Effects of appearance-related testing on ethnically diverse adolescent girls.
Yoo, Jeong-Ju ; Johnson, Kim K.P.
Adolescents rank teasing and bullying as greater problems than
racism, AIDS, or alcohol. Keltner et al. (2001) defined teasing as
"an intentional provocation accompanied by off-record markers that
together comment on something relevant to the target" (p. 234).
Teasing is a painful but persistent part of adolescents' lives and
has become near epidemic in America's classrooms (Adler, 2004).
Kalman (2003) reported that about 15% of all children are victims of
constant teasing.
Teasing is an important problem because consequences can be severe
and include acts of violence to self and others. Targets of teasing are
at risk of depression and thoughts of suicide (Kahn, 1995). Kahn (1995)
found that children who were teased about their body type were two to
three times more likely to think about or to attempt suicide than those
who were not teased. A 12-year-old in Michigan committed suicide after
experiencing continuous teasing about her clothing, being shy, and her
religious beliefs (Hunter, 2001). Several of her peers had teased her
since elementary school because she wore dark "gothic"
clothing to school.
Although adolescents may be teased about a variety of aspects of
their appearance, several researchers have documented that teasing about
weight in particular, is damaging to the emotional and physical
well-being of an adolescent. Thompson et al. (1995) found that being
teased resulted in a negative body image and an increase in restrictive
eating. In related work, Eisenberg, Newmark-Sztainer, and Story (2003)
found, regardless of the actual body weight of the adolescent, teasing
about weight was consistently associated with low body satisfaction, low
self-esteem, high depressive symptoms, and thinking about and attempting
suicide.
Combined with other problems, constant teasing by peers can push an
adolescent to violence against others. A common denominator in many of
the shooting tragedies in the past few years is that the shooters were
the victims of teasing. In 1999, two students who were reportedly
victims of teasing for years, gunned down 12 high school classmates and
a teacher before killing themselves at Columbine High School in
Littleton, Colorado (Hunter, 2001). Olejarczyk (2000) reported that a
16-year-old high school student was arrested for bringing a gun to
school, intending to shoot two female students who had been continuously
teasing him and his friends. In California, at Santee High School, one
student reportedly had been bullied about his physical appearance before
he killed two students and injured 13 others ("School
Shooting," 2001). In 2001, a ninth grader shot and killed two
students and wounded two others as a result of being "picked
on" ("Keeping School," 2001). In Cold Spring, Minnesota in September 2003, a high school student shot and killed two other high
school students. The student had been persistently teased about his
severe acne (Sand, 2003). The need to understand how teasing impacts
adolescents and how they respond is apparent from the severity of the
potential outcomes. The ability to successfully respond to teasing has
implications for maintaining and developing adolescents'
self-image, confidence, and coping behaviors as well as their ability to
form friendships.
Teasing that occurs during childhood and adolescence can have
enduring effects. In early research, Berscheid, Walster, and Bohrnstedt
(1973) found that adult women who had been teased about their appearance
during their adolescent years had a higher level of body dissatisfaction
than those who had not been teased. Cash, Winstead, and Janda (1986)
found that people who reported being teasing by peers during childhood
about their appearance were more likely to report being dissatisfied with their present body image than those who had not been teased.
Thompson et al. (1991) investigated connections between physical
appearance satisfaction and history of being teased about weight. Those
who suffered with eating-disturbances compared to those with no eating
disturbances, had been teased more often and reported experiencing a
greater impact as a result.
In subsequent research, Grilo, Wilfley, Brownell, and Rodin (1994)
found that the frequency of being teased about weight and size while
growing up was associated with body-image concerns during adulthood.
Cash (1995) reported similar long-term effects of appearance-related
teasing. The majority of women reported that they had experienced
appearance-related teasing often during middle childhood to early
adolescence. Moreover, the majority found these events to be moderately
or very upsetting, and said that their current body had, to some extent,
been affected by the experiences.
Teasing can have varying levels of impact depending on whether the
focus of the tease is on innate (unchangeable) or mutable (controllable)
characteristics (Brown, Cash & Noles, 1986; Patzer, 1985). Since
innate characteristics are taken more seriously, the teasing may have a
greater negative effect (Alberts, Kellar-Guenther, & Corman, 1996).
Researchers (Kowalski, 2000; Thompson, Fabian, Moulton, Dunn, &
Altabe, 1991) have focused mostly on how body satisfaction of Caucasians
is affected by weight-related teasing. However, since aspects of
appearance other than weight can provoke teasing, we asked ethnically
diverse adolescents about their experiences with teasing concerning any
aspect of their appearance. In addition, the potential consequences of
teasing that may affect adolescent decision-making were investigated.
Specifically we were interested in examining relationships between
ethnicity and (1) the content of the tease, (2) experiences of being
teased, (3) reasons for being teased, and (4) behavioral responses
(i.e., change of appearance, consumption behavior).
RELATED LITERATURE
Teasing and the Self
The self consists of an individual's consciousness of being
(James, 1890). A main principle of symbolic interaction theory is that
the "self is established, maintained, and altered in and through
communication" (Stone, 1962, p. 86). Dress is a form of nonverbal
communication that may play a role in establishing and maintaining a
sense of self. Dress consists of all the modifications made to the body
and all of the supplements added to the body (Roach-Higgins &
Eicher, 1992). Using this definition, dress includes decisions made
about hair, color, makeup, weight, clothing, accessories, and many other
components of appearance management. Through our dress, as well as our
bodies, we communicate to others and to ourselves who we are and what we
view as important (Roach-Higgins & Eicher, 1992). Sontag, Peteu, and
Lee (1997) contend that adolescents in particular regard dress as a tool
for establishing self. They argue that adolescents may view clothing as
central to the self and, as a result, become meaningfully
"linked" to their clothing. The process of establishing and
communicating self through clothing may be particularly important to
adolescents as they transition through identities from childhood to
adulthood.
Stone (1962) outlined a three-step process consisting of a program,
review, & validation or challenge to explain how identities are
established through clothing. Although Stone did not describe how an
appearance is constructed, he did note that once it is constructed, a
"program" occurs. A program is derived from an
individual's attempt to construct an appearance that communicates
an identity and to respond to that appearance. Thus, even before a
person presents an appearance publicly, it is assessed privately to
determine its effectiveness in communicating the desired identity. Stone
calls "reviews" responses to an individual's appearance
by others. He maintains that when programs and reviews match, the
identity of the individual is established and the program has been
validated. When programs and reviews do not match, "the self of the
one who appears is challenged and conduct may be expected to move in the
direction of some redefinition of the challenged self" (Stone, p.
398). In this latter case, persons may decide to change their appearance
and make another attempt to communicate the desired identity or move to
adopt another.
Appraisals by peers (i.e., reviews) of adolescents' dress and
appearance play a significant role in developing self-concepts (Burns,
1979). When adolescents engage in developing their programs (i.e.,
appearance management), they anticipate the identities they would like
to have in social situations so they can present themselves accordingly.
Thus, dressing becomes an important component of establishing and
maintaining identities. Teasing can play a central role in establishing
adolescent identities (Furman & Thompson, 2002) because it
represents a review of a program.
Content of a Tease
Individuals can be teased about a wide variety of their
characteristics. Several researchers have reported that the major
content of a tease is body parts and other aspects of physical
appearance (Alberts-Kellar-Guenther & Corman, 1996; Kowalski, 2000;
Mooney, Creeser, & Blatchford, 1991). Cash (1995) found that facial
appearance and weight were the most frequent teasing content. Kowalski
(2000) reported that, for college women, the largest category of teases
dealt with appearance. Furthermore, those who teased to hurt others,
elected to tease body parts because of the target's inability to
control many of these features (Kowalski, 2000). In terms of ethnic
differences, Mooney, Creeser, and Blatchford (1991) found that more
Black children than White children reported that they were teased about
their clothes and accessories.
Teasing and Ethnicity
Some research suggests that the effects of teasing are not
necessarily similar across ethnicities. In a study of both Caucasian and
African American adult women, Akan and Grilo (1995) found weight-related
teasing was not a significant predictor of body image dissatisfaction
for African-Americans, but was a significant predictor of
dissatisfaction for Caucasians. African American women had a higher
level of body mass index, but also a higher level of body satisfaction
compared to Caucasian women.
In earlier work, Mooney, Creeser, and Blatchford (1991) also
studied both Caucasian and African American children. Two thirds of
their participants reported having been teased. Caucasian children
reported being teased more than did African American children. African
American boys were likely to say they would retaliate verbally or ignore
being teased. African American children explained that they were teased
less often than others because other children were afraid of what they
might do. African American children also noted that they received less
teasing than Caucasian children because they were popular and well-liked
among their peers.
METHOD
Participants Participants were 49 girls--Caucasian (n = 27) and
African American (n = 22). Their ages ranged from 12 to 17, with an
average age of 14.3 years. They were recruited from middle and high
schools located in a large metropolitan area in the Midwest. The teasing
experiences that participants shared all occurred within 13 months of
the time of data collection.
Data Analysis Technique
Data on experiences with teasing were collected through
autobiographical narratives (Kowalski, 2000). Participants were asked to
write about a recent occasion of being teased about some aspect of their
appearance. To assist with the development of their narratives, all
participants were asked a series of questions including "What were
you teased about?" "How did you feel about being teased?"
"How did you respond to being teased?" and "Did you
change anything about your appearance as a result of being teased? If
yes, what did you change?"
The content analytic approach outlined by Berelson (1952) was used
to analyze the narratives. This is a research strategy for drawing
inferences by identifying characteristics of messages (Holsti, 1969).
Further, content analysis is a careful reading of written material,
focused on the actual content to determine the presence of certain
words, concepts, themes, phrases, characters, or sentences in order to
quantify them. The narratives provided were defined as "text"
which was read, coded, and categorized.
The narratives were divided into two groups: Caucasian and African
American. Responses within a group were arranged by question number
(e.g., responses to the question, "How did you feel about being
teased?" were arranged together for analysis). The themes that
emerged from responses to a specific question were examined together and
compared between the two groups. Differences in substance were noted in
order to assess possible differences between the groups. As the content
emerged from the data, participants' comments on specific issues
were included in order to support the findings.
FINDINGS
Differences in the Content of Appearance-related Teasing
Although there were 49 participants in this study, some noted they
were teased about more than one aspect of appearance during the teasing
incident they described. As a result, 60 responses were included (see
Table 1). The specific items that served as the focus of a tease were
categorized as follows: clothing (26.0%); facial features (15.0%);
weight (15.0%); height (15.05%); hair (15.0%); body (8.3%); and other
(5.5%) Items in the clothing category included entire outfits, shoes,
and glasses. Items classified as facial features included makeup, acne,
teeth, forehead, and nose. Weight-related teasing included either
overweight or underweight. Height-related teasing included either being
tall or short. Hair-related teasing included hairstyles. Teasing about
the body included hips, skin color related to race--e.g., tan or pale.
Other aspects of appearance-related teasing content included being
considered as boyish, having a skin disease other than acne, and having
an attractive appearance compared to other friends (see Table 1). Our
data provide evidence that the focus of a tease can be several aspects
of physical appearance some more easily altered than others (e.g.,
clothing).
There were similarities and differences in the content of teases
between the two groups. In general, clothing was the most frequent focus
of a tease for both groups. Clothing is immediately visible and could be
the stimulus for a tease between adolescents who are unknown to each
other or seeing each other in a new context. For example, being teased
about clothing might take place as new students enter into an
established environment of students. Older students may want to
communicate the established hierarchical power structure of the
organization and use comments about clothing to "put new students
in their place." One participant shared that she was teased about
her clothing because she was new to the school.
Noticeable differences in teasing content were also found between
ethnicities. Caucasians reported being frequently teased about a single
physical feature---being overweight. Specific comments directed at
Caucasian girls included: "Shut up fatty! You need a diet. Are you
hungry?" and "You look like a fat boy." In contrast,
teasing about several different physical attributes including facial
characteristics (i.e., forehead, nose) and hairstyle were frequent among
African American participants (see Table 1).
Teasing about physical characteristics was prevalent among African
Americans. For example, only African Americans reported being teased
about having a large "butt," a large forehead, skin color, and
a large nose. Several of these items could be considered archetypical physical characteristics reflective of distinctive ethnic
characteristics and perhaps the result of teasing across ethnicities
(see Table 1).
Relationship between the Teased and Those Who Teased Them
Participants' teasing experience could also be understood in
terms of relationships to the people who teased them. Teasing stemmed from both familiar and unfamiliar others among both groups. However, for
both groups, the incident of teasing they elected to share was linked to
an individual who was known to them. For Caucasians, teasing incidents
occurred most frequently with acquaintances (35.7%), friends (28.6%),
immediate family members (14.3%), strangers (14.3%), relatives (3.6%),
and by groups of people (3.6%). For the African Americans incidents were
by acquaintances (28.0%), family members (20.0%), friends (16.0%),
strangers (16.0%), group of people (12%), and relatives (8%).
Teasing Experiences Based on Ethnicity
Five patterns of teasing experiences. Participants' responses
about their teasing experiences were placed in five broad categories.
The first contained statements reflecting negative opinions and general
attitudes toward being teased. Included were statements such as
"Teasing is bad"; many participants either hated or did not
like being teased. The second category contained statements concerning
their feelings about being teased. Included in this category were
statements such as "I was mad" or "I felt
depressed." The third category contained statements about the
residual effects of teasing on the target. Included were statements such
as "I felt lowered self-esteem," or "It made me feel
really self-conscious." The fourth category contained statements
including prosocial experiences of teasing, such as "It depends on
what they are teasing you about," or "It depends if it is my
friends, then I took it as a joke," suggesting that these
individuals perceived some teasing as playful. The fifth category
contained statements about behaviors that participants either engaged in
or considered engaging in as a result of being teased. Examples are
"It made me fight" or "I went to the bathroom to
cry" (see Table 2).
Caucasian participants frequently reported negative opinions and
attitudes toward being teased (41.3%), followed by negative emotions
(28.3%), residual effects (19.6%), prosocial aspects of teasing (8.7%),
and action taken in response to teasing (2.2%) (see Table 2). The
African American participants showed a similar pattern in that they also
frequently expressed negative opinions and attitudes toward teasing
(40.0%), followed by negative emotions (26.7%), prosocial aspects of
teasing (16.7%), actions taken in response to teasing (8.5%), and
residual effects (3.3%) (see Table 2). A noticeable difference in the
pattern is that African Americans reported pro-social aspects of teasing
with greater frequency than did Caucasians as well as willingness to
engage in some type of behavioral response (e.g., fighting).
Opinions and attitudes about teasing. Within this category, there
were more similarities than differences between the two groups. Several
participants in both groups noted that they did not care about being
teased or they did not like being teased. Examining the data for
differences between the two groups, Caucasian participants'
responses reflected strong emotions about being teased. They hated being
teased. One participant noted, "Teasing was hard to endure because
then it made me think about myself and the way I looked." In
contrast, African American participants also noted that teasing was
wrong and should be stopped (see Table 3).
Emotional aspects of teasing. There were similarities and
differences between the two groups concerning their emotional response
to teasing. For both groups, it is evident that their teasing
experiences were stressful. Comparing the two groups, the Caucasian
expressed a wide range of feelings they had when they were being teased.
They felt depressed, frustrated, and unintelligent. For example, they
commented, "Horrible, like everyone hated me and wanted me to just
go away." "I don't like it because it make me feel
stupid." Similar sentiments were not expressed by African American
participants (see Table 3).
Residual effects of teasing. With respect to the residual or
potential long-term effects of teasing, the Caucasian participants
frequently indicated that teasing made them feel self-conscious and
insecure. One commented, "When it's how I usually look, it
makes me concerned." Neither group at this point in time, provided
responses suggesting these feelings were long-term (see Table 3).
Prosocial effects of teasing. The African Americans more frequently
than the Caucasians reported prosocial aspects of teasing. Their
experiences were described as playful if they were teased by friends.
For example, one participant stated, "It depends if it's my
friend, then I took it as a joke, but if it's not my friend, I
think it's mean and hurtful." Another said, "With my
friends, I am fine with being teased because I teased them" (see
Table 3).
Behaviors as a consequence of teasing. Specific behaviors resulted
from teasing. The African Americans were willing to take action against
the teaser. Several indicated that they felt like fighting back and
standing up for themselves (see Table 3). One said, "I wanted so
much to fight her."
Reasons victims believed they were teased. Participants noted the
reasons they thought they were being teased. This question resulted in
56 responses (some participants gave more than one reason). Responses
were categorized as follows: looking different from the majority group
(32.1%); jealousy (21.4%); to get attention (12.5%); new member of
school (7.4%); quiet and small in stature (5.4%); and unknown to teaser
(5.4%). Less frequent responses included teasing was funny; due to my
friends; teaser had problems at home and took them out on me; teaser
liked me; teaser felt peer pressure to tease me; teaser did not like me;
and teaser was judgmental (see Table 4). There were differences in
responses between the two ethnic groups. A higher number of Caucasians
reported that they were teased because they looked different from the
group than did African Americans.
Responses to being teased. Concerned with identifying whether any
changes in appearance or any purchases were made to alter appearance as
a consequence of being teased, both Caucasians (37.0%), and African
Americans (40.9%) indicated that they had made changes to their
appearance or had bought new clothing as a result of being teased (see
Table 5).
The tendency for participants to change their appearance varied by
teasing content. Participants tried to modify their clothing (53.8%),
followed by hair (50%) and weight (44.4%) (see Table 6). Those who were
teased about clothing said: "I wear contacts so I won't be
teased"; and, "Because I don't want the same thing to
happen, I don't wear skirts any more. I always wear pants."
Another who was teased about her hair stated, "I went to buy some
stuff for my hair so it can grow." Although weight is one category
not easily changed quickly, a few engaged in weight management
behaviors. For example, some stopped eating for a day; others mentioned
trying to lose weight, while others started to work out (see Table 6).
DISCUSSION AND CONCLUSIONS
Clothing was noted as the most frequent cause of teasing for both
groups. This finding is somewhat inconsistent with that of Cash (1995)
who found that weight and facial characteristics were the most frequent
cause. Clothing may serve as the basis for teasing because adolescents
place importance on it. Researchers have demonstrated that clothing is
important for social acceptance during adolescence (Songtag, Peteu,
& Loe, 1997), and some adolescent groups develop informal dress
codes (Eicher & Baizerman, 1991). Not wearing the "right"
clothing may make one a target because clothing symbolizes the
wearer's otherness. One participant noted, "I was teased about
my clothes because I didn't wear Abercrombie and Fitch often and
because I was wearing something different from everyone else."
Those who were teased about their clothing or hairstyle indicated that
they did alter their appearance in an attempt to halt the teasing.
Weight was a more frequent cause of teasing for Caucasians than for
African Americans. There are differences in the way being overweight is
perceived between cultural groups which may explain why Caucasians
reported being teased about their weight to a greater extent than did
African Americans. For example, Fallon (1990) suggested cultural groups
have their own definitions of the ideal body. It is commonly observed
that Euro-Americans are obsessed with the pursuit of thinness (Brownell,
1991) and being concerned about their weight. In addition, Caucasian
participants represented the majority population at each location where
data were collected. Weight would be an observable
"difference" that could be a cause of teasing in addition to
clothing among the majority group members.
Responses to teasing varied, with African Americans indicating a
tendency to be willing to fight or at least take some action to defend
themselves from the teasing. Lamb (2001), in her discussion of
aggression in girls, noted that girls will fight when teased. Lamb noted
that the explanations provided by adolescent girls demonstrate that
their aggression was useful to them. When you "stand up to teasing
with a show of aggression, people leave you alone." (Lamb, 2001; p.
212). Fighting back in some instances may have been a matter of
survival. Young girls, especially minority girls may need to show the
teaser that they cannot be picked on and that they are to be respected.
Our results are consistent with those of Stone (1962). Some
incidents of teasing were negative reviews that resulted in at least
some participants working to change their appearance (i.e., alter their
program). Changing their appearance could result in halting the teasing
(in Stone's words--"a type of validation"), and we find
evidence that in some instances, a change in appearance did halt the
teasing. Among those participants who indicated that their response did
stop the teasing, they commented, "I styled my hair different and
wore different clothes," "I wear contacts so I won't be
teased," or "Because I don't want to look fat, I bought a
new swim suit." However, changing appearance did not always stop
the teasing because the "problem" was not easily altered. For
example, one girl noted, "I tried to lose weight, but I can't
suddenly become really skinny." Another who was teased about
wearing glasses and a scarf stated, "I did not wear the scarf
anymore, but I have to wear the glasses."
Teasing can have a significant impact during adolescence and in
many of the instances that were shared, it was negative. However,
African American participants' narratives revealed that there can
be a fun side to teasing. Similar to Keltner (1999) who found that
although Caucasians had a particularly negative view of teasing, other
ethnicities, African Americans in particular, view teasing as a part of
everyday life. Several participants noted that they liked being teased
by their friends and at least one said that the reason she was teased
was because she was liked.
Explanations for being teased can be divided into two broad
categories: those attributes associated with the teaser (e.g., jealousy,
wanting to get attention), and those associated with the teased (e.g.,
being new, looking different).
Teasing during adolescence is a complex behavior. Although several
of our participants did change their appearance as a way to stop it, it
is not certain that this response was or could be effective especially
in light of the finding that the cause of the teasing was a
characteristic of the teaser and not under their control. Assessing the
response(s) to teasing that adolescents' view as effective is
recommended as an area for additional research. Further, additional
research is needed to better understand differences between the
intra-racial and inter-racial teasing experiences and subsequent
behaviors. For example, one's ethnicity may play a role in being
able to appropriately interpret whether a tease is prosocial or
anti-social.
REFERENCES
Adler, E. (2004). Little Miss bully. St. Paul Pioneer Press, p. 1.
Akan, G. E., & Grillo, C. M. (1995). Sociocultural influences
on eating attitudes and behaviors, body image, and psychological
functioning: A comparison of African-American, Asian-American, and
Caucasian college women. International Journal of Eating Disorders,
18(2), 181-187.
Alberts, J. K., Kellar-Guenther, Y., & Corman, S. R. (1996).
That's not funny: understanding recipients' responses to
teasing. Western Journal of Communication, 60(4), 337-357.
Berelson, B. (1952). Content analysis in communication research.
Glencoe, IL: The Free Press. Berscheid, E., Walster, E., &
Bohrnstedt, G. (173). Body image: The happy American body: A survey
report. Psychology Today, 119-146.
Brown, T. A., Cash, T. F., & Noles, S. W. (1986). Perceptions
of physical attractiveness among college students: Selected determinants
and methodological matters. Journal of Social Psychology, 126(3),
305-316.
Brownell, K. D. (1991). Dieting and the search for the perfect
body: Where physiology and culture collide. Behavior Therapy, 22, 1-12.
Burns, R. B. (1979). The self concept in theory, measurement,
development and behavior. New York: Longman.
Cash, T. F. (1995). Developmental teasing about physical
appearance: Retrospective descriptions and relationships with body
image. Social Behavior and Personality, 23(2), 123-130.
Cash, T. F., Winstead, B. A., & Janda, L. H. (1986). The great
American shapeup: We're healthier than ever, fitter than ever ...
but less satisfied with how we look. Psychology Today, 20(4), 30-37.
Eicher, J. B., & Baizerman, S. (1991). Adolescent dress, Part
II: A qualitative study of suburban high school students. Adolescence,
26(103), 679-687.
Eisenberg, M. E., Neumark-Sztainder, D., & Story, M. (2003).
Associations of weight-based teasing and emotional well-being among
adolescents. Archives of Pediatrics and Adolescent Medicine, 157,
733-738.
Fallon, A. (1990). Culture in the mirror: Sociocultural
determinants of body image. In T. F. Cash & T. Pruzinsky (Eds.),
Body images: Development, deviance, and change (pp. 80-109). New York:
Guilford.
Furman, K., & Thompson, K. J. (2002). Body image, teasing, and
mood alterations: An experimental study of exposure to negative verbal
commentary.
Grillo, C. M., Wilfley, D. E., Brownell, K. D., & Rodin, J.
(1994). Teasing, body image, and self-esteem in a clinical sample of
obese women. Addictive Behavior, 19(4), 443-450.
Holsti, O. R. (1969). Content analysis for the social sciences and
humanities. Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley.
Hunter, G. (2001). Teasing and taunting led girl to end her life.
Retrieved August 29, 2003, from
www.detnews.com/2001/schools/0103/07/a01196600.htm
James, W. (1890). The principles of psychology. New York: Holt,
Rinehart and Winston.
Kahn, J. (1995). Adolescent depression: An overview. Salt Lake
City: Available from University of Utah Neuropsychiatric Institute.
Kalman, I. (2003). Teasing victims. Retrieved September 3, 2003,
from http:// community-silive.com/cc/teasingvictims
Keeping Schools Safe. (2001). Retrieved August 28, 2003, from
www.uni.edu/ profdev/safeschool/
Keltner, D., Capps, L., Kring, A. M., Young, R. C., & Heerey,
E. A. (2001). Just teasing: A conceptual analysis and empirical review.
Psychological Bulletin, 127(2), 229-248.
Kowalski, R. M. (2000). "I was only kidding!":
Victims' and perpetrators' perceptions of teasing. Personality
and Social Psychology Bulletin, 26(2), 231-241.
Lamb, S. (2001). The secret lives of girls. New York: Free Press.
Mooney, A., Creeser, R., & Blatchford, P. (1991).
Children's views on teasing and fighting in junior schools.
Educational Research, 33(2), 103-112.
Olejarczyk, G. (2000). Psychologist: Teens most vulnerable to
teasing by peers. Retrieved December 27, 2002, from
www.thehollandsentinel.net/stories/ 031300/new_teens.html
Patzer, G. L. (1985). The physical attractiveness phenomena. New
York: Plenum.
Roach-Higgins, M. E., & Eicher, J. B. (1992). Dress and
identity. Clothing and Textiles Research Journal, 10(4), 1-8.
Sand, P. (2003). 1 dead, 1 critical in school gunfire. Pioneer
Press, p. A1.
School shooting near San Diego leaves 2 dead, 13 wounded. (2001,
March 5), from http://archives.cnn.com/2001/US/03/05/school.shooting.04/
Sontag, M. S., Peteu, M., & Lee, J. (1997). Clothing in the
self-system of adolescents: Relationship among values, proximity of
clothing to self, clothing interest, anticipated outcomes and perceived
quality of life. Michigan Agricultural Experiment Station Michigan State
University Research Report.
Stone, G. P. (1962). Appearance and the self. In A. Rose (Ed.),
Human behavior and social processes (pp. 86-116). Boston: Houghton
Mifflin.
Thompson, J. K. (1991). Body shape preferences: Effects of
instructional protocol and level of eating disturbance. International
Journal of Eating Disorders, 10(2), 193-198.
Thompson, J. K., Coovert, M. D., Richards, K. J., Johnson, S.,
& Cattarin, J. (1995). Development of body image, eating
disturbance, and general psychological functioning in female
adolescents: Covariance structure modeling and longitudinal investigations. The International Journal of Eating Disorders, 18(3),
221-236.
Thompson, J. K., Fabian, L. J., Moulton, D. 0., Dunn, M. E., &
Altabe, M.N. (1991). Development and validation of the physical
appearance related testing scale. Journal of Personality Assessment,
56(3), 513-521.
This study was supported by a Faculty Grant-in-Aid from the
University of Minnesota. The authors gratefully acknowledge the
assistance of Marcus Pope, a youth director, for his assistance during
data collection.
Jeong-Ju Yoo, Textiles, Apparel and Merchandising Program, West
Virginia University.
Kim K. P. Johnson, Department of Design, Housing and Apparel,
University of Minnesota.
Requests for reprints should be sent to Jeong-Ju Yoo, Textiles,
Apparel and Merchandising Program, West Virginia University, PO Box
6124, Morgantown, WV 26506. E-mail: Jay.Yoo@mail.wvu.edu
Table 1
Teasing Categories
Teasing Specific content Caucasians African- Total
Category Americans
% n % n % n
Clothing * Clothing in 16.7 5 20.0 6 18.3 11
general 10.0 3 3.3 1 6.7 4
* Glasses 0 0 3.3 1 1.7 1
* Shoes (**)
Total 26.7 8 26.0 8 26.0 16
Face * Forehead (**) 0 0 10.0 3 5.0 3
* Acne (*) 6.7 2 0 0 3.3 2
* Makeup (*) 3.3 1 0 0 1.7 1
* Nose (**) 0 0 3.3 1 1.7 1
* Teeth (*) 6.7 2 0 0 3.3 2
Total 16.7 5 13.3 4 15.0 9
Weight * Overweight 16.7 5 10.0 3 13.3 8
* Underweight 3.3 1 0 0 1.7 1
Total 20.0 6 13.0 3 15.0 9
Height * Tall 6.7 2 10.0 3 8.3 5
* Short 6.7 2 6.7 2 6.6 4
Total 13.4 4 16.7 5 15.0 9
Hair * Hairstyles 10.0 3 20.0 6 15.0 9
Total 10.0 3 20.0 6 15.0 9
Body * Large "butt" (**) 0 0 6.7 2 3.3 2
* Racial skin- 0 0 3.3 1 1.7 1
color (**) 3.3 1 0 0 1.7 1
* skin (*) 3.3 1 0 0 1.7 1
* Tanned skin (*)
Total 6.6 2 10.0 3 8.3 5
Other * Boyish (*) 3.3 1 0 0 1.7 1
aspects of * Skin disease 0 0 3.3 1 1.7 1
appearance other than 3.3 1 0 0 1.7 1
acne (**)
* Attractive
appearance (*)
Total 6.6 2 3.3 1 5.0 3
Total 100 30 100 30 100 60
* Teasing content found only among Caucasian participants
** Teasing content found only among African-American participants
Table 2
Five Patterns of Teasing Experiences
Category How felt about being teased Caucasians Non-
Caucasians
% n % n
Opinions/ Did not care/mind 13.0 6 16.7 5
attitudes Did not like it 13.0 6 13.3 4
towards Hated being teased 8.7 4 3.3 1
teasing Felt that teasing is bad (*) 2.2 1 0 0
Felt that teasing is wrong (**) 0 0 3.3 1
Felt teasing is not cool (*) 2.2 1 0 0
Felt that easing should
stop (**) 0.0 0 3.3 1
Felt that teasing is hard to
endure (*) 2.2 1 0.0 0
Total 41.3 19 40.0 12
Emotional Hurtful 8.7 4 10.0 3
aspects Angry/upset/mad 6.5 3 6.7 2
of teasing Sad 4.3 2 3.0 1
Embarrassed (**) 0 0 6.7 2
Depressed (*) 2.2 1 0 0
Horrible (*) 2.2 1 0 0
Frustrated (*) 2.2 1 0 0
Felt stupid (*) 2.2 1 0 0
Total 28.3 13 26.7 8
Residual Felt self-conscious (*) 8.7 4 0 0
effects on Felt bad about myself 2.2 1 3.0 1
teasing Felt lower self-esteem (*) 2.2 1 0 0
target Felt bothered (*) 2.2 1 0 0
Felt I am not wanted (*) 2.2 1 0 0
Felt insecure (*) 2.2 1 0 0
Total 19.6 9 3.0 1
Prosocial Felt teasing is playful, 8.7 4 16.7 5
aspects depending on the content
of teasing or who did it
Total 8.7 4 16.7 5
Action/ * Felt like fighting (**) 0 0 6.7 2
Behavior * Cried 2.2 1 3.3 1
* Felt like standing
up for myself (**) 0.0 0 3.3 1
Total 2 1 8.5 4
Total 100 46 100 30
Category How felt about being teased Total
% n
Opinions/ Did not care/mind 14.5 11
attitudes Did not like it 13.2 10
towards Hated being teased 6.6 5
teasing Felt that teasing is bad (*) 1.3 1
Felt that teasing is wrong (**) 1.3 1
Felt teasing is not cool (*) 1.3 1
Felt that easing should
stop (**) 1.3 1
Felt that teasing is hard to
endure (*) 1.3 1
Total 40.8 31
Emotional Hurtful 9.2 7
aspects Angry/upset/mad 6.6 5
of teasing Sad 3.9 3
Embarrassed (**) 2.6 2
Depressed (*) 1.3 1
Horrible (*) 1.3 1
Frustrated (*) 1.3 1
Felt stupid (*) 1.3 1
Total 27.6 21
Residual Felt self-conscious (*) 5.3 4
effects on Felt bad about myself 2.6 2
teasing Felt lower self-esteem (*) 1.3 1
target Felt bothered (*) 1.3 1
Felt I am not wanted (*) 1.3 1
Felt insecure (*) 1.3 1
Total 13.2 10
Prosocial Felt teasing is playful, 11.8 9
aspects depending on the content
of teasing or who did it
Total 11.8 9
Action/ * Felt like fighting (**) 2.6 2
Behavior * Cried 2.6 2
* Felt like standing
up for myself (**) 1.3 1
Total 6.6 5
Total 100 76
* Specific content identified only among Caucasian participants
** Specific content identified only among African-American
participants
Table 3
Adolescents' Responses Concerning Their Shared Teasing Experience
Category Specific experience Quotes from Quotes from
when participants Caucasian African-American
were teased participants participants
Opinions, Did not like it/ I don't like I don't like it
attitudes hated it being put on the at all.
towards spot.
teasing
(I think) I hated it. I hate being
teased
Did not care/mind I didn't really I don't really
mind. care. They're
just doing
something to
make people
laugh
Felt teasing is It is bad N/A
bad (*)
Felt teasing is N/A It is just wrong
wrong (**)
Opinions, Felt teasing is I think that N/A
attitudes not cool by being teased
towards strangers (*) when it's your
teasing friends, and
(I think) they are just
being playful.
It is fine, but
otherwise, it's
not cool
Felt that teasing N/A It should stop
should stop (**)
Felt that teasing Teasing is hard N/A
is hard to to endure
endure (*)
Emotional Hurtful Teasing makes I think it is
aspects Angry/upset/mad you hurt inside. hurtful.
of teasing
(I feel) I feel upset, I got really mad
especially when
they are not my
friends
Sad I felt really Sometimes it
sad makes me very
sad.
Embarrassed (**) N/A I was very
embarrassed
about it
Depressed (*) Depressed N/A
Felt horrible (*) Horrible. Like N/A
everyone hated
me
Frustrated (*) At the moment, I N/A
was kind of
frustrated
Felt stupid (*) It makes me feel N/ACategory
stupid
Residual Felt bad about It makes you Teasing makes me
effects on myself feel bad. feel not good
teasing about myself.
target
(long Felt self- It made me feel N/A
period) conscious (*) really self-
conscious, like
he was always
watching me.
Felt lower self- It lowers my N/A
esteem. (*) self-esteem.
Felt bothered/ It gets really N/A
annoyed (*) annoying
Felt not wanted (*) Like everyone N/A
wanted me to
just go away
Felt insecure (*) I feel insecure N/A
Prosocial Felt that teasing I think that It depends on
aspects of is playful, being teased what they are
teasing depending on the when it's your teasing you
content or who did friends and they about.
it are just being
playful It depends if it
is my friend,
then I took it
as a joke; if
it's not my
friend, I think
it is mean and
hurtful
Action/ Felt like N/A It made me fight
fighting (**)
Behavior Cried I went to It hurt my
(I took an bathroom to cry feeling, and
action) sometimes I cry
Felt like standing N/A It makes me
up for myself (**) stand up for
myself
* Specific content found only among Caucasian participants
** Specific content found only among African-American participants
Table 4
Reasons Teaser Believed that They Were Teased
Reasons that they were teased Caucasian African- Total
American
% n % n % n
I looked different 37.5 12 25.0 6 32.1 18
Teaser is jealous of me 21.9 7 20.8 5 21.4 12
Teaser wanted attention 9.4 3 16.7 4 12.5 7
I am quiet and little to attack 3.1 1 8.3 2 5.4 3
back
Teaser did not know me exactly 6.3 2 4.2 1 5.4 3
I was new at school 9.4 3 4.2 1 7.1 4
Teaser thought it was funny 3.1 1 4.2 1 3.6 2
Due to friends I hang out 3.1 1 4.2 1 3.6 2
Teasers' problem at home took 0 0 4.2 1 1 1
them out on me
Teaser liked me 0 0 4.2 1 1 1
Teaser had a peer pressure 0 0 4.2 1 1 1
Teaser did not like me 3.1 1 0 0 1 1
Teaser was judgmental 3.1 1 0 0 1 1
Total 100 32 100 24 100 56
Table 5
Modification of Appearance and Ethnicity
Caucasian African-American Both groups
% n % n % n
Yes, I did 37.0 10 40.9 9 38.8 19
change.
No, I did not 63.0 17 54.5 12 59.2 29
change.
Not sure 0 0 4.5 1 2.0 1
(Both)
Total 100% 27 22 100% 100% 59
Table 6
Changes Participants Made after Being Teased, Based on Content
Content of teasing Quotes concerning why participants modified
their appearance
Clothing I didn't change it for them. I changed it for
myself.
(modified 53.8%; Yes, I bought new clothes and shoes. Because
not modified 38.5%; I feel strongly about not changing for anyone.
not sure 7.7%)
I wear contacts so I won't be teased.
Face I started to buy stuff for my acne.
(modified 40.0% Well, I got my tooth fixed, even though I was
not modified 60.0%) going to anyway, but then kind of showed it off.
Weight I stopped eating for days at a time.
(modified 44.4%, Because I don't want to look fat, I bought a new
not modified 44.4%; swimsuit.
not sure 11.1%
I tried to lose weight.
Height I didn't wear high-heeled shoes.
(not modified 1000
Hair Yes, I went to buy some stuff for my hair so it
(modified 50.0%1 can grow.
not modified 50.0%)
I grew my hair really long.
Body Not found
(modified 20%;
not modified 1000
Other I started working out and wearing makeup.