Motivational goals and school achievement: Lebanese-background students in south-western Sydney.
McInerney, Dennis M.
There is a general belief that Lebanese-background students in
Australia do not usually perform well at school. This study examines the
school achievement of Lebanese-background students and the major
motivational factors related to their achievement. A total of 271 Year
Nine students (117 Lebanese-background and 154 non-Lebanese background)
in three high schools in southwestern Sydney completed the Inventory of
School Motivation and the Facilitating Conditions Questionnaire and this
information was used to examine the students' motivational profiles
and the salience of motivational and other background variables in
predicting school achievement.
The study indicates that the Lebanese-background group is lower on
variables which predict both their English and Science results, such as
their valuing of education, and their perceived family and teacher
support. The strongest predictors for achievement for the
Lebanese-background group were competition, social concern, recognition,
and token reward; however, these variables were not the strongest
motivators for the students of Lebanese background. The variable most
strongly valued by the Lebanese-background group was power, and that was
found to be negatively related to their school achievement.
The significance of these findings is that the Lebanese-background
group does not strongly value the positive predictors for school
achievement and attention needs to be paid to enhance these among this
group; and that the Lebanese-background group shows higher levels of the
negative predictors and hence attention needs to be given to diminish
the students' negative perspectives which predict lower academic
performance.
Keywords
learning
multicultural
motivation
school achievement
cross-cultural
Non-English Speaking Background (NESB)
Introduction
At an aggregate level, young people from non-English speaking
backgrounds (NESB) have higher post-compulsory education participation
rates than Australian-born and English-speaking background (ESB) migrant groups (Williams, Long, Carpenter & Hayden, 1993). Moreover, it is
found that students from NESB have a more positive view of their
schooling experience (Ainley, 1995; Ainley & Perry, 1994; Sturman,
1997) and that the academic aspirations of students and parents of
students from NESB appear to be higher than for ESB students and parents
(Meade, 1983).
On the other hand, studies have also indicated that the apparent
high rate of participation in higher education is not in itself a true
indication of the success of all minority NESB students in schools and
that the school performance of children frequently did not match their
high aspirations or the high aspirations of parents (Teese, McLean,
& Polese, 1993).While NESB students, on average, have higher Year
Twelve participation rates, a larger proportion of them achieve low to
medium Higher School Certificate scores in comparison to students whose
parents are Australian born, and that English language proficiency appears to be a major determinant of achievement for NESB students.
Studies (Meade, 1983; Marjoribanks, 1980; Taft, Strong & Fensham,
1971) also indicate that there are variations within and between ethnic
groups in educational achievement; some minority groups perform better
than others.
Sturman (1997) argues that one of the major limitations of reviews
of research into the educational experiences of immigrant Australians is
that most research combines immigrant-background students into very
broad categories distinguishing between three blanket groups:
Australian-born students; students born overseas in non-English-speaking
countries; and students born overseas in English-speaking countries. The
problem with this type of categorisation is that there can be variations
and differences within a group that are sometimes greater than the
differences between groups. For instance, within the group of students
born in non-English-speaking countries, Sturman points out that there
can be differences in parents' aspirations and students'
achievement. There are also differences within groups of students who
are born in Australia but whose parents come from non-English speaking
countries. While there are Asian students who have difficulty with
English and do not satisfy their educational aspirations, there are many
Asian students who are academically highly successful.
Therefore, in examining the school achievement of students from
different ethnic and migrant backgrounds, it is important to look at
specific groups of students and examine some of the circumstances associated with their education in order to understand the factors
behind their achievement or lack of it.
Among the groups that have high academic aspirations but low school
achievement are the Lebanese-background students (Horvath, 1979;
Kalantzis, Cope & Slade, 1989; Meade, 1983). These studies seem to
be the most recent and only available data we have on the achievement of
Lebanese-background students in schools, although the apparent lack of
achievement of these students remains a problem. In qualitative research related to this project (Suliman, 2001), the author found that schools
that have a high percentage of Lebanese-background students have
expressed their concern about the lack of motivation and low achievement
of Lebanese-background students. The two main problems identified by
principals and teachers at these schools were an apparent lack of
motivation on the part of Lebanese-background students and an apparent
lack of support by parents for the education of their children. Both of
these factors were believed to have a negative impact on students'
school achievement. There was also a prevailing assumption among school
administrators that parents are disinterested in what goes on in the
school and do not show support for the school or for the education of
their children. The concern of principals was how to engage the
Lebanese-background students in the process of education and motivate
them to achieve better results, and how to gain the support and positive
participation of parents in school meetings and in the general
educational challenges that face schools and parents. Therefore, lack of
motivation on the part of the students and the parents was considered a
major cause for the relatively poor school achievement of
Lebanese-back-ground students.
While it might be accepted that lack of motivation may have a
negative impact on the school achievement of Lebanese-background
students, there is inadequate research data to support this belief.
There is little empirical evidence on the nature and range of the
motivational goals held by Lebanese-background students and the
relationship of these goals to their academic achievement. There is also
little evidence as to whether Lebanese students are individualist and
more Western goal-oriented or collectivist and more traditionalist
oriented; and whether their goal orientation is in harmony with the
goals fostered in schools or not. We know little about other factors
that are believed to impact on school achievement, such as the
students' self esteem, their sense of competence, their sense of
purpose for schooling and their valuing of education. We have no data to
indicate the kind of encouragement and support or lack of it that they
receive from parents, teachers and peers and how they feel about school.
Although some studies in Australia and the United States have examined
the motivational goals of students from different cultural background
(Maehr, 1998; Maehr & Midgley, 1996; Maehr & Pintrich, 1995;
McInerney, 1990; 1992; McInerney & Swisher, 1995; McInerney, Roche,
McInerney & Marsh, 1997), there has been no research in this area
concerning students from a Lebanese background.
Hence, this research sets out to fill the gap in available
knowledge by examining the school achievement of Lebanese-background
students and, in particular, to identify some of the major factors
contributing to their achievement or lack of it. In order to do this, we
addressed the following research questions:
* What is the level of school achievement of Lebanese-background
students in comparison to other students in high schools?
* What are the motivational goals of Lebanese-background students?
* Which goals are predictive of students' school achievement?
In order to better understand the factors behind the achievement
patterns of Lebanese-background students and their goal orientation, it
is important to give some background information about the Lebanese in
Australia in the context of their immigration and social experiences as
well as review the goal theory of achievement motivation.
Background
Lebanese immigrants in Sydney
The history of Lebanese settlement in Australia goes back to the
late nineteenth century, with the latest phase of immigrants arriving
from 1975 onwards as a result of the civil war in Lebanon. The period
1971 to 1977 saw the heaviest influx of Lebanese immigrants to Sydney.
Chain migration was the major pattern, with extended families and
sometimes almost whole villages migrating together. Most of these
immigrants settled in Sydney bringing the number of Lebanese-born
immigrants in Sydney to 50,000 by 1996 and making them the sixth largest
immigrant group in Sydney. More than half of the total number of
Lebanese who arrived during this period was Muslims: sixty per cent,
approximately 14,000 people (Humphrey, 1998).
The Lebanese immigrants came to Australia with the hope of
returning to Lebanon once the war was over, and therefore saw themselves
as temporary residents and were not very serious about finding jobs,
learning English or improving their economic and educational status.
There was a high rate of unemployment and considerable settlement
problems among these people. Many of these arrivals had disrupted education and work experience prior to immigration, due to the civil
war. They also had little economic security and limited literacy ability
in Arabic.
The situation was made worse with the depressed economic situation
in Australia at the time of their arrival: 'The economic situation
in Australia at the time with a decline in the number of jobs in
manufacturing industries, together with discrimination in recruitment by
Australian employers made it harder for these people, with limited
skills and experience, to find work' (Clyne & Kipp, 1996, p.
23). Moreover, the influx of Lebanese migrants in big numbers over a
period of about six years placed enormous financial and social pressures
on the existing Lebanon-born population in terms of the organisation of
housing, jobs and financial support, especially for the Muslim population.
Recently, Lebanese unemployment rates were measured at between
three to five times that of the national average (Collins, Noble,
Poynting & Tabar, 2000). Among those who have jobs, first-generation Lebanese-born immigrants are more likely to be over-represented in
blue-collar jobs and under-represented in professional and managerial
jobs when compared to other immigrants and the Australian-born. Muslim
Lebanese seem to be more disadvantaged than their Christian counterparts. Most Muslim Lebanese in the workforce appear to be
employed as unskilled workers in Australia with their Christian
counterparts more likely to be involved in small business. Sixty per
cent of Australia's Sunni Muslim immigrants rely on welfare and
workers compensation for income as do thirty-two per cent of Maronites (Humphries, 1998, in Collins et al., 2000). Therefore, this group of
Lebanese immigrants are disadvantaged and the Muslim group more so than
the Christian group.
In spite of their disadvantaged status, the Lebanese people in
general have very strong family and social relations and they offer
support to members of their family and community. Family members,
friends and members of the Lebanese community hold strong moral and
social values and adhere to cultural traditions, all of which influence
individual behaviour. Children within this closely knit family and
community unit are expected to adhere to the norms of behaviour of the
family unit and the Lebanese community and they are also expected to
respect their elders, who, in turn, have high aspirations for them
(Bouma, 1994; El-Hassan, 2000; Faour, 1998; McKay, 1989). The Lebanese
also receive great support from the many organisations and groups that
offer services to the community in Arabic.
The Lebanese image
What is the image of the Lebanese in Australia at the present time?
Up until recent times there was no specific focus on the Lebanese
community, neither in a positive nor negative way, but recent
international incidents--mainly the Gulf and Iraqi wars, September 11
and the association of terrorism with Arabs, especially Muslim
Arabs--have contributed to negative stereotyping of all Arabic-speaking
people and led to harassment, intimidation and vilification of the
Arabic-speaking community. This in turn, had its negative impact on the
youth of the community.
A recent study of Lebanese immigrant youth in western Sydney
(Poynting, Noble & Tabar, 1999) has pointed out that Lebanese young
boys tend to 'stick together' and have strong solidarity for
each other and for other migrant groups but in contraposition to Anglo Australians. This 'separateness' from Anglos extends to an
attitude of disrespect. They do not regard 'Aussies' with
respect, because, they say, this group does not treat them and others of
their own ethnicity with respect. These same youth do, however, regard
their own parents and members of their community with respect. They
express respect for their religion, whether Christian or Muslim, and for
their culture and ethnic traditions. For them, being Lebanese entails
respect to the family, to traditions and respect for others. They
perceive this as an allegiance to their ethnicity and ethnic solidarity.
Studies have also pointed out that there is a power conflict
between the Lebanese youth on the one side and the Asians and
Australians on the other; and the latter two groups are seen to be
superior to the Lebanese students both academically and physically. The
Lebanese youth also see themselves and are seen by others to be less
academic than other groups (Collins et al., 2000).
Goal theory of achievement motivation
The goal theory of achievement motivation has made a significant
contribution in explaining the relationship between students'
motivation and their school achievement, while taking into account the
schools' and students' cultures and examining the interaction
of these factors. The goal theory of achievement motivation stresses
that school motivation and achievement for an individual are the product
of a complex set of interacting goals that reflect personal, family and
cultural values and that the goals held by students direct and guide
their cognition and academic behaviour. Personal goals are defined as
cognitive representations of the different purposes that students may
have in different achievement situations, and are presumed to guide
students' behaviour, cognition, and affect as they become involved
in academic work. It is also believed that the goals stressed by schools
may have dramatic consequences as to whether students are motivated and
succeed at school or not (Ames, 1992; Ames & Ames, 1984; Blumenfeld,
1992; Dweck & Elliot, 1983; Elliot & Dweck, 1988; McInerney,
Roche, McInerney & Marsh, 1997; Pintrich, Marx & Boyle, 1993;
Wentzel, 1991).
Mastery and performance goals
Researchers in the past have focused particularly on two goals:
mastery and performance. Mastery goals are intrinsic goals that are self
referenced. Individuals who hold mastery goals focus on the intrinsic
value of learning and work hard to understand their work, improve their
competence and skills and achieve a sense of mastery. Success, for them,
is not measured against other students; rather, the focus is on the
intrinsic value of learning.
Performance goals, on the other hand, are extrinsic and other
referenced. The focus is on obtaining a good grade and being better than
others. For students who hold performance goals, rewards and approval
from others are very important motivational factors and one's
ability, self worth and success are measured in relation to others or to
norm-referenced standards. Success is measured by doing better than
others and self worth is determined by one's ability to perform and
compete successfully. Performance-oriented students appear to focus on
getting a good mark or doing better than other students; and they use
surface level strategies such as rote learning. Because this approach is
associated with social comparison and wanting to be better than others,
students may tend to avoid challenging tasks and withdraw from tasks
after an initial failure, which may be followed by negative self worth.
Research has indicated that performance goal orientation was not
significantly related to cognitive engagement or regulatory strategies
(Ames, 1992).
How do students become either mastery or performance oriented? Most
goal theorists point out that students' goal orientations are
dependent on and influenced by school culture (the goals stressed by
schools) and that classroom context has dramatic consequences as to
whether students work hard and take on challenges, or whether they avoid
challenging tasks and give up when faced with difficulties and failure
(Ames, 1984, 1992; Covington, 1992; Elliot & Dweck, 1988; Maehr,
1989; Maehr & Midgley, 1996; McInerney, 1995). It is also argued
that schools and teachers either implicitly or explicitly reflect these
goals through educational structures, such as the nature of the teaching
tasks, assessment procedures and power structures used in the classroom.
These school goals in turn influence the goals students adopt (Ames,
1992; Blumenfeld, 1992; Meece, 1991).
Mastery and performance goals, however, are not mutually exclusive.
Recent research suggests that individuals may hold both mastery and
performance goals, varying in importance, depending on the nature of the
task, the school environment and the broader social and educational
context of the institution (Meece, 1991; Pintrich & Garcia, 1991;
Wentzel, 1991). Within school structures, beside mastery and performance
goals, there are other relevant and interacting multiple goals that may
impact on students' academic behaviour in different ways. The
social dimension of schooling, including the influence of parents,
teachers and peers, may interact with both mastery and performance
goals, and be extremely influential in affecting children's
attitudes towards schooling in general and learning in particular
(McInerney 1995, 1988, 1989; McInerney & Sinclair, 1992; Pintrich
& Schrauben, 1992).
Students may also hold multiple goals reflecting social values such
as a desire to please parents, to be important in the peer group, or to
preserve a cultural identity; each of which may impact upon their level
of motivation for particular tasks in school settings. These multiple
goals interact providing a complex framework of motivational
determinants of action.
Both mastery goals and performance goals are individualist in
orientation: both focus on individual goals, whereas some cultures are
collectivist oriented (McInerney, Roche, McInerney & Marsh, 1997).
Therefore, reducing a study on motivation to performance versus mastery
goals is unsatisfactory when examining the goals of students from
non-individualist societies.
Individualist and collectivist
Triandis (1994) draws contrasts between individualist-oriented
Western industrialised cultures and collectivist non-Western traditional
cultures and points out that it is commonly believed that Western
industrialised and heterogeneous societies are more individualist
oriented: that is, individuals think of themselves as autonomous
entities and their behaviour reflects personal beliefs; they stress
independence, emphasise pleasure, are competitive, seek power and
control over others and are desirous of individual success through
achieving personal goals. On the other hand, non-Western homogeneous societies are more collectivist oriented: individuals see themselves as
belonging to a collective and their behaviour is dictated by in-group
norms and expectations. They emphasise interdependence and values such
as security, obedience, duty, in-group harmony, hierarchy and
personalised relationships, and their individual goals reflect in-group
goals. It is also believed that Western-type schools foster
individualistic goals and place little emphasis on collectivism, which
focuses on group goals and affiliation. In these Western-type schools
little attention is paid to 'group orientations such as working to
preserve in-group integrity, interdependence of members and harmonious
relationships' (McInerney, 1995, p. 157).
Goals of Arabic-speaking people
Although very little research has previously been conducted into
the goal orientations of Lebanese school students, research on the goal
orientation of Arab school teachers (Abu Saad, 1998) and on Gulf States
and Egyptian businesspeople (Abu-Saad & Hendrix, 1995; Almaney,
1981; Barakat, 1993; El Sayed-El Khouly & Buda, 1997; Hofstede,
1984;) indicates that Arab people are strongly collectivist oriented and
that Arab society tends to de-emphasise the individual as an end in
itself, stressing instead the relationships, the obligations and
responsibilities of the individual towards his or her immediate
community: 'Traditional Arab values put more emphasis on the group
than on the individual, on solidarity than on the activity and needs of
the individual and on the communion of person than on their
autonomy' (Abu-Saad, 1998, p. 379).
In view of the above, the present study sets out to assess the
nature of the goal orientations of Lebanese-background students and
establish whether they are performance or mastery oriented and
individualists or collectivists, and to examine the impact of these
goals on their school achievement.
Methodology
Participants A total of 271 Year Nine students in three high
schools in southwestern Sydney participated in this study: an all-boys
school, an all-girls school, and a co-educational school. We chose the
Year Nine level in order to allow us to collect the participants'
Year Ten School Certificate results the following year.
Of these students, 117 were of Lebanese-background (LB) and 154 of
non-Lebanese-background (NLB). Seventy-two per cent of the students in
the study were born in Australia, but only 11.8 per cent of fathers and
14.4 per cent of mothers were born in Australia. Overall, 85.1 per cent
of students came from a background where a language other than English
is used at home. Approximately 43.5 per cent of participating students
came from homes where the father was a tradesperson, salesperson or
clerk, while 18.0 per cent of mothers were employed in one of these
categories. Approximately 25.2 per cent came from homes where the father
was in a labouring or related occupation or unemployed while 56.1 per
cent classified their mother's occupational role in this way.
Approximately 10.7 per cent of fathers and four per cent of mothers held
a professional, para-professional or managerial job. Only 14.8 per cent
of fathers and 13.2 per cent of mothers completed schooling to Years
Eleven and Twelve. The majority of participants (45.8 per cent)
described their religion as Muslim, 37.7 per cent as Christian, 9.6 per
cent as Buddhist, and 2.6 per cent as Hindu. Of the Lebanese-background
students, (n = 117), 88.0 per cent described their religion as Muslim.
In order to make comparisons between the LB students and others,
one primary and three subgroups were drawn. The primary comparison group
consisted of all Non-Lebanese-background students (NLB) (n = 154).The
three subgroups of the NLB group were: English-background group (EB) (n
= 60) made up of students who did not have another language beside
English at home; Chinese-background group (CB) (n = 12) made up of
students who spoke Chinese at home; and a Vietnamese-background group
(VB) (n = 21) made up of students who spoke Vietnamese at home.
Sixty-one students who were not part of these three smaller comparative
groups also formed part of the NLB group.
Instruments Two instruments were used in this study: the Inventory
of School Motivation (ISM) and the Facilitating Conditions Questionnaire
(FCQ).
The Inventory of School Motivation is an exploratory instrument
designed to measure motivation in cross-cultural settings (McInerney,
Marsh, Yeung, 2003; McInerney & Sinclair, 1992; McInerney, Yeung
& McInerney, 2001).
Thirty-seven items were used to measure seven scales reflecting
students' perceived goals and three scales reflecting
students' sense of self. The perceived goals are the
respondents' personal incentives in schoolwork: task/effort,
competition, power, affiliation, social concern, recognition, and token
reward. The three sense of self scales measure the respondents'
perceptions, beliefs, and feelings: self-esteem, sense of competence,
sense of purpose. The questions were answered using a five-point
Likert-type scale anchored with 1 (strongly agree) and 5 (strongly
disagree).
The Facilitating Conditions Questionnaire (McInerney, 1989) asked
questions about background variables that are believed to facilitate or
inhibit the performance of students at school. Twenty-five items
measured the following seven dimensions: positive affect to school,
perceived peer support, perceived positive family support, perceived
negative family support, perceived teacher support, positive valuing of
education, negative valuing of education. Items were answered using a
Likert-type scale that ranged from 1 (strongly agree) and 5 (strongly
disagree).
Both the Inventory of School Motivation and the Facilitating
Conditions Questionnaire have been used in several cross-cultural
settings (McInerney, 1989, 1990; McInerney & Sinclair, 1992;
McInerney, Roche, McInerney, & Marsh, 1997). The Inventory of School
Motivation and Facilitating Conditions Questionnaires are available
through Professor Dennis McInerney.
The results of the Year Ten School Certificate (a certificate
awarded to school students who satisfactorily complete a program of
studies in Years Seven to Ten) were used as outcome measures for the
school achievement of students in English, Science and Mathematics. The
results in each course are issued as grades awarded by each
student's school, with the school's grading patterns in
English, Mathematics and Science being determined by public reference
tests held in mid-year.
As well as collecting information about the motivational goals of
students, the survey also sought information on the sociocultural and
demographic profiles of participants including place of birth of
students and parents, language spoken at home, parental occupation,
level of education of parents, religion and cultural traditions
practised by students.
Administration of survey
Surveys were administered in each of the three schools by the chief
researcher and with the help of teachers' aides from each school.
Statistical analyses
Analysis of variance and multivariate analysis of variance were
used to test for significant differences between groups. Multiple
regression analyses were used to examine the relationship between
motivational, sense of self, and facilitating conditions variables and
outcome variables.
Hypotheses
From previous studies on the school achievement of
Lebanese-background students (Kalantzis, Cope & Slade, 1989) and the
impressions gathered from school principals and teachers, it is expected
that the school achievement of this group of Lebanese-background
students would be lower than that of other groups.
Research on the goal orientation of Arab school teachers and
businesspeople has indicated that Arab people are strongly
collectivist-oriented. It is therefore expected that LB students would
be stronger on collectivist goals such as affiliation and social
concern. It is also expected that they would be weaker on individualist
goals such as competition and power, and on task/effort, which is
considered important within individualist societies that emphasise
individual rather than group Success.
Public recognition and approval from parents and other members of
the community would be very important to Lebanese students. It is
therefore expected that they would seek recognition and praise from
members of the community, friends and parents for good work. They would
therefore be stronger on recognition and token reward than other NLB
students.
Research examining the youth in south-western Sydney indicated that
the Lebanese youth have formed an oppositional culture to counteract
what they see as discrimination against them by the school and the wider
community and that they feel allegiance to their own group but not to
others. These studies also indicated that the Lebanese youth see
themselves and are seen by others to be less academic than other groups
(Collins et al., 2000). From this we can, therefore, draw the hypotheses
that Lebanese students would be less inclined than other NESB students
to value task/effort, self-esteem, sense of competence, sense of
purpose, positive affect towards school, positive value of education and
teacher support. It was expected that Lebanese-background students would
rate peer support highly.
Studies (Suliman, 2001) indicated that parents have high
aspirations and expectations for their children. It is, therefore,
expected that
parents would be supportive of the education of children and
encourage them to achieve academically in order to reach the
professional and social status aspired for them. Lebanese students
would, therefore, be stronger on positive family support for education
and weaker on negative family support.
The following hypotheses were drawn for the Lebanese-background
students: Perceived goals Hypothesis One: LB students would be weaker on
task/effort than NLB students.
Hypothesis Two: LB students would be stronger on affiliation and
social concern than other NLB students.
Hypothesis Three: LB students would be stronger on competition and
power goals than other NLB students.
Hypothesis Four: LB students would be stronger on recognition and
token reward than other NLB students.
Sense of self goals Hypothesis Five: LB students would have lower
self-esteem, lower sense of competence and lower sense of purpose than
NLB students.
Facilitating conditions Hypothesis Six: LB students would have
stronger perceived family support for their education than other NLB
students. Hypothesis Seven: LB students would have lower positive affect
towards school and lower perceived teacher support than other NLB
students.
Results
School Certificate
The first objective of this study was to examine the level of
school achievement of Lebanese-background students. Results of the Year
Ten School Certificate were used as outcome measures for the school
achievement of students in English, Science and Mathematics.
An analysis of the frequencies for the Year Ten School Certificate
results for the whole group--Lebanese-background (LB) and
non-Lebanese-background (NLB)--indicated that in all subject areas
(English, Science and Mathematics), there is a high concentration of
students in the two lower grades and a low concentration in the two top
grades. These results indicated that a high percentage of the cohort of
students at these three schools achieved in the lower grades in
comparison to other students in the state of New South Wales.
An analysis of the Year Ten School Certificate results for LB
students and other comparative groups (NLB, EB, CB, VB) indicated that
in English, Science and Maths there was a higher percentage of LB
students achieving lower grades, higher than that for the
non-Lebanese-background students. Similarly, in all subject areas, the
percentage of LB students achieving top grades was lower than that for
all the comparative groups.
Examination of the means indicated that in English, Science, and
Advanced, Intermediate and General Mathematics, the mean for the LB
students in each of these subjects was lower than it was for any of the
other four comparative groups, and in many cases significantly lower
(see Table 1).
Results also indicated that the LB students achieved significantly
lower than the NLB group in English, Science and Intermediate Maths;
significantly lower than the EB group in English, Intermediate Maths,
and Science; significantly lower than the CB group in Science and
Intermediate Maths; and significantly lower than the VB group in Science
(see Table 1).
Motivational profile of students
The second objective of this study was to identify the motivational
goals of students. For this purpose, a set of Manova analyses was used
to describe the motivational profiles of the LB students and to examine
similarities and differences between them and the NLB students.
The Inventory of School Motivation measured the perceived goals:
task/ effort, competition, power, affiliation, social concern,
recognition and token reward.
The Facilitating Conditions Questionnaire measured sense of self
variables: self-esteem, sense of competence, and sense of purpose; and
facilitating conditions variables: positive affect, perceived peer
support, perceived positive and negative family support, perceived
teacher support, positive and negative value of education.
Multiple regression analyses were then conducted to identify goals
that are predictive of students' achievement. These analyses
indicated that each of the three sets of scales was able to explain a
significant amount of variance in English and Science scores for both
the LB and NLB groups. The most successful set appears to be the
facilitating conditions, which explained thirty-one per cent of the
variance for English results and thirty-five per cent for Science
results. This was followed by the motivational goals, which explained
thirty-two per cent variance for English and twenty-three per cent for
Science. Last was the sense of self scales, which explained nine per
cent of variance for English and fifteen per cent for Science.
Perceived Goals
Task/Effort It was hypothesised that LB students would be more
collectivist than individualist and therefore would not be strong on
task/effort (Hypothesis One). The research findings run contrary to
expectations and clearly indicate that there is no significant
difference between the LB and NLB groups on task/effort--the LB students
endorse this mastery goal as strongly as the other groups.
Multiple regression analyses indicate that task/effort is a
significant and strong predictor of English and Science results for both
the LB and NLB students. For both the LB and NLB groups, those students
who strive for individual excellence in their schoolwork and are willing
to put in the effort, are more successful in English and Science.
Affiliation and Social Concern It was hypothesised that LB students
would be significantly stronger than NLB students on both affiliation
and social concern (Hypothesis Two).
The multivariate analysis in this study indicate that LB students
are significantly stronger on social concern for others in the school
setting but that, contrary to hypothesis, there was no significant
difference between them and the NLB students on affiliation.
Multiple regression analyses indicated that neither affiliation nor
social concern are significant predictors for the school achievement of
LB and NLB students. This means that their social goals at school appear
not to be related to their school achievement.
Competition and power It was hypothesised that LB students would be
less competitive than NLB students (Hypothesis Three). Evidence from
this study suggests the contrary: that LB students are significantly
more competitive than others in the school setting, but for them
competition is not a significant predictor for any of the school
certificate results, whereas for the NLB students competition is a
strong predictor for Science results.
It was also hypothesised (Hypothesis Three) that LB students would
be less power oriented than other students. The multivariate results of
this study ran contrary to hypothesis and indicated that LB students are
significantly more power oriented than other students in the school
setting. Multiple regression analysis indicated that for the LB
students, however, power is a negative predictor of school achievement.
This means that LB students who are high achievers are not power
oriented and those who are power oriented are not high achievers.
For the NLB students, power is also significantly and negatively
related to their results in English and Science. Therefore, for both the
LB and NLB groups, power is significantly and negatively related to both
English and Science results. The more successful students are less power
oriented.
Recognition and token reward Because approval by parents and the
community are important to young Lebanese, it was hypothesised
(Hypothesis Four) that for the LB students recognition and token
reinforcement from others is very important. In support of Hypothesis
Four, the multivariate analysis of variance indicated that LB students
are significantly more token-oriented and recognition seeking than
others.
Multiple regression analyses indicated that for the LB students,
neither recognition nor token reward were significant predictors of
their school achievement. For the NLB students, recognition and token
were significant positive predictors of English results and token for
Science results.
Sense of self
Self esteem, sense of competence, and sense of purpose It was
hypothesised that LB students would have low sense of competence and low
self esteem which would also infer low sense of purpose (Hypothesis
Five).
Multivariate analysis of variance in this study has indicated that,
contrary to hypotheses, there were no significant differences between
the LB and NLB groups on self esteem and sense of purpose. On sense of
competence, the LB students were significantly stronger than the NLB
students. Although the literature reviewed earlier indicated that the LB
students perceived themselves and were perceived by others as less
intelligent and less academic-oriented than others, this has not
negatively affected their sense of competence. In fact, the LB students
express more competence than other students.
Multiple regression analyses indicated that, among the three sense
of self factors, self-esteem was found to be a significant predictor of
achievement in Science for both the LB and NLB students and in English
for the NLB group. This means that for both groups, those who are
successful in Science have high self-esteem.
For the LB students, sense of competence is a significant and
negative predictor of English and Science results. The results suggest
that high-achieving LB students have a low sense of competence and low
achievers have a high sense of competence. Sense of competence in this
study is defined as an ability to work on one's own and feeling
confident and able to do school work and not being worried about
difficult work. This anomalous result may be explained by assuming that
those students who are successful in English and Science feel they have
achieved this by putting in a great deal of effort and by getting help
from others and that they are not over-confident about their competence
as measured by this scale. It may also mean that they perceive
competence as something that is never actually achieved because there is
more to learn and new things to master all the time.
Facilitating conditions
Positive affect Multivariate analysis of variance indicated that
contrary to hypotheses, the LB students are stronger than others on
positive affect: they like school and look forward to going to classes
more than the NLB students.
Perceived positive and negative family support Contrary to
Hypothesis Six, the LB students do not feel strong support from parents.
Perceived teacher support, perceived peer support, positive and
negative value of education Contrary to Hypothesis Seven, results
indicated that there are no significant differences between the LB and
NLB students in any of the above goals.
Multiple regression analyses indicated that among the seven
facilitating conditions factors, three factors indicated a significant
prediction of the school achievement of LB students: teacher support was
a weak positive predictor for English and Science achievement; positive
family support was a significant positive predictor for Science results;
negative valuing of education was a significant negative predictor for
both English and Science results. This indicates that students who
receive positive encouragement from parents and teachers and who see the
value of education as a means to future success are good achievers. None
of the other variables were significant predictors for the LB
students' achievement.
For the NLB students, negative family support was also a strong
negative predictor for English and Science results. Therefore for both
the LB and NLB groups, positive family encouragement and support are
important predictive variables for their success at school.
Summary
This study clearly indicates that the LB students are falling
behind in their school achievement in all three subjects of the School
Certificate. Examination of the goal orientation of the same group of
students indicated that the LB students were significantly stronger than
the NLB group on perceived goals of competition, power, social concern,
recognition and token reward but there was no significant difference on
task/effort and affiliation.
Results also indicate that among the sense of self factors, there
was no significant difference between the LB and NLB groups on self
esteem and sense of purpose. The only significant difference between the
two groups was on sense of competence, the LB students being stronger
than the NLB group.
Among the facilitating conditions factors, there were significant
differences between them and NLB students on positive affect, positive
and negative family support. The LB students have stronger positive
affect but weaker positive family support than the NLB group. Although
there are no significant differences between the two groups on peer
support, teacher support and positive and negative valuing of education,
the means for the LB students for peer support and positive valuing of
education indicate that they are stronger than the other group on these
variables, but weaker on positive teacher support.
Among the seven perceived goals variables, task/effort was found to
be a significant positive predictor and power a significant negative
predictor of LB students' achievement in English and Science.
Among the three sense of self factors, self esteem was found to be
a significant positive predictor of achievement in Science for the LB
students. Sense of competence was a negative predictor for both English
and Science indicating that those who are high achievers do not have a
strong sense of competence as measured by this scale.
Among the seven facilitating conditions factors, the strongest
predictor of achievement for the LB group was negative valuing of
education followed by negative family support, both found to be negative
predictors for English and Science results. This means that those
students who value education and who receive family support are more
successful. Teacher support was also a significant predictor for
achievement in both English and Science achievement.
Discussion
This study has enabled us to assess the school achievement of a
group of LB students in south-western Sydney and to describe their
motivational characteristics in terms of achievement goal theory and to
explain the differences between them and other students.
Some of the findings of this study run counter to widely held views
about Lebanese-background students in Australian schools. Contrary to
the stereotypical view that Lebanese-background students are
non-academic, this study indicates that there were no significant
differences between them and the non-Lebanese-background group on
task/effort. Many Lebanese-background students, like others, try hard at
schoolwork because they are interested in their work; they need to know
they are getting somewhere with their schoolwork; try hard to understand
new things at school; and try harder when they see improvement.
Contrary to hypotheses, there were no significant differences
between the two groups on affiliation. Both groups appear to like
working with other people at school and work best and enjoy work more
when working with and helping others. These results may be explained by
the fact that some of the non-Lebanese-background students come from
backgrounds that could be construed as collectivist, for example, the
Chinese and Vietnamese students. It is, for example, argued that Chinese
students are more collectivist than Western students, and that they are
highly achievement motivated and that this is directed towards the
benefit of the group, rather than the individual. It is also pointed out
that other people such as family members set the standard of achievement
for the individual and they place greater importance on family and group
goals than on individual goals and 'they are very concerned about
loss or gain of their collective face in their pursuit of
achievement' (Salili, 1995, p.76). It is also found that although
Chinese culture is very much influenced by the collectivists traditions
of Confucius, Taoism and Buddhism, among the Chinese there are both
collectivism and individualism, especially among the young people.
Contrary to hypotheses, this study has also indicated that the
Lebanese-back-ground students are stronger on positive affect to school,
and have stronger self esteem and stronger sense of competence than the
non-Lebanese-background group. Although literature reviewed indicated
that the Lebanese-background students perceived themselves and were
perceived by others as less capable and non-academic, this has not
affected their self esteem and sense of competence. In fact, the
Lebanese-background students have stronger self esteem--they feel they
can do things as well as most people at school; feel they are bright
enough to complete their schooling and feel they succeed at what they do
at school. The Lebanese-background students also feel more competent
than other students--they feel that most of the time they can do school
work by themselves and they are confident, try new things and like to
think for themselves at school. Surprisingly, and contrary to
expectations, the Lebanese-background students have stronger positive
affect to school than the other group; that is, they find school
subjects interesting and like school.
Contrary to hypotheses but in agreement with generally-held
beliefs, Lebanese-background students' feeling of support from
parents is not as strong as it is for other students. This means that
many Lebanese-background students do not feel that they receive strong
encouragement from their parents for their studies. They feel more
positive about teachers' support than about parents' support.
In agreement with hypotheses, there are significant differences
between the two groups on competition and power. To the
Lebanese-background students, winning is important, competition makes
them work harder and they are happy only when they are one of the best.
Lebanese-background students are also found to be more power
oriented--being a leader of a group, being put in charge of things,
feeling important, and being noticed by others are significantly
stronger variables than for other students. The Lebanese-background
students are also stronger on social concern. They value helping and
caring for each other at school and enjoy helping others to do well.
Both recognition and token rewards are also significantly more important
to Lebanese-background students than for others. Praise and
encouragement from friends and parents as well as rewards from teachers
and parents such as presents and merit certificates are very important.
The results of this study indicate that Lebanese-background
students are stronger on a range of collectivist and individualist
variables. Socially and within their own families and community the
Lebanese are strongly collectivist and give a lot of support and help to
each other. Within the school setting, this study indicates that the
Lebanese-background students have retained this collectivist
characteristic in being more socially concerned than others, but that
they are also stronger on individualist goals such as competition and
power.
These results support other research studies which indicate that
there is no simple polar contrast between the individualist and
collectivist groups and that significant differences may arise mostly as
a matter of degree rather than direction of values held: 'It is not
possible to say the individualistic groups are competitive,
individualistic and power seeking, while the collectivist groups are
noncompetitive, non-individualistic and non-power-seeking'
(McInerney, Roche, McInerney & Marsh 1995, p.15). The
Lebanese-background students were found to hold both collectivist and
individualist goals: they are significantly stronger on social concern,
and at the same time they are more competitive and more power-oriented
than the non-Lebanese-background group.
It is possible that the findings indicating that the
Lebanese-background students are stronger on a large number of scales
may reflect response bias. It is evident from the academic outcome
measures that the Lebanese students did more poorly in English,
Mathematics and Science, so it is somewhat paradoxical that their
self-esteem and motivational value beliefs were so high. There is
evidence from other research (Hacker & Bol, 2004) which indicates
that lower-achieving students are more inaccurate and usually
over-confident when judging their performance. Perhaps the
Lebanese-background students, who according to achievement results are
the lowest achieving students in this study, were also more inaccurate
in estimating and judging their level of ability and motivation. It is
possible that low-achieving students may demonstrate a self-serving bias repeatedly overestimating their performance to protect their self-worth
or their image of themselves as relatively good students in comparison
with others. This is an empirical question worth investigation.
As mentioned earlier, research indicates that students' goals
direct their behaviour and attitudes towards learning and achievement
and that the goal orientation of schools affects students' goals
and the way they view education and achievement. In looking at ways to
enhance the educational outcomes of the Lebanese-background students, it
is important that schools aim at developing and enhancing those goals
which are found to be strong predictors of the school achievement of
Lebanese-background students. Schools need to consider ways by which
parents and teachers can show more support to students. Students also
need to see the value of education as a means to a better and more
successful career path and life style. Moreover, attention should be
paid to other variables which were strongly held by the
Lebanese-background students but which were not predictors of their
school achievement, such as competition and power. Therefore, tasks
which involve competition and which give Lebanese-background students
the opportunity to be in charge and to be leaders, together with public
recognition and reward can prove to be successful techniques. In trying
to improve the educational outcomes of Lebanese-background students in
schools it is important to identify their goal orientation to understand
the type of things that motivate them to learn. At the same time, it is
equally important that schools take into consideration these goals when
planning and implementing teaching strategies and learning tasks for
students.
References
Abu-Saad, I., (1998). Individualism and Islamic work beliefs.
Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 29(2), 377-383.
Abu-Saad, I., & Hendrix, V. (1995). Organisational climate and
teachers' job satisfaction in a multi-cultural milieu: The case of
the Bedouin Arab schools in Israel. International Journal of Educational
Development, 15, 141-153.
Ainley, J. (1995). Students' views of their schools. Unicom,
21(3), 5-16.
Ainley, J. & Perry, L. (1994). Students and their primary
schools. In State of Queensland, Shaping the Future: Vol.2. (Report of
the Review of the Queensland School Curriculum). Brisbane: State of
Queensland.
Almaney, A.J. (1981). Cultural traits of Arabs: Growing interest
for international management. Management International Review, 21,
10-18.
Ames, C. (1984). Competitive, cooperative and individualistic goal
structures: A motivational analysis. In R. Ames & C. Ames (Eds.)
Research on Motivation in Education (Vol. 1, pp. 177-207). San Diego,
CA: Academic Press.
Ames, C. (1992). Classrooms: Goals, structures, and student
motivation. Journal of Educational Psychology, 84(3), 26-271.
Ames, R.E. & Ames, C. (Eds.). (1984). Research on Motivation in
Education: Vol. 1. Orlando: Academic Press.
Barakat, H. (1993). The Arab World: Society, Culture and State.
Berkeley: University of California Press.
Blumenfeld, P.C. (1992). Classroom learning and motivation:
Clarifying and expanding goal theory. Journal of Educational Psychology,
84, 272-281.
Bouma, G. (1994). Mosques and Islam settlement in Australia.
Canberra: Australian Government Publishing Service.
Buda, R. & Elsayed-Elkhouly, S. (1998). Cultural differences
between Arabs and Americans: Individualism-collectivism revisited.
Journal of Cross-cultural Psychology, 29(3), 487-492.
Clyne, M & Kipp, S. (1996). Language maintenance and language
shift in Australia, 1991. Australian Review of Applied Linguistics.
Collins, J., Noble, G., Poynting, S., & Tabar, T. (2000).
Kebabs, Kids, Cops & Crime: Youth, Ethnicity & Crime. Sydney:
Pluto Press.
Covington, M.V. (1992). Making the Grade. A Self-Worth Perspective
on Motivation and School Reform. New York: Cambridge University Press.
Dweck, C.S. & Elliot, E.S. (1983). Achievement motivation. In
P.H. Mussen (series Ed.) & E.M. Hetherington (Vol. Ed.), Handbook of
Child Psychology: Vol.4, Socialization,
Personality, and Social Development (4th ed., pp. 643-691). New
York: Wiley.
El-Hassan, K. (2000). Structure and correlates of self-concept in
Lebanon. Paper presented at the International Symposium on Multiple
Dimensions of Academic Self-Concept, Forms of Reference, Transitions,
and International Perspectives. New Orleans: AERA.
Elliott, E. & Dweck, C. (1988). Goals: An approach to
motivation and achievement. Journal of personality and social
psychology, 54, 5-12.
Elsayed-Elkhouly, S. & and Buda, R. (1997). A cross-cultural
comparison of value systems of Egyptians, American, Africans and Arab
executives. International Journal of Commerce and Management, 7,
102-119.
Faour, M. (1998). The Silent Revolution in Lebanon: Changing Values
of the Youth. Beirut: American University of Beirut.
Hacker, D. J., & Bol, L. (2004). Metacognitive theory:
Considering the social-cognitive influences. In D. M. McInerney & S.
Van Etten (Eds.), Research on Sociocultural Influences on Motivation and
Learning: Vol. 4: Big Theories Revisited. Greenwich, CT: Information Age
Publishing.
Hofstede, G. (1984). The cultural relativity of the quality of life
concept. Academy of Management Review, 9, 389-398.
Horvath, B.M. (1979). The Education of Migrant Children: A Language
Planning Perspective. HRDC Report No.24, Feb. 1979. Canberra: Australian
Government Publishing Service.
Humphrey, M. (1998). Identity and mediated violence: Ethnic
conflict, the media and globalisation. In D. Bretherton (Ed.) No Longer
Black & White: Media Responsibility in Ethnic and Racial Conflict.
Melbourne: International Conflict Resolution Centre, University of
Melbourne. pp. 170-80.
Humphries, D. (1998, November 4). Lebanese youths and police to
meet. Australian, p. 5.
Kalantzis, M., Cope, P. & Slade, D. (1989). Minority Languages
and Dominant Culture. London: Falmer Press.
Kalantzis, M., Slade, D. & Cope, P. (1986). The Language
Question--The Maintenance of Languages Other Than English. Sydney: The
Social Literacy Monograph.
Maehr, M.L. (1989). Thoughts about motivation. In C. Ames, & R.
Ames (Eds.), Research on Motivation in Education Vol.3 Goals and
Cognitions. Orlando: Academic Press.
Maehr, M.L. (1998, April). Confronting culture with culture:
Creating optimum learning environments for students of diverse
sociocultural backgrounds. A paper presented at the annual meeting of
the American Educational Research Association, San Diego, CA.
Maehr, M.L. & P.R. Pintrich (Eds.). (1995). Advances in
motivation and achievement: A research annual. Greenwich, CT: JAI Press.
Maehr, M.L. & Midgley, C. (1996). Transforming School Culture.
Colarado: Westview Press.
Marjoribanks, K. (1980). Ethnic Families and Children's
Achievements. Sydney: Allen and Unwin.
McInerney D.M. & Sinclair, K.E. (1992). Dimensions of school
motivation: A cross-cultural validation study. Journal of Cross-Cultural
Psychology, 23, 389-406.
McInerney, D.M. & Swisher, K.G. (1995). Exploring Navajo
motivation in school settings. Journal of American Indian Education, 34,
28-50.
McInerney, D.M. (1988). The psychological determinants of
motivation of urban and rural non-traditional Aboriginal students in
school settings: A cross-cultural study. Dissertation submitted to the
University of Sydney, NSW, Australia.
McInerney, D.M. (1989). Cross-cultural analysis of student
motivation. In D.M. Keats, D. Munro, & L. Mann (Eds.), Heterogeneity in Cross-cultural Psychology (pp. 332-345). Lisse, the Netherlands:
Swets & Zeitlinger.
McInerney, D.M. (1990). The Determinants of motivation for urban
aboriginal students: A cross-cultural analysis. Journal of Cross
Cultural Psychology, 21, 274-495.
McInerney, D.M. (1992). Cross-cultural insights into school
motivation and decision making. Journal of Intercultural Studies, Vol.
13, No.2.
McInerney, D.M. (1995). Goal theory and indigenous minority school
motivation: Relevance and application. In P. R. Pintrich & M. L.
Maehr (Eds.),). Advances in motivation and achievement: A research
annual (Vol. 9., pp. 153-181). Greenwich, CT: JAI Press
McInerney, D.M., Marsh, H. W., & Yeung, A. S. (2003). Toward a
hierarchical model of school motivation. Journal of Applied Measurement,
4(4), 335-357.
McInerney, D.M., Roche, L.A., McInerney, V., & Marsh, H.W.
(1997). Cultural perspectives on school motivation: The relevance and
application of goal theory. American Educational Research Journal, 34,
207-236.
McInerney, D. M., Yeung, S. Y., & McInerney, V. (2001).
Cross-cultural validation of the Inventory of School Motivation (ISM).
Journal of Applied Measurement, 2, 134-152.
McKay, J. (1989). Phoenicial Farewell. Three generations of
Lebanese Christians in Australia.
Melbourne: Ashwood House.
Meade, P. (1983). The Educational Experience of Sydney High School
Students, Report No.3. Canberra: Australian Government Publishing
Service.
Meece, J.L. (1991). The classroom context and students'
motivational goals. In Maehr, M.L., & Pintrich, P.R. (Eds Advances
in motivation and achievement: A research annual (Vol.7, pp. 261-285).
Greenwich, CT: JAI Press.
Pintrich, P.R., & Garcia, T. (1991). Student goal orientation
and self-regulation in the college classroom. In M. L. Maehr & P.R.
Pintrich (Eds.). Advance.; in motivation and achievement: A research
annual (Vol.7, pp. 371-402). Greenwich, CT: JAI Press.
Pintrich, P. R., Marx, R. & Boyle R. (1993). Beyond cold
conceptual change: The role of motivational beliefs and classroom
contextual factors in the process of conceptual change. Review of
Educational Research, 63, 2. 167-199.
Pintrich, P.R. & Schrauben, B. (1992). Students'
motivational beliefs and their cognitive engagement in classroom
academic tasks. In D. Schunk & J. Meece (Eds.). Student Perceptions
in the Classroom: Causes and Consequences (pp. 149-183). Hillsdale, NJ:
Erlbaum.
Poynting, S., Noble, G., & Tabar, P. (1999)
'Intersections' of masculinity and ethnicity: a study of male
Lebanese immigrant youth in western Sydney. Race, Ethnicity and
Education, 2(1).
Salili, F. (1995). Explaining Chinese students' motivation and
achievement: A socio-cultural analysis. In Maehr M.L. & Pintrich,
P.R. (eds.) Advances in motivation and achievement: A research annual
(Vol. 9, pp. 73-118). Greenwich, CT: JAI Press.
Sturman, A. (1997). Social Justice in Education. Australian
Education Review No. 40. Melbourne: Australian Council for Educational
Research.
Suliman, R. (2001). The motivational and linguistic context of the
school achievement of Lebanese-background students in south-western
Sydney. PhD dissertation submitted to the University of Western Sydney,
NSW, Australia.
Taft, R., Strong, P. and Fensham, P.J. (1971). National background
and choice of tertiary education in Victoria. Melbourne: La Trobe
University.
Teese, R., McLean, G., & Polese, J. (1993). Equity Outcomes.
Canberra: Commonwealth Government Printer.
Triandis, H.C. (1994). Culture and Social Behavior. New York:
McGraw-Hill.
Wentzel, K.R. (1991). Social and academic goals at school:
Motivation and achievement in context. In M.L. Maehr & P.R. Pintrich
(Eds.) Advances in motivation and achievement: A research annual (Vol.
7, pp. 185-212). Greenwich, CT: JAI Press.
Williams, T., Long, M., Carpenter, P., & Hayden, M. (1993).
Entering Higher Education in the 1980s. Canberra: Australian Government
Publishing Service.
Rosemary Suliman
Dennis M McInerney
University of Western Sydney
Rosemary Suliman is Senior Lecturer in Languages in the College of
Arts at the University of Western Sydney. Her specialisations are
interpreting and translation, and multicultural education.
Dennis M McInerney is Professor of Educational Psychology and
Research Professor in the SELF Research Centre at the University of
Western Sydney. He is the author of several textbooks including
Educational Psychology: Constructing Learning (Pearson) and
Developmental Psychology for Teachers: An Applied Approach (Allen &
Unwin).
Correspondence for this article should be directed to Professor
Dennis M McInerney, SELF Research Centre, University of Western Sydney,
Locked Bag 1797, Penrith South DC, Penrith, 1797, Australia. Email:
d.mcinerney@uws.edu.au
Table 1 Means, standard deviations and significance
levels for School Certificate Results for the
Lebanese (LB), English (EB), Chinese (CB),
Vietnamese (VB) and NLB background groups
Means, Standard Deviation
and Significance Levels
LB ES CB VB NLB
English
Mean 2.17 2.72 2.50 2.52 2.59
S.D. 1.02 1.01 1.17 .98 1.02
Significance .001 * .302 .150 .002 *
Science
Mean 1.92 2.66b 2.66b 2.76 2.62
S.D. .82 .93 1.07 .94 .96
Significance .000 * .005 * .000 * .000 *
Adv. Maths
Mean 2.31 2.50 2.33 2.83 2.61
S.D. 1.16 1.00 1.53 1.53 1.27
Significance .653 .981 .293 .402
Int. Maths
Mean 2.03 2.69 2.86 2.71 2.65
S.D. .88 1.05 1.46 .95 1.16
Significance .009 * .048 * .906 .007 *
Gen. Maths
Mean 2.49 2.77 3.00 2.75 2.75
S.D. 1.12 1.11 .00 .95 .95
Significance .330 .330 .654 .251
Note: Adv. Maths = Advanced Maths, Int. Maths = Intermediate
Maths, Gen. Maths = General Maths.
Table 2 Multivariate and Univariate Results for the LB and
NLB Groups on Perceived Goals, Sense of Self and Facilitating
Conditions variables
Perceived Goals
Multivariate Analysis Univariate
Lambd F df P
a F P Means SD
.93 2.90 7,266 .006
Task/Eff
LB 2.29 .131 1.76 .64
NLB 1.87 .57
Comp
LB 14.30 .000 2.33 .93
NLB 2.77 .98
Power
LB 9.67 .002 3.02 1.05
NLB 3.36 .82
Afi
LB 0.01 .912 2.33 .75
NLB 2.34 .83
Soci
LB 3.97 .047 2.21 .73
NLB 2.38 .68
Rec
LB 4.47 .035 2.23 .73
NLB 2.42 .82
Token
LB 5.47 .020 2.53 .97
NLB 2.79 .89
Sense of Self
Lambd F D f P
a F P Means SD
0.92 7.39 3,267 0.000
LB
NLB
Soc
LB 9.08 .003 2.07 .59
NLB 2.29 .61
Sop
LB 1.19 .278 1.70 .63
NLB 1.62 .56
Est
LB 0.24 .624 2.20 .69
NLB 2.23 .59
0.88 4.74 7,261 .000
Posaff
LB 9.01 .003 2.49 0.94
NLB 2.83 0.89
Peersup
LB 0.33 .566 2.21 0.89
NLB 2.15 0.81
P/Fam
LB 7.21 .008 1.70 0.86
NLB 1.47 0.67
N/Fam
LB 14.86 .000 4.04 1.19
NLB 4.53 0.85
.85
Teach/s
LB 1.33 .250 2.61 0.86
NLB 2.38 0.84
Pos/Val
LB 3.23 .073 3.35 1.09
NLB 3.57 0.91
Neg/Val
LB 3.28 .071 1.78 0.77
NLB 1.61 0.76
Note: The questions were answered using a five point Likert-type scale
anchored with 1 (strongly agree) and 5 (strongly disagree).
Lower mean = stronger; higher mean = weaker
Key: Task/Eff = Task/Effort; Corn = Competition; Afi = Affiliation;
Soci = Social Concern; Rec = Recognition; Soc = Sense of Competence;
Sop = Sense of Purpose; Est = Self Esteem Posaff = Positive Affect;
Peersup = Peer support; P/Fam = Positive Family Support;
N/Fam = Negative Family Support; Teach/S = Teacher support;
PosNal = Positive value of Education;
Neg/Val = Negative value of Education.
Table 3 Sets of standardised beta weights and multiple
regression coefficients for perceived goals for
Lebanese-background (LB) and non-Lebanese-background
(NLB) groups for English and Science results
Scales English Results
LB
Perceived R = 0.568
Goals [R.sup.2] = 0.322
Beta T P
Task/Effort .345 3.12 .002
Competition -.205 -1.61 .112
Social Power -.281 -2.67 .009
Affiliation .010 .110 .914
Social Concern .068 0.59 .552
Recognition -.116 -0.92 .357
Token -.173 -1.25 .216
Sense of Self R = 0.300
[R.sup.2] = 0.09
Beta T P
Sense of
Competence -.282 -2.09 .038
Sense of
Purpose .229 1.62 .109
Self Esteem .221 1.56 .122
Facilitating R= 0.555
Conditions [R.sup.2] = 0.308
Beta T P
Positive Affect -.155 -1.55 .125
Peer Support .014 1.44 .154
Positive Family
Support .155 1.49 .141
Negative
Family Support -.211 -1.92 .058
Teacher
Support .211 2.08 .040
Positive Value
of Education -.059 -0.54 .592
Negative Value
of Education -.248 -2.39 .019
Scales English Results
NLB
Perceived R = 0.425
Goals [R.sup.2] = 0.180
Beta T P
Task/Effort .198 1.95 .054
Competition .224 1.79 .075
Social Power -.286 -2.53 .013
Affiliation -.096 -1.03 .303
Social Concern .047 0.43 .665
Recognition .238 1.97 .051
Token -.359 -2.87 .005
Sense of Self R = 0.324
[R.sup.2] = 0.105
Beta T P
Sense of
Competence -.090 -0.75 .455
Sense of
Purpose .062 0.59 .558
Self Esteem .343 2.77 .006
Facilitating R= 0.399
Conditions [R.sup.2] = 0.159
Beta T P
Positive Affect .002 .02 .986
Peer Support -.059 -56 .573
Positive Family
Support .034 .33 .738
Negative
Family Support -.342 -3.55 .001
Teacher
Support .138 1.38 .171
Positive Value
of Education .015 .15 .878
Negative Value
of Education -.053 -.58 .565
Scales Science Results
LB
Perceived R = 0.747
Goals [R.sup.2] = 0.225
Beta T P
Task/Effort .425 3.53 .000
Competition -.186 -1.34 .184
Social Power -.264 -2.29 .024
Affiliation -.024 -0.23 .821
Social Concern -.087 -0.69 .493
Recognition .074 0.54 .592
Token -.119 -0.79 .431
Sense of Self R = 0.386
[R.sup.2] = 0.149
Beta T P
Sense of
Competence -.344 -2.62 .010
Sense of
Purpose .197 1.41 .163
Self Esteem .369 2.65 .009
Facilitating R= 0.591
Conditions [R.sup.2] = 0.349
Beta T P
Positive Affect -.059 -0.59 .551
Peer Support .060 0.61 .542
Positive Family
Support .285 2.75 .007
Negative
Family Support -.206 -1.93 .056
Teacher
Support .230 2.28 .025
Positive Value
of Education -.261 -2.4 .018
Negative Value
of Education -.287 -2.84 .005
Scales Science Results
NLB
Perceived R = 0.375
Goals [R.sup.2] = 0.140
Beta T P
Task/Effort .248 2.38 .019
Competition .268 2.10 .038
Social Power -.244 -2.10 .038
Affiliation -.028 -.29 .771
Social Concern -.005 -.05 .963
Recognition .029 .24 .811
Token -.353 -2.75 .007
Sense of Self R = 0.346
[R.sup.2] = 0.119
Beta T P
Sense of
Competence -.111 -0.93 .355
Sense of
Purpose .009 0.08 .932
Self Esteem .409 3.33 .001
Facilitating R= 0.399
Conditions [R.sup.2] = 0.159
Beta T P
Positive Affect .102 0.99 .326
Peer Support -.184 -1.81 .073
Positive Family
Support .036 0.36 .720
Negative
Family Support -.353 -3.77 .000
Teacher
Support .067 0.69 .490
Positive Value
of Education .109 1.18 .240
Negative Value
of Education -.022 -0.24 .809