% of
them male) 15-to 17-year-old annual custody admissions. In addition,
follow-up information shows that only about three per cent of looked
after 16-year-olds are in custody three years later. So, how can these
apparent discrepancies be reconciled? The solution is one familiar in
drawing conclusions from sequential processes: when a small receptacle
is fed by a much larger reservoir, inferences about the relationship of
one to the other need handling with caution.
Similarly with regard to the proposal that more children should be
taken into care, it is difficult to comment without knowing the
epidemiological basis and the assessments of types of care and their
ability to meet particular needs. It may be right but so too might be
the critics who claim with equal force that some children in care should
not be there, or that the 'in' or 'out' question is
irrelevant as it is the system itself that needs reforming. In the
absence of sound evidence, the debate feels more like watching ping pong
than listening to discourse.
Social scientists have long asked why certain public concerns lead
to political action while others do not. In child care, researchers
posed this question with regard to child protection and concluded that
policy reflected the threshold drawn between 'abuse' and
'not abuse'. They went on to argue that this was influenced by
four factors--legal/moral questions, users' views, pragmatic
considerations and evidence--and that the balance between these factors
changes. (1) So, for the reason explained, evidence currently is riding
high in the policy equation.
This conclusion raises the key question of what is evidence with
regard to looked after children? Evaluating outcomes is an academic
minefield fraught with searching questions, such as whose outcome, when
it should be assessed and how we decide whether it is good or bad. More
difficult still is proving that effects, however well measured, are the
result of what has been done and not something else. There are also many
who claim that a task as broad as parenting cannot be assessed on simple
criteria as it is as much an art as a science; evidential evaluation is
only really suitable for uncomplicated preventive or therapeutic
interventions. Moreover, others, such as psychotherapists, who study the
unconscious as well as the conscious, will defend their work from any
reductionist thinking and phenomenologists will stress the importance of
the meaning and emotions we attribute to situations --after all, this is
what makes us human even if it messes up cause-and-effect models.
But research is not a monolith and different types of study carry
different sorts of messages with different time dimensions. There are,
for example, general theories of child development that offer a
conceptual framework. These cannot be tested in themselves although
hypotheses derived from them can. They often take a long time to seep
into thinking and the most fashionable of these at the moment in
fostering and adoption is Bowlby's theory of attachment. As this
work is 50 years old, Bowlby must surely be awarded a prize for
'winning in the end'. Then there are the large-scale empirical
studies for which the US and UK are renowned. These link children's
experiences and identify the risk and protective factors associated with
particular outcomes. Finally, there are the small-scale studies of the
sort that abound in this journal, which some critics might say are
scientifically weak but which can add a smidgeon of support and
refinement to existing knowledge.
It is at this point that technical questions begin to bite because
evaluations of different kinds tend to produce different results. There
is a hierarchy of methodologies to assess impact that is based on
quality, ranging from descriptive studies at the bottom, via those using
a control group, through quasi-experimental designs, to
randomised-controlled trials at the top. Unfortunately, the likelihood
of finding effects diminishes as the scale is ascended; so, as most
studies of looked after children are in the lower echelons, the
knowledge that practitioners rely on might be over overconfident.
If evidence-based social work is inescapable, how should it be
handled? The recent report on child protection by Eileen Munro (2) is
helpful since it discusses the value and challenges associated with
evidence-based interventions in children's services. She defines
these as work with regard to prevention, early intervention or treatment
that is proven by a high standard of evidence to improve children's
health or development.
But what qualifies as an evidence-based programme? Criteria
generally accepted in other disciplines are that the intervention is
supported by at least two robust evaluations in which the effects on
those receiving the intervention were compared with a control group. One
of these should be a randomised control-led trial.
Given this criterion, what types of evidence-based programme exist
in the child development field? They fall into at least five categories:
community/ public health strategies; health visiting activities; early
years support; parenting initiatives; and therapeutic interventions.
Details of specific examples can be found in the Allen (3) (pp 145-7)
and Munro Reviews (paras 6.31-39), which offer important points of
reference when deciding how best to allocate scarce resources in the
areas of prevention/early intervention and child protection.
It is, of course, important to use the same high-quality
evaluations to find out what does not work, as some popular services are
likely to be exposed as ineffective or even to have serious unintended
consequences.
But it is here that the other three factors affecting the policy
threshold come into play. Evidence does not produce a prescription for
what must be encouraged or axed but suggests a direction of travel and
encourages further testing and exploration. While every decision has a
downside, the dilemma for professionals is whether the strengths of the
upside are sufficient to go ahead. The dearth of rigorous evaluations in
UK children's services means that it is not known whether many
popular services are effective but that legal/ moral and pragmatic
considerations that something 'needs to be done' demand their
continuation.
The problem with evidence-based programmes
This brief discussion of evidence-based programmes runs the danger
of giving the impression that they have the potential to eradicate the
problems in hand. Sadly, they do not and some qualification is
necessary.
One is that an exciting result can have contrasting implications
for individuals as opposed to larger groups. For example, a two per cent
reduction in the rate of conduct disorders among the looked after
population might seem small but would benefit over a thousand children.
Another is that exceptions to expected outcomes are not necessarily
irrelevant; those cases that do fit the evidence can stimulate important
research studies. A further issue is that many proven interventions
originate from outside the UK. This does not invalidate them but it does
urge retesting to ensure that the ideas travel well.
There are also inevitable difficulties in getting evidence-based
programmes accepted and implemented on a wide scale and ensuring that
once in place, their fidelity is maintained--that is, as they were
intended, with the correct levels of training, coaching and adherence to
the manuals. Failure to do this can reduce their intended benefits and
can even be damaging. At the moment mainstream services in the UK have
limited experience of providing such services as most of them depend on
short-term, marginal funding. Getting systems ready for evidence-based
programmes and evidence-based programmes ready for systems is,
therefore, fundamental to any progress in this area.
Some of these suggestions will undoubtedly be anathema to many
professionals and carers, but the current recession means that the
evidence debate cannot be ignored. It is highly functional in
cash-strapped times as it is ethical in that it identifies the most
effective response, justifies public spending in terms of value for
money and protects agencies from litigation at a period when many
lawyers and pressure groups are also struggling to maintain their
employment.
Finally, one has to ask sceptics and critics: What is the
alternative? That we allow people to do what they like to families and
children or remain free to dictate what others need? Good results might
emerge from such an approach, but they are most likely to be due to
serendipity rather than systematic calculation. Child care history is
littered with good intentions that now appear, at best crackers, or, at
worst, wicked. While some seeking to assess child welfare might regret
replacing the ability to recite the catechism with the Strengths and
Difficulties Questionnaire, the fact is that the latter is more likely
to produce the ethical, least harmful and more effective practice that,
in my view at least, should be among the hallmarks of a modern society.
So to return to the seminar; I am not sure if 'evidence is
winning in the end' but in the present climate it is certainly a
candidate for a good score draw.
(1) Department of Health, Child Protection: Messages from research,
London, HMSO, 1995, p 16
(2) The Munro Review of Child Protection, Final Report, Department
of Education, Cmd 8062, May 2011
(3) Early Intervention: The next step, An independent report to Her
Majesty's Government by Graham Allen MP, HM Government, January
2011
Roger Bullock is Commissioning Editor of Adoption & Fostering
and a Fellow, Centre for Social Policy, Warren House Group at Dartington