Negative effects of school-average achievement on academic self-concept: a comparison of the big-fish-little-pond effect across Australian states and territories.
Marsh, Herbert W.
Attending academically selective schools is intended to have
positive effects, but a growing body of theoretical and empirical
research demonstrates that the effects are negative for academic
self-concept. The big-fish-little-pond effect (BFLPE), based on social
comparison theory, posits that equally able students will have lower
academic self-concepts in academically selective schools than in
nonselective schools. Here we test the validity of these predictions for
representative samples of 15-year-olds from eight Australian states and
territories by using multilevel modelling. Consistent with the BFLPE,
the effects of individual student achievement were positive but the
effects of school-average achievement were negative. Although there were
small differences between states/territories in academic achievement,
there were no significant differences between states/territories in the
negative effects of school-average ability.
Keywords
ability grouping
academic achievement
school effectiveness
selective admission
self concept
social theories
**********
The importance of high self-concept as a desirable outcome variable
is evident in diverse settings, including education, child development,
mental and physical health, social services, organisations, industry,
and sport (Branden, 1994). For example, educational policy statements
throughout the world list self-concept enhancement as a central goal of
education and an important vehicle for dealing with social inequities
experienced by disadvantaged groups. This study evaluates the
generalisability of theoretical predictions about the effects of
school-average achievement based on the big-fish-little-pond effect
(BFLPE), by using large representative samples of 15-year-olds from
eight Australian states/territories.
Frame of reference effects in the formation of academic
self-concept: The BFLPE
Self-concept cannot be adequately understood if the role of frames
of reference is ignored. The same objective characteristics and
accomplishments can lead to disparate self-concepts, depending on the
frame of reference or standards of comparison that individuals use to
evaluate themselves. In an educational context, Marsh (1984; Marsh &
Parker, 1984) proposed a frame of reference model called the
big-fish-little-pond effect (BFLPE) to encapsulate frame of reference
effects posited in social comparison theory. In the BFLPE, academic
self-concept is influenced substantially by the ability levels of other
students in the immediate context, in addition to one's own ability
and academic accomplishments.
The historical and theoretical background for this research program
(see Marsh, 1974, 1984, 1991, 1993; Marsh & Parker, 1984) derives
from research in psychophysical judgement (e.g. Helson, 1964; Marsh,
1974; Parducci, 1995), social judgement (e.g. Morse & Gergen, 1970;
Sherif & Sherif, 1969; Upshaw, 1969), sociology (Alwin & Otto,
1977; Hyman, 1942; Meyer, 1970), social comparison theory (e.g.
Festinger, 1954; Suls, 1977), and the theory of relative deprivation (Davis, 1966; Stouffer, Suchman, DeVinney, Star, & Williams, 1949).
Marsh (1984) first proposed the theoretical model underlying the BFLPE,
where he hypothesised that students compare their own academic ability
with the academic abilities of their peers and use this social
comparison impression as one basis for forming their own academic
self-concept. A negative BFLPE (a contrast effect) occurs where equally
able students have lower academic self-concepts when they compare
themselves with more able students, and higher academic self-concepts
when they compare themselves with less able students. For example, if
average ability students attend a school where the average ability level
of other students is high (hereafter referred to as a 'high-ability
school') so that their academic abilities are below the average of
other students in the school, it is predicted that this educational
context will foster social comparison processes that will lead to
academic self-concepts that are lower than if the same students attended
an average-ability school. Conversely, if these students attend a low
ability school, then their abilities will be above average in relation
to other students in the school and social comparison processes will
result in higher academic self-concepts. Hence academic self-concepts
depend not only on a student's academic accomplishments but also
the accomplishments of those in the school that the student attends.
According to this model, academic self-concept will be affected
positively by individual achievement (higher achieving children will
have higher academic self-concepts). However, academic self-concept
should be affected negatively by school-average achievement (equally
able students will have lower academic self-concepts in a school where
the average ability is high and higher academic self-concepts in a
school where the average ability is low).The BFLPE is typically
operationalised (see Marsh & Craven, 2002) as a path model (see
Figure 1) in which the effects of individual student achievement on
academic self-concept are predicted to be positive whereas the effects
of school-average achievement are predicted to be negative. Empirical
support for this negative effect of school-average achievement on
academic self-concept (the BFLPE) comes from numerous studies based on a
variety of experimental/analytical approaches (see recent review by
Marsh & Craven, 2002).
[FIGURE 1 OMITTED]
Diener and Fujita (1997, p.350) reviewed BFLPE research in relation
to the broader social comparison literature. In relation to the broad
spectrum of social comparison research, they emphasised that
Marsh's BFLPE provided the clearest support for predictions based
on social comparison theory in an imposed social comparison paradigm.
The reason for this, they surmised, was that the frame of reference,
based on classmates within the same school, is more clearly defined in
BFLPE research than in most other research settings. Clearly the
importance of the school setting is that the relevance of the social
comparisons in school settings is much more ecologically valid than
manipulations in typical social psychology experiments involving
introductory psychology students in contrived settings. Indeed, except
for opting out altogether, it is difficult for students to avoid the
relevance of achievement as a reference point within a school setting or
the social comparisons provided by the academic accomplishments of their
classmates. A major focus of BFLPE research has been on the
substantively important and surprising implications of this research,
which undermines the assumed advantages of attending academically
selective schools at one end of the ability spectrum and mainstreaming
academically disadvantaged students at the other end (see also Robinson,
Zigler, & Gallagher, 2000).
Two theoretically important issues are the extent to which the
BFLPEs are specific to academic self-concept and the extent to which the
BFLPE varies across different individual student ability levels.
Consistent with theoretical predictions and a growing emphasis on the
multidimensionality of self-concept, the BFLPE is very specific to
academic components of self-concept. Marsh and Parker (1984; Marsh,
1987) showed that there were large negative BFLPEs for academic
self-concept, but little or no BFLPEs on general self-concept or
self-esteem. Marsh, Chessor, Craven, and Roche (1995; see also Craven,
Marsh, & Print, 2000) reported two studies of the effects of
participation in gifted-and-talented programs on different components of
self-concept over time and in relation to a matched comparison group.
There was clear evidence for negative BFLPEs, in that academic
self-concept of students in the gifted and talented programs declined
over time and in relation to the comparison group. These BFLPEs were
consistently large for academic components of self-concept, but were
small and largely nonsignificant for four nonacademic self-concepts and
for general self-esteem.
Whereas the BFLPE predicts declines for academic self-concept for
students in academically selective settings, there is some theoretical
and empirical disagreement about whether these effects vary according to
the initial achievement levels of individual students within these
settings. Coleman and Fults (1985), for example, predicted and found
that students in the top half of academically selective classes
experienced little or no decline in self-concepts. In contrast, Marsh
(1984, 1987, 1991, 1993; Marsh et al., 1995; Marsh & Rowe, 1996)
argued that attending selective schools should lead to reduced academic
self-concepts for students of all achievement levels based on several
different theoretical perspectives. For a large, nationally
representative (US) database, Marsh and Rowe (1996) found that the BFLPE
was clearly evident for students of all achievement levels and that the
size of the BFLPE varied only slightly with individual student
achievement. In two studies demonstrating BFLPEs in students attending
gifted-and-talented programs, Marsh et al. (1995) found no significant
interaction between the size of the BFLPE and the achievement level of
individual students. However, Marsh, Koller, and Baumert (2001) found
small interaction effects in their large German study based on three
waves of data collected on three occasions in the first year after
reunification of the East and West German school systems. Whereas the
size of the BFLPE--the negative effect of class-average achievement--did
not vary with individual student achievement at any of the three times
considered separately or for changes between Time 1 and Time 2
self-concept, the size of this negative effect diminished slightly for
the most able students over the three occasions.
An Australian perspective
Australia continues to experience a substantial growth in the
numbers of both gifted-and-talented (GAT) classes and secondary
selective schools. This growth reflects strong parental interest in, and
political support for, special educational settings for academically
able students. Several early studies were undertaken by the New South
Wales (NSW) Government to evaluate special educational provisions for
GAT students. Sampson (1969) matched students in regular classes (who
had declined offers to participate in specialised GAT classes) with
students who participated in GAT classes in Years 5 and 6 (the last two
years of primary school), on the basis of information collected in Year
4. He found that the two groups did not differ significantly in
subsequent Commonwealth Secondary Scholarship Examination scores, Higher
School Certificate scores, or school persistence. However, regular-class
students performed significantly better than GAT class students on the
aggregate School Certificate in Year 10, although this difference
occurred primarily with boys. In subsequent research, Sampson (1977)
compared a random sample of 240 students from eight selective high
schools and a comparison group of comprehensive high school students
matched on the basis of gender, age, socioeconomic status, IQ, and prior
achievement. He found that there were no statistically significant
differences between selective and comprehensive high school students on
subsequent School Certificate scores, Higher School Certificate scores,
or school retention rates. This result was consistent across initial
ability levels and for boys and girls. This research contributed to a
ministerial report recommending that selective schools should be phased
out, but this recommendation was not enacted due in part to parental
pressure to maintain these schools. In both studies, Sampson emphasised
that he was unable to consider affective variables (e.g. self-concept)
that he suggested might be enhanced by attending selective schools.
Much of the BFLPE research, not surprisingly, is based on
Australian settings (e.g. Craven, Marsh, & Print, 2000; Marsh, 1984;
Marsh & Parker, 1984; Marsh et al., 1995). In a critique of this
research, Gross (1997) argued against the validity of the BFLPE in
Australian high school settings, which prompted a rejoinder by Marsh and
Craven (1998). Gross (1992; see also Gross, 1993) argued:
It might be anticipated that exceptionally gifted children who have
been radically accelerated would score high on the index of academic
self-esteem. By contrast, they display positive but modest scores,
between the mean for their age groups and .7 of a standard deviation
above ... Interestingly, it is the children who have not been
radically accelerated whose academic self-esteem is unusually
inflated. (p. 97)
However, Marsh and Craven countered that, although Gross argued
that students in non-accelerated settings have 'inflated'
academic self-concepts, her results support the BFLPE. Despite being
four standard deviations above the mean IQ, radically accelerated
children have 'radically deflated' academic self-concepts that
are only slightly above average because they compare their academic
skills with those of their older, more able classmates. In contrast, the
non-accelerated gifted students have realistically high academic
self-concepts because they compare their abilities with those students
from a normal range of abilities. Thus radical acceleration is likely to
produce substantial declines in academic self-concept that are
consistent with the BFLPE. Marsh and Craven suggested that the
implication of Gross's argument was that it is somehow bad for
gifted children to have academic self-concepts commensurate with their
high levels of academic achievement and good for them to experience a
substantial decline in academic self-concept, but that Gross provided no
evidence in support of this implication.
Gross (1997) subsequently evaluated shifts in academic and
nonacademic self-concepts for students in their first year of selective
high schools and comprehensive high schools. She implied that her
results did not support the big-fish-little-pond effect (BFLPE).
However, Marsh and Craven (1998) noted that a careful evaluation of her
published data and statistical analyses shows that her results were
clearly consistent with BFLPE predictions. In particular, Marsh and
Craven emphasised that, consistent with BFLPE predictions, Gross's
published results showed that: (a) students from academically selective
schools experienced significant declines in academic self-concept over
time; (b) these shifts were more negative for academic than for
nonacademic self-concepts; and (c) the declines in selective schools
were more negative than in comprehensive schools, particularly for
academic selfconcept (which increased slightly in comprehensive
schools). Hence Marsh and Craven (1998) concluded that a critical
reanalysis of data and findings published by Gross provided clear
support for the BFLPE.
This controversy about selective schools was reignited by the
Vinson Report (Inquiry into the Provision of Public Education, 2000),
the most far-reaching public inquiry into education in NSW in the last
50 years. Based in part on BFLPE research summarised in this article,
the Vinson Report recommended a moratorium on new selective high schools
and selectively cutting back on existing ones--recommendations that were
not endorsed by the NSW Government. In an editorial comment published in
The Sydney Morning Herald responding to this report and the ongoing
controversy about selective schools, Marsh (2002) noted:
It has been more than a quarter of century since the NSW Government
conducted research into this critical educational issue. Why has
there not been a systematic evaluation of the effects of selective
schools on a whole range of outcomes, including achievement,
academic self-concept, educational aspirations, university
attendance, etc.? Where is the solid research upon which to base
decisions about maintaining, increasing, or decreasing the numbers
of students attending selective schools? Given the controversy
surrounding the Vinson report, there is a clear challenge to the NSW
Government to initiate such a research program. Or maybe we will
just wait until the next time this issue erupts and again ask why
educational decisions are based on political expediency instead of
good research. (p. 11)
An international perspective
There exists a growing body of international support for the BFLPE
(see Marsh & Craven, 2002) that substantiates the generalisability
of the theoretical predications and empirical results. Jerusalem (1984)
examined the self-concepts of West German students who moved from
nonselective, heterogeneous primary schools to secondary schools where
selection was on the basis of academic achievement. At the transition
point, students selected to enter the high ability schools had
substantially higher academic self-concepts than did those entering the
low ability schools. However, by the end of the first year in the new
schools, no differences in academic self-concepts for the two groups
were present. Path analyses indicated that the direct influence of
school type on academic self-concept was negative. The most able
students in the low ability schools were less able, but had much higher
academic self-concepts, than the least able children in the high ability
schools.
In 1991, former East and West German students experienced a
remarkable social experiment--the reunification of very different school
systems after the fall of the Berlin Wall. Self-concepts were collected
at the start, middle, and end of the first school year after
reunification (Marsh, Koller & Baumart, 2001). East German students
had not previously been grouped according to ability. For them, the
BFLPE was initially small, then moderate, and then substantial by the
end of the year. West German students had attended schools based on
ability grouping for the two years prior to the reunification. For them,
the BFLPE was substantial at all three times. A large East-West
difference in the size of the BFLPE at the start of the year disappeared
completely by the end of the year. The evolvement of the BFLPE in the
East and West German settings supported the social comparison process
posited to underlie the BFLPE and its cross-cultural generalisability.
In Hong Kong, schools are more highly segregated in relation to
ability than anywhere else in the world. However, collectivist cultural
values prevail that are posited to counter social comparison processes
(compared with more individualistic values in most western countries).
Marsh, Kong, and Hau (2000) followed a large, nationally representative
sample of Grade 7 students through high school (7997 students, 44 high
schools, 4 years) based on a Chinese translation of the Self Description
Questionnaire (SDQII). Psychometric properties of responses to the
instrument were similar to--or even stronger than--those based on the
Australian research that was the basis of the SDQII instrument.
Consistent with the BFLPE, school-average ability (based on measures
collected in Grade 6, prior to the start of high school) had negative
effects on academic self-concept during high school.
Zeidner and Schleyer (1999) tested the BFLPE in a large-scale study
based on a nationally representative sample (N = 1020) of Israeli gifted
students participating in either special homogenous classes for the
gifted or mixed ability classes. Path analyses indicated that gifted
students in mixed ability classes evidenced markedly higher academic
self-concepts, lower anxiety and higher school grades than gifted
students in specialised classes.
The focus of BFLPE research has been on the negative effects of
ability segregation on academically gifted students, but the theoretical
basis of the BFLPE also has important policy implications for the
placement of learning disadvantaged students as well (see also Robinson,
Zigler, & Gallagher, 2000). Tracey, Marsh, and Craven (2003)
extended this research to focus on BFLPE predictions for special classes
for academically disadvantaged students (see also Chapman, 1988; Marsh
& Johnston, 1993). They studied 211 special education students in
Grades 2-6 who had an intellectual disability (IQ of 56 to 75). These
students were either in regular classroom settings or in full-time
support units. Consistent with BFLPE predictions, students in special
classes had significantly higher self-concepts for all three academic
scales (Reading, Math, School) but did not differ significantly from
other special education students in regular classes for Parents,
Physical Ability, and Physical Appearance self-concepts. Somewhat
unexpectedly, special education students in special classes had
significantly higher Peer and General self-concepts.
Broader policy implications of the BFLPE
The results of the BFLPE are very important for understanding the
formation of academic self-concept and testing frame-of-reference
models. However, classroom teachers, policy makers and parents might ask
'So what?'. What are the consequences of attending
high-ability schools on other academic outcomes and how are these
related to academic self-concept? Educators and particularly parents
often assume that there are academic benefits associated with attending
higher-ability schools. After all, academic achievement, aspirations and
subsequent attainment are typically higher in these schools. This naive analysis, however, fails to take into account the initially higher
abilities and other pre-existing differences of students who attend
academically selective high schools. A better test would be to compare
academic outcomes after controlling for the pre-existing differences.
Marsh (1991) considered the influence of school-average ability on
a much wider array of outcomes in a very large-scale longitudinal study--the High School and Beyond Study, in which 36 students each from
1000 high schools were surveyed in Year 10 (T1),Year 12 (T2), and again
two years after graduation from high school (T3).The outcomes in this
study included many of the most important outcomes of education. After
controlling background and initial ability, the effects of
school-average ability were negative for almost all of the Year 10,Year
12, and postsecondary outcomes: 15 of the 17 effects were significantly
negative and 2 were non-significant. School-average ability most
negatively affected academic self-concept (the BFLPE) and educational
aspirations, but school-average ability also negatively affected general
self-concept, selection of advanced coursework, school grades,
standardised test scores, occupational aspirations and college
attendance. Even after controlling for all Year 10 outcomes,
school-average ability negatively affected many subsequent outcomes.
This implies that school-average ability continues to affect negatively
Year 12 and post-secondary outcomes beyond the negative effects
experienced at Year 10. Consistent with the proposal that these negative
effects were substantially mediated by academic self-concept, controling
for the negative effects of school-average ability on academic
self-concept substantially reduced the size of negative effects on other
outcomes.
The present investigation
According to the BFLPE (see Figure 1), the predicted effect of a
student's own individual achievement is positive (+ in Figure 1),
whereas the effect of school-average achievement is negative (- in
Figure 1). in the present investigation we test these predictions in a
nationally representative sample of 15-year-old Australians and evaluate
the generalisability of the effects across eight Australian states and
territories. Based on theory and previous research, the following a
priori hypotheses were developed:
1 (a) The effect of individual student achievement on academic
self-concept is predicted to be positive. (b) Whereas there are likely
to be some differences between states/territories, the direction of the
effect is predicted to be consistently positive and substantial.
2 (a) The effect of school-average achievement on academic
self-concept (the BFLPE) is predicted to be negative. (b) Whereas there
are likely to be some differences between states/territories, the
direction of the effect is predicted to be consistently negative.
3 The size of the BFLPE (the negative effect of school-average
achievement) is predicted to be reasonably consistent across different
levels of individual student achievement (i.e. the interaction between
individual student achievement and school-average achievement is
predicted to be small or non-significant). In particular,
higher-achieving students are predicted to suffer BFLPEs as well as
lower-achieving students.
Although not a major focus of the study, it is also an interesting
research question to evaluate the size of differences in academic
self-concept and achievement as a function of the state/territory and
the school. In pursuing these hypotheses and research questions,
multilevel modelling was used--a state-of-the-art statistical
methodology specifically designed to evaluate multilevel data. This
provided a partition of variance associated with different effects into
components associated with the individual student (level 1) and the
school (level 2). In particular, it provided appropriate tests of the
extent to which the effects of individual student achievement and
school-average achievement differ according to state/territory.
Methods
Data source and sample
The study was based on the Program of Student Assessment (PISA)
database, compiled by the Organisation for Economic Cooperation and
Development (OECD), that consisted of nationally representative
responses by 15-year-olds collected in Australia and other countries in
2000 (see OECD, 2001a, 2001b, for a description of the database and
variables). The PISA database was collected in response to the need for
internationally comparable evidence of student performance and related
competencies within a common framework that is internationally agreed
upon. Selection of the measures was made on the basis of advice from
substantive and statistical expert panels and results from extensive
pilot studies. Substantial efforts and resources were devoted to
achieving cultural and linguistic breadth in the assessment materials,
stringent quality-assurance mechanisms were applied in the translation
of materials into different languages, and data were collected under
independently supervised test conditions. Paper-and-pencil assessments
consisted of a combination of multiple-choice items and written
responses. Whereas all students completed some reading assessment items
(which were the focus of the 2000 data collection), only random samples
of students completed mathematics or science assessments. In addition,
countries were given the option of collecting materials on a Cross
Curriculum Competencies questionnaire which included the academic
self-concept items that are the focus of the present investigation; 26
of 32 countries in the PISA project--including Australia--administered
this additional survey.
The present investigation is based on students who completed the
academic self-concept items on the Cross Curriculum Competencies
questionnaire and standardised academic achievement tests that were
developed specifically for the PISA. The self-concept items were from
the highly regarded Self Description Questionnaire II (Byrne, 1996; see
also Marsh, 1991, 1993; Marsh, Plucker, & Stocking, 2001). However,
because of limitations on the length of the total instrument, only three
items were used to represent academic self-concept. Nevertheless the
reliability for this scale was consistently reasonable across all 26
countries (mean coefficient alpha = .76; SD = .04) and did not differ
significantly from the reliability of responses in Australia (.74).
Because of the nature of the PISA project, nine versions of the
achievement tests were administered, which contained different
combinations of verbal, mathematics, and science test items. Because of
the focus of the PISA study, all students completed at least some verbal
test items whereas only about half of the students completed mathematics
and science tests. Using test-equating procedures based on item response
theory, scores based on each version of the various achievement tests
were put onto a comparable scale. For present purposes, a total
achievement test score was obtained by taking an average score for all
the achievement test scores available for each student. As recommended
in the database documentation (OECD, 2001a, 2001b), analyses were
conducted using sample weights to obtain unbiased estimates of
population parameters. For purposes of the present investigation, the
effective sample size for each Australian state/territory was set equal
to the number of cases for that state/territory prior to weighting so
that the weighted sample size was the same as the unweighted sample size
(i.e. the average weight across all cases was 1.0).Analyses were based
on responses from 4916 students from 223 schools (from a total sample of
5106 students from 231 schools) who had no missing data on variables
considered here (4921 students completed the self-concept items) and
attended high schools in which there were at least 10 students who had
complete data. The number of students in each of the 223 schools varied
from 11 to 40 (M = 22, SD = 3.83).
Statistical analysis
In the present investigation, consistent with the design of the
PISA data, we consider a two-level multilevel model in which students
(level 1) are nested within schools (level 2). In general, it is
inappropriate to pool responses of individual students without regard to
schools or countries unless it can be shown that schools and countries
do not differ significantly from each other. If, for example, there were
systematic differences between schools, then the typical single-level
analyses that ignored this clustering of students into schools would
probably be invalid (violating statistical assumptions in a way that
increases the likelihood of finding a significant effect when there is
none). Furthermore characteristics associated with individual students
are likely to be confounded with those based on schools. From a
practical perspective, a systematic multilevel approach allows
researchers to pursue new questions about how effects vary from school
to school, state/territory differences in the pattern of results, and
the characteristics of schools that are associated with this variation.
This is particularly important in studies such as the present
investigation in which critical variables are associated with both the
individual student level (academic self-concept and achievement) and the
school level (school-average achievement). In the present investigation,
for example, the use of a multilevel approach provides appropriate tests
to determine the extent to which the observed pattern of relations
between academic self-concept and achievement generalises across schools
and across the different Australian states and territories. Hence the
multilevel approach provides a much richer and more appropriate approach
to testing support for the BFLPE than would be possible with traditional
single-level approaches that ignore the fact that students are clustered
within schools (for further discussion, see Goldstein, 1995; Goldstein
et al., 1998; Raudenbush & Bryk, 2002). Of particular relevance in
the present investigation is the effect of school-average ability.
According to the BFLPE, the effect of school average ability on
individual student academic self-concept should be negative. The present
investigation is unique in being able to evaluate the extent to which
this effect varies across eight Australian states and territories.
Based on a large, nationally representative sample of United States high school students, Marsh and Rowe (1996) found a nonlinear relation
between academic self-concept and achievement. Whereas the relation was
strictly monotonic, increments in ability had less effect on academic
self-concept near the lower end of the ability range than near the top.
They speculated, however, that this reflected idiosyncratic scaling
issues in the normally distributed standardised test scores and
negatively skewed self-concept ratings. In support of this suggestion,
they found no nonlinearity in relations between school grades (which
were also negatively skewed) and self-concept responses. Based on these
results, we also included a nonlinear (quadratic) component of
individual student achievement as a predictor of academic self-concept
in the present investigation.
In testing the effect of states/territories, the set of eight
states/territories was treated as a categorical predictor variable represented by a set of eight dummy variables (for further discussion,
see Goldstein et al., 1998; see also Aiken & West, 1991). The dummy
variables were scored 1 (from a particular state/territory) or 0 (not
from the particular state/territory).Thus each student had a value of 0
for seven of the dummy variables and a value of 1 for one of the eight
dummy variables. For purposes of statistical analyses one of the
states/territories was 'left out' (i.e. the eight levels of
the categorical variable were represented by seven degrees of freedom).
New South Wales was selected as the left-out category because it had the
largest sample size of the eight states/territories. Whereas the
selection of the state/territory to be left out had no effect on the
overall significance of the effect of states/territories (i.e. the set
of dummy variables), effects associated with individual dummy variables
must be interpreted in relation to the left-out state. Thus, for
example, a statistically significant effect for a given state means that
the effect in that state was significantly different from the effect in
New South Wales. In different models, the main effects of
states/territories and the interaction of this variable with individual
student achievement and school-average student achievement was
considered. Finally, in supplemental analyses, analyses were conducted
separately for each of the eight states and territories
For purposes of the present investigation, individual student
self-concept was the main outcome (dependent) variable, whereas
predictor variables in different models included individual student
achievement (linear and quadratic terms), school-average achievement,
the interaction between school-average and individual achievement (to
test whether the BFLPE varies with individual student ability), and the
eight Australian states/territories and their interactions with other
predictor variables. We began by standardising (z-scoring) all variables
to have M = 0, SD = 1 across the entire sample (see Marsh & Rowe,
1996; see also Aiken & West, 1991; Raudenbush & Bryk, 2002).
Product terms were used to test interaction and quadratic effects. In
constructing these product variables, we used the product of individual
(z-score) standardised variables (and the product terms were not
re-standardised). Similarly, school-average achievement for each of the
223 schools was computed as the average score of the (standardised)
achievement test scores for students attending those schools without
restandarising these scores (i.e. individual and school-average
achievement scores are in the same metric).
Results and discussion
I begin with an overview of the main results of the study as an
advanced organiser, and then provide more detailed summaries of the
statistical analyses. Across the eight Australian states/territories,
there were no significant differences in academic self-concept, but
small differences in academic achievement (Table 1). Achievement scores
were significantly higher than the Australian average in the Australian
Capital Territory and New South Wales and significantly lower in
Victoria and Northern Territory, but no differences were greater than .3
SD. Results in Figure 2 indicated that there were substantial
differences between individual schools in terms of school-average levels
of achievement, but not for school-average levels of academic
self-concept. Across all Australian states and territories, there was
good support for the BFLPE (Table 1). In particular, the effect of
individual achievement on academic self-concept was positive in each of
the eight states/territories and the total sample (path coefficient =
.40), whereas the effect of school-average achievement (the BFLPE) was
negative across all states/territories and the total sample (path
coefficient = -.35).
[FIGURE 2 OMITTED]
In order to evaluate the BFLPE, a nested series of a priori models
was posited in which new predictor variables were added to each
subsequent model (Table 2). Results summarised in Table 2 indicate the
statistical significance of new predictor variables added to each model,
and the effects of the predictor variables are summarised in Tables 3
and 4.
Effects of individual student achievement
In Model 1 (Table 2), individual student achievement scores were
used to predict individual student academic self-concept. The effects of
both linear and quadratic components of achievement were significantly
positive. In support of Hypothesis la, the effect of academic
achievement on academic self-concept was positive (linear component),
but the quadratic component (see Figure 2) indicated that the effect of
achievement on academic self-concept was stronger for higher achieving
students and weaker for lower achieving students. Whereas there was
substantial consistency in the effect of achievement across the 223
schools, the significant variance component associated with school
intercepts (Table 3) indicated that there were school-to-school
differences in academic self-concept after controlling for academic
achievement (also see Figure 2).
Effects of school-average achievement
In Model 2 (Table 2), school-average achievement was added to the
predictor variables. In support of Hypothesis 2a and the BFLPE, the
effect of school-average achievement was negative and highly significant
(Table 2). The residual variance component associated with schools
decreased substantially (from .025 to .010) due to the introduction of
school-average achievement, which indicates that many of the differences
associated with schools in Model 2 were explained by the negative effect
of school-average achievement in Model 3 (See Figure 3).
[FIGURE 3 OMITTED]
In Model 3 (Table 2), the interaction between school-average
achievement and individual student achievement (linear and quadratic
terms) was added to the predictor variables. Of particular importance,
in support of Hypothesis 3, the individual achievement (linear) x
school-average achievement interaction term was not statistically
significant. Hence the size of the BFLPE did not vary linearly with
levels of individual student achievement. There was, however, a
significant individual achievement (quadratic) x school-average
achievement effect. The nature of this interaction indicated that the
negative effects of school-average ability were slightly smaller for
students of intermediate ability levels. Given the large sample size,
these results indicated that the negative effect of school-average
ability was consistent across the range of student achievement levels.
In particular, there was no evidence that the BFLPE had systematically
larger effects on lower-achieving students than on higher-achieving
students.
Effects of eight Australian states and territories
In Models 4-7 (Table 2), I explored the generality of the effects
across the eight Australian states/territories. In each of these models,
the set of eight states/ territories was represented by a set of seven
dummy variables in which one of the set of predictor variables was
'left out' (i.e. the eight levels of the categorical variable
states/territories are represented by seven degrees of freedom). New
South Wales was selected as the left-out category because it had the
largest sample size of the eight states/territories. Whereas the
selection of the state/territory to be left out had no effect on the
overall significance of the effect of states/territories (i.e. the set
of dummy variables, Table 2), effects associated with individual dummy
variables must be interpreted in relation to the left-out state (i.e. a
statistically significant effect for a given state/territory means that
the effect in that state was significantly different from the effect in
New South Wales). Finally, in supplemental analyses, Model 3 was applied
separately to each of the eight states and territories (and key effects
from these separate analyses were summarised in Table 1). In Model 4,
the main effect of the states/territories was not statistically
significant, which indicates that there were no differences between
states/territories in terms of academic self-concept.
In Model 5, the interaction between individual achievement (linear
component) and states/territories (i.e. the set of seven dummy
variables) was added to the predictor variables considered in Model 4.
The overall effect of this interaction was statistically significant
(Table 2), and the effects associated with three individual
states/territories were statistically significant (Table 4). Relative to
New South Wales (the left-out category), the positive effects of
individual student achievement on academic self-concept were smaller
(less positive) in the Australian Capital Territory, Western Australia,
and Tasmania. Importantly, however, the results based on separate
analyses for each of the eight states/territories (Table 1) indicated
that the effects of individual students' achievement on academic
self-concept were highly significant and positive in each of the
states/territories. These results provide partial support for Hypothesis
1b.
In Model 6, the interaction between school-average achievement and
states/territories (i.e. the set of seven dummy variables) was added to
the predictor variables considered in Model 5. The overall test of
significance for this interaction effect was not statistically
significant (Table 2). Hence, in support of Hypothesis 2b, the size of
the negative effect of school-average achievement (the BFLPE) did not
differ significantly in the eight Australian states/territories.
Summary, conclusions and implications
International interest in the BFLPE and its relevance to
educational settings throughout the world provides exciting new
opportunities to evaluate the generalisability of theoretical
predictions and empirical findings. The availability of the Australian
PISA data provided a unique opportunity to evaluate rigorously the
generalisability of the BFLPEs across eight Austrahan
states/territories. Of central importance to the present investigation,
the effects of school-average achievement were significantly negative
and the sizes of these negative effects did not vary significantly
across the eight states/territories. Given the size and
representativeness of the PISA data and the rigorous statistical
analyses, the results apparently provided strongest support for the
generalisability of the BFLPE in an Australian setting.
School-average achievement effects represent complicated
combinations of the social comparison processes emphasised here, the
quality of the education (e.g. resources, curriculum, expertise of the
teachers) and perhaps family background factors. Furthermore these
effects are likely to be confounded (see Gamoran, Nystrand, Berends,
& LePore, 1995). In BFLPE studies based on true longitudinal data in
which the first wave was collected before the start of high school (e.g.
Marsh, 1994; Marsh et al., 2000), it is possible to unconfound
characteristics of the student cohort from subsequent school effects.
These results show that it is the initial ability levels of students
attending a school that drive the BFLPE rather than subsequent school
effects (although Marsh and Craven, 2002, argue that school policies in
academically selective schools can exacerbate the BFLPE). However,
because features other than the achievement grouping per se are likely
to have a positive effect on subsequent outcomes, potential biases are
likely to be conservative in relation to the negative BFLPE. Therefore,
because of the direction of this potential bias, interpretations of the
negative effects of school-average achievement on academic self-concept
are likely to underestimate the true BFLPE.
Another feature of the present investigation is that the
achievement tests were specifically designed to reflect generic skills
rather than the curriculum in any particular school or country.
Furthermore students had no opportunity to study for any of these
examinations, they were given no feedback on the examination, and they
knew that it would have no consequences for them. Marsh (1987, 1990,
1993; Marsh & Yeung, 1998) argued that relations between academic
self-concept and academic achievement were likely to be stronger when
achievement was based on: classroom performance measures such as school
grades, which are more easily influenced by characteristics such as
quality and amount of student effort; high-stakes achievement tests that
have important implications for students' future education and
career path; and tests that closely match the school curriculum in a
particular school. Following from this reasoning, the BFLPE is likely to
be smaller when based on 'low stakes' tests that do not
specifically match the school curriculum like those used in the present
investigation. This consideration is, however, a double-edged sword in
that achievement tests that did match the curriculum of each school and
state/territory would not have provided a common basis of comparison.
BFLPE research provides an alternative, contradictory perspective
to educational policy on the placement of students in special education
settings that is being enacted throughout the world. Remarkably, despite
the very different issues, this clash between our research and much
existing policy exists at both ends of the achievement continuum (also
see Robinson, Zigler, & Gallagher, 2000). In gifted education research and policy, there is an increasing trend towards the provision
of highly segregated educational settings--special gifted-and-talented
classes and academically selective schools for very bright students.
This policy direction is based in part on a labelling theory
perspective, which suggests that bright students will have higher
self-concepts and experience other psychological benefits from being
educated in the company of other academically gifted students. Yet our
BFLPE and empirical evaluation of the effects of academically selective
settings (e.g. Marsh, Chessor, Craven, & Roche, 1995) showed exactly
the opposite effects. Placement of gifted students in academically
selective settings results in lower academic self-concepts, not higher
academic self-concepts. In recent research and policy for academically
disadvantaged students, there is a worldwide inclusion movement to
integrate these students into mainstream, regular classroom settings.
Although economic rationalist perspectives appear to be the underlying
motive for such decisions, the espoused rhetoric is based on a direct
application of labelling theory. According to labelling theory,
academically disadvantaged children are likely to be stigmatised and
suffer lower self-concepts as a consequence of being placed in special
classes with other academically disadvantaged students. Yet theory
underpinning the BFLPE and empirical evaluation of the effects of
including academically disadvantaged students in regular mainstream
classrooms showed exactly the opposite effects (Tracey, Marsh, &
Craven, 2003). Placement of academically disadvantaged children into
regular classrooms results in lower academic self-concepts, not higher
academic self-concepts. Furthermore the negative effects of inclusion on
Peer self-concept reported by Tracey et al. suggested that academically
disadvantaged children in regular classrooms actually feel socially
excluded, not included. Whereas not all gifted and talented students
will suffer lower academic self-concepts when attending academically
selective high schools, many will. Similarly not all academically
disadvantaged students will suffer lower academic self-concepts when
attending regular, mixed-ability classes, but many will. BFLPE research,
however, provides an important alternative perspective to existing
policy directions that have not been adequately evaluated in relation to
current educational and psychological research. The present
investigation is particularly important in demonstrating the
generalisability across Australian states/ territories of the
theoretical and empirical basis of our claims.
Table 1 Summary of multilevel models applied separately to each
state/territory
Mean Mean
Number Number academic academic
State/ of of achieve- self-
territory students schools ment concept
1 Australian Capital
Territory 500 23 .20 * .07
2 New South Wales 806 38 .15 * .00
3 Victoria 782 34 -.15 * .00
4 Queensland 761 34 -.04 -.04
5 South Australia 606 28 .04 -.02
6 Western Australia 579 27 .07 .00
7 Tasmania 535 24 -.13 .03
8 Northern Territory 287 15 -.29 * -.04
Total 4916 223 .00 .00
Effects (in prediction
of academic self-
concept) for individual
states/territories
School
Individual Individual average
student student achieve-
State/ achieve- achieve- ment
territory ment ment (BFLPE)
1 Australian Capital
Territory .27 * .14 * -.40 *
2 New South Wales .50 * .05 -.35 *
3 Victoria .51 * .13 * -.36 *
4 Queensland .38 * .07 * -.47 *
5 South Australia .45 * .07 -.44 *
6 Western Australia .30 * .06 -.29 *
7 Tasmania .31 * .04 -.32 *
8 Northern Territory .44 * .08 -.14
Total .40 * .07 -.35 *
Note: Individual student achievement = Individual student achievement
(linear component); Individual student [achievement.sup.2] = Squared
Individual student achievement (quadratic component). All outcome
and predictor variables were standardised (M = 0, SD = 1) across the
entire sample of 4916 individual students (product terms and
school-average variables were not restandardised). A multilevel
analysis of achievement test scores indicated that the set of eight
states/territories was statistically significant and when each
state/ territory was considered separately, four were statistically
significant. A similar analysis on academic self-concept responses
resulted in no significant differences among the states/territories.
* p<.05
Table 2 Summary of different multilevel models considered
(see Tables 3 and 4)
Model LLH DLLH Ddf Description of model
0 13948 Variance component (baseline) model
with no predictor variables
1 13299 649 * 4 Model 0 + linear individual
achievement (LIA) (a) and Quadratic
IA (QIA)
2 13232 67 * 1 Model 1 + school-average
achievement (SAA)
3 13222 12 * 2 Model 2 + SAAxLIA and SSAxQIA
4 13218 4 7 Model 3 + main effects of states/
territories (State) (b)
5 13200 18 * 7 Model 4 + interaction of states/
territories and LIA (StatexLIA)
6 13194 6 7 Model 5 + interaction of states/
territories and SAA (StatexSSA)
Note: LLH = -2*log likelihood ratio. DLLH = difference in LHR between
the model being tested and the model with which it is compared (as
indicated in the Model description). Ddf = difference in degrees of
freedom (number of new parameters estimated in the new model).
(a) The linear component of individual student achievement (LIA) was
random at the school level (level 2) so that there were three
additional parameters associated with the introduction of this effect
(its effect, a variance component at level 2, and a covariance
component between this variable and variance in school intercept
terms). All other variables were fixed effects so that the
introduction of one additional variable resulted in one additional
parameter estimate and a change of 1 degree of freedom.
(b) The set of 8 states and territories was represented by a set of 7
dummy variables (scored 0 or 1) in which one state was arbitrarily left
out to serve as a basis of comparison (see Table 4).
* p < .05
Table 3 Academic self-concept: The effects of individual student
achievement and school-average achievement
Model 1 Model 2
Model
variables Effect SE Effect SE
Fixed effects
Ind Ach .375 .015 .418 .016
Ind [Ach.sup.2] .061 .010 .063 .010
School MAch (BFLPE) -.302 .035
SMAch x IAch
SMAch x I[Ach.sup.2]
Constant -.059 .019 -.066 .018
Residual variance components
Lev 2 school .025 .006 .010 .005
Ind Ach .007 .005 .007 .004
Lev 1 students .851 .018 .849 .018
Model 3
Model
variables Effect SE
Fixed effects
Ind Ach .396 .017
Ind [Ach.sup.2] .072 .012
School MAch (BFLPE) -.353 .038
SMAch x IAch .004 .034
SMAch x I[Ach.sup.2] .058 .017
Constant -.072 .018
Residual variance components
Lev 2 school .010 .005
Ind Ach .006 .004
Lev 1 students .848 .018
Note: Ind ach = Individual student achievement (linear component);
Ind [ach.sup.2] = Squared Individual student achievement (quadratic
component) ; School MAch = School-average achievement (the
big-fish-little-pond effect--BFLPE; indicated by shading) ; SMAch
x IAch = Interaction between School-average achievement and Individual
student achievement; SMAch x I[ach.sup.2] = Interaction between
School-average achievement and Squared Individual student achievement.
All outcome and predictor variables were standardised (M = 0, SD = 1)
so that effects correspond to standardised beta weights. All parameter
estimates are statistically significant when they differ from zero
by more than two standard errors (SEs). Analyses are based on responses
by 4916 students from 223 schools.
Table 4 Differences between states and territories (Model 6)
Interactions
with:
Main effects Individ Ach
Dummy variables Effect SE Effect SE
ACT .074 .061 -.141 .060
Victoria .047 .053 .025 .054
Queensland -.021 .052 -.064 .054
South Australia -.008 .056 .011 .059
Western Australia .027 .057 -.154 .057
Tasmania .054 .059 -.132 .059
Northern Territory .092 .088 -.058 .068 .366
Interactions
with:
School-avg
Ach
Dummy variables Effect SE
ACT .078 .141
Victoria .040 .127
Queensland .014 .142
South Australia .015 .142
Western Australia .154 .152
Tasmania .125 .136
Northern Territory .092 .188
Note: Ach = achievement. Effect = effect in the path model. SE =
standard error (effects greater than 2 SE are statistically significant
at p < .05). In Model 6 (see also Tables 1 and 2), the set of 8 states
and territories was represented by a set of 7 dummy variables (scored 0
or 1) in which New South Wales was arbitrarily selected as the 'left
out' category to serve as a basis of comparison (see earlier
discussion). Whereas the selection of which state was to be left out
had no effect on the overall test of significance, tests of the
individual components had to be made relative to the left-out category.
Acknowledgements
This research was funded in part by grants from the Australian
Research Council. I would like to thank Kit-Tai Hau, Wolfram Schulz, Ken
Rowe, and Upali Jayasinghe for comments on an earlier version of the
article. Requests for further information about this investigation
should be sent to the author.
References
Aiken, L. S. & West, S. G. (1991). Multiple regression: Testing
and interpreting interactions. Newbury Park, CA: Sage.
Alwin, D. F. & Otto, L. B. (1977). High school context effects
on aspirations. Sociology of Education, 50, 259-273.
Branden, N. (1994). Six pillars of self-esteem. New York: Bantam.
Byrne, B. M. (1996). Measuring self-concept across the lifespan:
Issues and instrumentation. Washington, DC: American Psychological
Association.
Chapman, J. W. (1988). Learning disabled children's
self-concepts. Review of Educational Research, 58, 347-371.
Coleman, J. M. & Fults, B. A. (1985). Special class placement,
level of intelligence, and the self-concept of gifted children: A social
comparison perspective. Remedial and Special Education, 6, 7-11.
Craven, R G., Marsh, H. W., & Print, M. (2000). Selective,
streamed and mixed-ability programs for gifted students: Impact on
self-concept, motivation, and achievement. Australian Journal of
Education, 44, 51-75.
Davis, J. A. (1966). The campus as a frog pond: An application of
theory of relative deprivation to career decisions for college men.
American Journal of Sociology, 72, 17-31.
Diener, E. & Fujita, F. (1997). Social comparison and
subjective well-being. In B. P. Buunk & F. X. Gibbons (Eds.),
Health, coping, and well-being: Perspectives from social comparison
theory (pp. 329-358). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Festinger, L. (1954). A theory of social comparison processes.
Human Relations, 7, 117-140.
Gamoran, A., Nystrand, M., Berends, M., & LePore, P. C. (1995).
An organizational analysis of the effects of ability grouping. American
Educational Research Journal, 32, 687-715.
Goldstein, H. (1995). Multilevel statistical models. London:
Arnold.
Goldstein, H., Rasbash, J., Plewis, I., Draper, D., Browne, W.,
Yang, M., Woodhouse, G., & Healy, M. (1998). A user's guide to
MLwiN. London: University of London, Institute of Education.
Gross, M. (1992). The use of radical acceleration in cases of
extreme intellectual precocity. Gifted Child Quarterly, 36, 91-99.
Gross, M. (1993). Exceptionally gifted children. London: Routledge.
Gross, M. (1997). How ability groups turns big fish into little--or
does it? Of optical illusions and optimal environments. Australasian
Journal of Gifted Education, 6(2), 18-30.
Helson, H. (1964). Adaptation-level theory. New York: Harper &
Row.
Hyman, H. (1942). The psychology of subjective status.
Psychological Bulletin, 39, 473-474.
Inquiry into the Provision of Public Education in New South Wales
(Tony Vinson, Chair). (2002). Report of the Vinson Inquiry: Sponsored by
the New South Wales Teachers Federation and the Federation of P&C
Associations of New South Wales (3 vols.). Retrieved from
http://www.pub-ed-inquiry.org/reports/final_reports/
Jerusalem, M. (1984). Reference group, learning environment and
self-evaluations: A dynamic multi-level analysis with latent variables.
In R. Schwarzer (Ed.), The self in anxiety, stress and depression (pp.
61-73). Amsterdam: Elsevier Science Publishers.
Marsh, H. W. (1974). Judgmental anchoring: Stimulus and response
variables. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of California,
Los Angeles.
Marsh, H. W. (1984). Self-concept: The application of a frame of
reference model to explain paradoxical results. Australian Journal of
Education, 28, 165-181.
Marsh, H. W. (1987). The big-fish-little-pond effect on academic
self-concept. Journal of Educational Psychology, 79, 280-295.
Marsh, H. W. (1990). A multidimensional, hierarchical self-concept:
Theoretical and empirical justification. Educational Psychology Review,
2, 77-172.
Marsh, H. W. (1991). The failure of high ability high schools to
deliver academic benefits: The importance of academic self-concept and
educational aspirations. American Educational Research Journal, 28,
445-480.
Marsh, H. W. (1993). Academic self-concept: Theory, measurement and
research. In J. Suls (Ed.), Psychological perspectives on the self (Vol.
4, pp. 59-98). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Marsh, H. W. (1994). Using the National Educational Longitudinal
Study of 1988 to evaluate theoretical models of self-concept: The
Self-Description Questionnaire. Journal of Educational Psychology, 86,
439-456.
Marsh, H. W. (2002, July 26). Comment: Pupils can lose their shine
in elite schools. Sydney Morning Herald, Editorial Section, p. 11.
Retrieved from http://www.smh.com.au/articles/2002/07/25/1027497382341.html
Marsh, H. W., Chessor, D., Craven, R. G., & Roche, L. (1995).
The effects of gifted and talented programs on academic self-concept:
The big fish strikes again. American Educational Research Journal, 32,
285-319.
Marsh, H. W. & Craven, R. G. (1998). The big fish little pond
effect, optical illusions, and misinterpretations: A response to Gross
[1997]. Australasian Journal of Gifted Education, 7, 6-15.
Marsh, H. W. & Craven, R. (2002). The pivotal role of frames of
reference in academic self-concept formation: The big fish little pond
effect In F. Pajares & T. Urdan (Eds.), Adolescence and education
(Vol. 2, pp. 83-123). Greenwich, CT: Information Age.
Marsh, H. W. & Johnston, C. F. (1993). Multidimensional
self-concepts and frames of reference: Relevance to the exceptional
learner. In F. E. Obiakor & S. Stile (Eds.), Self-concept of
exceptional learners: Current perspectives for educators. Dubuque, IA:
Kendall/Hunt.
Marsh, H. W., Koller, O., & Baumart, J. (2001). Reunification
of East and West German school systems: Longitudinal multilevel modeling
study of the big fish little pond effect on academic self-concept.
American Educational Research Journal, 38, 321-350.
Marsh, H. W., Kong, C-K, Hau, K-T. (2000). Longitudinal multilevel
modeling of the big fish little pond effect on academic self-concept:
Counterbalancing social comparison and reflected glory effects in Hong
Kong high schools. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 78,
337-349.
Marsh, H. W. & Parker, J. (1984). Determinants of student
self-concept: Is it better to be a relatively large fish in a small pond
even if you don't learn to swim as well? Journal of Personality and
Social Psychology, 47, 213-231.
Marsh, H. W., Plucker, J. A., & Stocking, V. B. (2001). The
Self-Description Questionnaire II and gifted students: Another look at
Plucker, Taylor, Callahan & Tomchin's (1997) 'Mirror,
mirror on the wall'. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 61,
976-996.
Marsh, H. W. & Rowe, K.J. (1996). The negative effects of
school-average ability on academic self-concept--an application of
multilevel modelling. Australian Journal of Education, 40, 65-87.
Marsh, H. W. & Yeung, A. S. (1998). Longitudinal structural
equation models of academic self-concept and achievement: Gender
differences in the development of math and English constructs. American
Educational Research Journal, 35(4), 705-738.
Meyer, J. W. (1970). High school effects on college intentions.
American Journal of Sociology, 76, 59-70.
Morse, S. & Gergen, K.J. (1970). Social comparison,
self-consistency, and the concept of self. Journal of Personality &
Social Psychology, 16, 148-156.
Organisation for Economic Cooperation and Development. (2001a).
Knowledge and skills for life: Results from the first OECD programme for
international student assessment (PISA) 2000. Paris: Author.
Organisation for Economic Cooperation and Development. (2001b).
PISA international data base (Technical Report). Paris: Author.
Parducci, A. (1995). Happiness, pleasure, and judgment: The
contextual theory and its applications. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Raudenbush, S. W. & Bryk, A. S. (2002). Hierarchical linear
models: Applications and data analysis methods (2nd ed.). Thousand Oaks,
CA: Sage.
Robinson, N. M., Zigler, E., & Gallagher, J. J. (2000). Two
tails of the normal curve: Similarities and differences in the study of
mental retardation and giftedness. American Psychologist, 55, 1413-1424.
Sampson, J. F. (1969). The high school examination achievements of
opportunity "C" class pupils. Sydney: NSW Department of
Education.
Sampson, J. F. (1977). The secondary school performance of pupils
in selective and comprehensive schools. In New South Wales Department of
Education, The education of the talented child (pp. 111-131). Sydney:
Author.
Sherif, M. & Sherif, C. W. (1969). Social psychology. New York:
Harper & Row.
Stouffer, S. A., Suchman, E.A., DeVinney, L.C., Star, S.A., &
Williams, R.M. (1949). The American soldier: Adjustments during army
life (Vol. 1). Princeton: Princeton University Press.
Suls, J. M. (1977). Social comparison theory and research: An
overview from 1954. In J. M. Suls & R. L. Miller (Eds.), Social
comparison processes: Theoretical and empirical perspectives (pp. 1-20).
Washington, DC: Hemisphere.
Tracey, D. K., Marsh, H.W., & Craven, R. G. (2003).
Self-concepts of preadolescent students with mild intellectual
disabilities: Issues of measurement and educational placement. In H. W.
Marsh, R. G. Craven, & D. M. McInerney (Eds.), International
advances in self research (Vol. 1, pp. 203-230). Greenwich, CT:
Information Age.
Upshaw, H.S. (1969). The personal reference scale: An approach to
social judgment. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances in experimental social
psychology (Vol. 4, pp. 315-370). New York: Academic Press.
Wills, T. A. (1981). Downward comparison principles in social
psychology. Psychological Bulletin, 90, 245-271.
Zeidner M. & Schleyer, E.J. (1999). The big-fish-little-pond
effect for academic self-concept, test anxiety and school grades in
gifted children. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 24, 305-329.
Professor Herbert W. Marsh is Director of the SELF Research Centre,
University of Western Sydney, Bankstown Campus, Penrith, New South
Wales, 1797. E-mail: h.marsh@uws.edu.au.