The influence of violent behavior and victimization at school on psychological distress: the role of parents and teachers.
Estevez, Estefania ; Musitu, Gonzalo ; Herrero, Juan 等
Since the 1980s, research analyzing behavioral problems at school
among school-aged children and adolescents has been increasing. Most of
the research focused on victimization problems has repeatedly shown that
victimized students exhibit some psychosomatic symptoms and poor
psychological adjustment (Alsaker & Olweus, 1992; Juvonen, Nishina,
& Graham, 2000; Kupersmidt, Coie, & Dodge, 1990). Recent
studies, for instance, report that depression and stress in particular
are common among adolescents involved in victimization problems
(Guterman, Hahm, & Cameron, 2002; Kumpulainen, Rasanen, & Puura,
2001; Hawker & Boulton, 2000). Previous research that focused on
violent students, however, has shown little co-occurrence between
violent behavior and psychological problems in adolescence (Angold &
Costello, 1993). Along this line, depressive symptoms and violent
behavior have been found to co-occur only in about 5% to 8% of
adolescents (Garnefski & Diekstra, 1997; Ge, Best, Conger, &
Simons, 1996). One possible explanation is that violent behavior at this
stage of life may be normative and even beneficial for social adjustment
in some adolescents (Little, Brauner, Jones, Nock, & Hawley, 2003).
As Hawley and Vaughn (2003) report, aggressive students are often
important figures in their peer group and tend to enjoy benefits of
social inclusion. However, this is not the case for a relatively small
group of adolescents who are at a particular high risk for future
maladjustment (Ferdinand, Stijnen, Verhulst, & Van der Reijden,
1999), and both behavioral and psychological problems. Other variables,
therefore, should be taken into account to understand the link between
these adjustment problems in adolescence.
In this sense, prior studies have examined the association betweren
some family variables and children's behavioral and psychological
problems. For example, a negative family environment has been found to
be a risk factor for peer victimization and violent behavioir at school
(Gerard & Buehler, 1999; Smith, Bowers, Binney, & Cowie, 1993),
as well as for psychological problems in adolescents (Cummings &
Davies, 1994; Formoso, Gonzales, & Aiken, 2000; Garber, 1996). In
particular, high levels of family conflict (Ary, Duncan, Biglan,
Metzler, Noell, & Smolkowski, 1999; Crawford-Brown, 1999; Cummings,
Goeke-Morey & Papp, 2003), negative communications with parents
(Liu, 2003; Loeber, Drinkwater, Yin, Anderson, Schmidt, & Crawford,
2000; Stevens, De Bourdeaudhuij, & Van Oost, 2002), and lack of
parental support (Barrera & Li, 1996; Sheeber, Hops, Alpert, Davis,
& Andrews, 1997), are important influential factors in the
development of behavioral and psychological problems among adolescents.
In addition, some studies indicate that when behavioral problems
co-occur with psychological distress, adolescents usually reveal an
especially negative family environment (Olsson, NordstrSm, Arinell,
&von Knorring, 1999; Overbeek, Biesecker, Stattin, Engels, &
Meeus, 2002). In contrast, close parent-child relationships
characterized by warmth, acceptance, and positive communication, perform
a protective function against violent behavior, depression, and anxiety
(Beam, Gil-Rivas, Greenberger, & Chen, 2002; Buist & Dekovic,
2004; Dadds, Sanders, Morrison, & Rebgetz, 1992; Gil-Rivas,
Greenberger, Chen, Montero, & Lopez-Lena, 2003; Kenny, Gallagher,
Alvarez-Salvat, & Silsby, 2002).
Some previous research has also analyzed the relationship between
teachers and students who are having behavioral and psychological
problems, pointing out that adolescents with high levels of behavioral
problems have more negative interactions with teachers (Fry, 1983; Jack,
Shores, Denny, Gunter, DeBriere, & DePaepe, 1996), which results in
a poor student-teacher relationship (Blankemeyer, Flannery, &
Vazsonyi, 2002). Furthermore, teachers' responses to these
adolescents are usually punishment (Coie & Koeppl, 1990), and lack
of warmth and encouragement (Birch & Ladd, 1998). More recent
studies note that a positive teacher-student relationship decreases the
level of violence in the classroom, as well as symptoms of depression in
students (Meehan, Hughes, & Cavell, 2003; Reddy, Rhodes, &
Mulhall, 2003).
Aims of the Current Study
The present study separately analyzed the influence of violent
behavior and victimization at school on adolescent psychological
adjustment. But given that both behavioral and psychological problems in
adolescence negatively affect and are negatively affected by family and
school variables, such as the quality of interaction with parents and
teachers (Begotti, Borca, Calandri, Cattelino, & Ingoglia, 2004;
Eisenberg, Fabes, Shepard, Guthrie, Murphy, & Reiser, 1999), it
seems appropriate to explore the role that parents and teachers play in
the relationship between violent behavior/victimization and
psychological adjustment. Therefore, we examined whether violence and
victimization problems would interact with family (communication with
father and mother) and school (teacher's perception of students)
variables in contributing to levels of psychological distress. Based on
findings of prior studies, it was expected that violent behavior and
victimization would show differential paths in their influence on
adolescents' psychological adjustment. In particular, we
hypothesized that victimization would be directly associated with
psychological distress, but that violent behavior would not directly
affect adolescents' psychological adjustment. Violent behavior was
expected to show, however, an influence on interactions with parents and
teachers which, in turn, would have a negative effect on psychological
distress.
METHOD
Participants
Participants in the study were 1,068 Spanish adolescents from four
public schools in the Valencian Community, of whom 983 provided complete
data for all study variables. Ages ranged from 11 to 16 years (mean age
13.7); 47.2% were boys. For multigroup analyses, we split two age groups
(11-13 and 14-16 years); 45.5% were in the 11- 13-year-old group.
Procedure
Teachers at the schools were informed about the objectives of the
study during a two-hour presentation. Parents received a letter
including an introductory summary of the investigation. Prior to data
collection, written parental consent was obtained. Participants
anonymously filled out the questionnaire during a regular class period
in May 2002, in addition to providing other measures not used in this
study. All measures were administered within each classroom on the same
day. Teachers were also asked to complete a teacher report for each
participating adolescent.
Measures
Depressive symptoms were assessed by the Center of Epidemiological
Studies Depression Scale (CES-D; Radloff, 1977). Participants reported
the frequency of 20 symptoms experienced over the past month, including
depressed mood, feeling of hopelessness, somatic and retarded activity,
and interpersonal distress (e.g., "I was bothered by things that
usually don't bother me"). Responses were rated on a
four-point scale (1 = never, 4 = always). Cronbach's alpha reliability for this scale in the present study was .90.
Perceived stress was measured using the Perceived Stress Scale
(PSS; Cohen, Kamarck, & Mermelstein, 1983). The PSS is a 14-item
scale which measures the degree to which respondents appraised
situations as stressful within the last month (e.g., "In the last
month, how often have you found that you could not cope with all the
things that you have to do?") on a five-point scale (1 = never, 5 =
very often). Coefficient alpha in the current sample for this scale was
.82.
Violent behavior at school and peer victimization were assessed
using a scale adapted from Emler and Reicher (1995). Participants
indicated the frequency with which they had engaged in 19 deviant
behaviors at school in the past 12 months on a six-point scale (0 = I
don't want to share this information, 1 = never, 5 = many times).
Approximately 7% of respondents chose not to provide the information
contained in some items marking them "0"; these subjects were
removed from the analysis. We conducted principal component analysis
with varimax rotation and found a two-factor structure. The first factor
explained 45.03% of variance and grouped 13 items referring to
disruptive behavior, physical and verbal aggression at school (e.g.,
"I have hit somebody at school"). The second factor explained
16.90% of variance ant~ grouped 6 items referring to victimization at
school (e.g., "A classmate insulted me"). Cronbach's
alpha coefficients for these subscales were .84 and .82, respectively.
Adolescent's communication with parents was measured using the
20-item Parent-Adolescent Communication Scale (PACS; Barnes & Olson,
1982). Adolescents described communication with their parents on a
five-point scale (1 = never, 5 = always). The original scale showed a
two-factor structure referring to degree of openness and extent of
problems in family communication (coefficients alpha .87 and .78;
test-retest reliabilities .78 and .77, respectively). However, we could
not replicate this factor structure in our data. Principal component
analysis with varimax rotation yielded a three-factor structure for
father and mother separately. The first factor explained 30.66% of
variance and grouped 10 items referring to open communication with
parents (e.g., "my mother/father is always a good listener").
The second factor explained 21.85% of variance and grouped 6 items
referring to offensive communication with parents (e.g., "my
mother/father insults me when she/he is angry with me"). Finally,
the third factor explained 9.52% of variance and group 4 items referring
to avoidant communication with parents (e.g., "I am sometimes
afraid to ask my mother/father for what I want"). Cronbach's
reliability coefficients for these subscales were .87, .76, and .75,
respectively.
Teacher's perception of students. Teachers were asked to
estimate levels of adolescents' social integration at school,
relationship with teacher, and degree of personal adjustment, on a
ten-point scale (1 = very bad, 10 = very good). Cronbach's alpha
reliability for these three items in the current study was .76.
RESULTS
Preliminary correlational analyses among all study variables were
carried out (see Table 1). Variables concerning communication with
parents and teacher's perception were significantly associated with
violent behavior at school, victimization, and psychological distress.
Therefore, these variables were included in the subsequent regression
analysis.
We used EQS 6.0 (Bender, 1995) structural equation program to
examine the influence of violent behavior and victimization at school on
adolescent psychological distress, through their effects on interactions
with both parents and teachers. Santorra-Bentler corrected statistics
were used to account for the non-normality of the data (normalized
estimate = 33.7939). The model showed a reasonably good fit: S-B[chi
square](47, N = 983) = 106.55, CFI = .986, IFI = .986, NFI = .977, and
RMSEA = .036 (90% confidence interval .027-.045). For the CFI, IFI, and
NNFI, values above .95 or higher are acceptable; for the RMSEA, values
above .05 or less are acceptable. This model explained 47.7% of variance
in psychological distress.
Table 2 reports all factor loadings of the observed variables on
their latent variables. Open communication, offensive communication, and
avoidant communication were used as indicators of the latent variables:
Communication with Father and Communication with Mother. Social
integration at school, relationship with teacher, and the
adolescent's personal adjustment, were used as indicators of the
latent variable; Teacher's Perception. Depressive symptoms and
perceived stress were used as indicators of the latent variable:
Psychological Distress. Finally, because Violent Behavior at School and
Peer Victimization consisted of only one indicator, their factor
loadings were 1 with an error 0.
Figure 1 presents the structural model with the standardized path
coefficients and their confidence intervals. We found violent behavior
to be positively associated with peer victimization (r = .39, p <
.001). Correlated errors were also found between communication with
father and mother (r = .65, p < .001), communication with father and
teacher's perception of students (r =. 14, p < .001), and
communication with mother and teacher's perception (r = .14, p <
.001).
[FIGURE 1 OMITTED]
In the prediction of adolescents' psychological distress,
results indicated violent behavior and victimization at school followed
different patterns. Peer victimization showed a direct contribution to
psychological distress ([beta] = .13, p < .001), reporting victimized
adolescents as having higher levels of distress than those not
victimized. Also, we found an indirect influence of peer victimization
on distress through its negative effect on adolescents'
communication with father (indirect effect .05, p < 001).
Victimization, however, did not show any influence on either
communication with mother or teacher's s perception.
Regarding violent behavior at school, no direct effect on
psychological distress was found. In this case, only an indirect path
was found: violent behavior showed a negative influence on
adolescents' communication with father ([beta] = -.23, p < .001)
and mother ([beta] = -.27, p < .001), as well as on teacher's
perception ([beta] = -.29, p < .001) which, in turn, were negatively
associated with psychological distress ([beta] = -.33, p < .001;
[beta] = -.36, p < .001; [beta] = -.09, p < .01, respectively).
These results indicated that the effect of violent behavior on distress
was mainly explained through its negative influence on interactions with
both parents and--though lower in strength--with teachers (indirect
effect .20, p < .001).
Finally, to test the measurement and structural invariance of the
general model across gender and age groups we conducted multigroup
analyses. Two models were tested for each multigroup comparison. In the
first between-group model (unrestricted model) all parameter estimates
(factor loadings and structural paths) were freely estimated across
groups. In the second (restricted model), each of the factor loadings
and structural paths were constrained to be invariant across groups. If
the chi-square for the restricted model was significantly larger than
the chi-square for the unrestricted model, the assumption of invariance
would not be tenable. Results indicated a non-significant difference
between these models for age groups: [DELTA][chi square](25, N = 983) =
35.44, ns. In the case of gender, a significant difference was found:
[DELTA][chi square](25, N = 983) 41.489, p < .001. Closer inspection
of cross-group constraints revealed that 5 out of 25 constraints would
significantly decrease [chi square] if released. After releasing these
constraints, the unconstrained and the constrained models for gender
groups were statistically equivalent--[Delta][chi square](25, N = 983) =
31.22, ns. Results supported, therefore, invariance of the general model
across gender and age groups.
DISCUSSION
The present study aimed to investigate the influence of violent
behavior and victimization at school on psychological distress
(perceived stress and depressive symptoms) among adolescents, examining
the role that parents and teachers play in the link between these
variables. As expected, violent behavior and victimization showed
differential paths in their influence on adolescents' psychological
adjustment.
Consistent with recent studies (Guterman et al., 2002; Kumpulainen
et al., 2001; Hawker & Boulton, 2000), we found peer victimization
to be directly and positively associated with levels of psychological
distress, showing that victimized students have higher levels of
depressive symptomatology and perceived stress in comparison with those
not victimized. Surprisingly, the results also showed victimization to
be associated with negative father-adolescent communication, which
seemed to mediate a part of the influence of victimization on distress.
In other words, our findings suggest that being a victimized student and
having conflictive communication with father are two factors
interrelated in the explanation of some psychological problems in
adolescence. This finding is in line with those recently reported by
Flouri and Buchanan (2002), who concluded that adolescents who had
experienced victimization problems at school and whose fathers were less
involved with them, appeared to be more at risk for poor mental health.
Regarding school-based violence, as hypothesized, violent behavior
showed a negative influence on relationships with parents and teachers,
which in turn affected levels of psychological distress. These findings
are consistent with those reporting that the quality of both
communication with parents and interactions with teachers are related to
adolescents' behavioral and psychological adjustment (Liu, 2003;
Meehan et al., 2003; Reddy et al., 2003; Stevens et al., 2002). But in
addition, our results contribute to the understanding of relationships
between these variables, since they suggest that adolescents who
participate in violent behavior at school, do not necessarily experience
higher levels of distress than those with no behavioral problems, unless
violent behavior worsens adolescent's interaction with parents and
teachers.
Though more investigation is needed to better clarify the role of
father, mother, and teacher on adolescent psychosocial adjustment,
results of the present study point out that relationships with parents
and teachers are potential mediators between behavioral problems at
school and psychological distress. Consequently, future interventions
focused on adolescent psychological adjustment should take into account,
not only the direct influence of violent behavior/victimization, but how
these problems in adolescence are affecting social interactions at home
and at school.
Finally, the following limitations are acknowledged. First, in
using a cross-sectional design, caution about drawing causal inferences
from the results should be maintained. Further clarification of the
relationships requires a longitudinal study. Second, in addition to
self-report measures from adolescents, inclusion of parental perceptions
of family communication and adolescents' behavioral problems would
shed greater light on the relationship between these variables. Finally,
we did not examine the mediating mechanisms between
violence-victimization at school and adolescents' interaction with
parents and teachers (e.g., attitude toward informal and formal
authority) or between adolescents' interaction with parents and
teachers and psychological distress (e.g., self-esteem, social support).
Future research should include some of these variables in order to
determine their contribution to psychosocial adjustment problems in
adolescence.
Table 1
Correlations Among Observed Variables
Variables 1 2 3
1. Mother--open communication --
2. Mother--offensive communication -.53 *** --
3. Mother--avoidant communication -.45 *** .30 *** --
4. Father--open communication .56 *** -.32 *** -.30 ***
5. Father--offensive communication -.32 *** .65 *** .19 ***
6. Father--avoidant communication -.27 *** .20 *** .68 ***
7. Teacher--relationship with the .06 -.11 ** -.06
teacher
8. Teacher--social integration at .08 -.15 *** -.02
school
9. Teacher--personal adjustment .14 ** -.20 *** -.05
10. Violent behavior at school -.23 *** .23 *** .14 ***
11. Victimization at school -.12 ** .12 *** .09 **
12. Perceived stress -.41 *** .42 *** .33 ***
13. Depressive symptomatology -.36 *** .41 *** .28 ***
Variables 4 5 6
1. Mother--open communication
2. Mother--offensive communication
3. Mother--avoidant communication
4. Father--open communication --
5. Father--offensive communication -.44 *** --
6. Father--avoidant communication -.32 *** .25 *** --
7. Teacher--relationship with the .05 -.10 ** -.05
teacher
8. Teacher--social integration at .09 -.15 *** -.04
school
9. Teacher--personal adjustment .12 -.17 *** -.02
10. Violent behavior at school -.18 *** .20 *** .12 ***
11. Victimization at school -.15 *** .14 *** .07 **
12. Perceived stress -.38 *** .35 *** .28 ***
13. Depressive symptomatology -.37 *** .36 *** .21 ***
Variables 7 8 9
1. Mother--open communication
2. Mother--offensive communication
3. Mother--avoidant communication
4. Father--open communication
5. Father--offensive communication
6. Father--avoidant communication
7. Teacher--relationship with the --
teacher
8. Teacher--social integration at .56 *** --
school
9. Teacher--personal adjustment .57 *** .63 *** --
10. Violent behavior at school -.20 ** -.13 *** -.27 ***
11. Victimization at school -.04 -.06 -.06
12. Perceived stress -.08 ** -.14 *** -.19 ***
13. Depressive symptomatology -.10 ** -.15 *** -.21 ***
Variables 10 11 12 13
1. Mother--open communication
2. Mother--offensive communication
3. Mother--avoidant communication
4. Father--open communication
5. Father--offensive communication
6. Father--avoidant communication
7. Teacher--relationship with the
teacher
8. Teacher--social integration at
school
9. Teacher--personal adjustment
10. Violent behavior at school --
11. Victimization at school .39 *** --
12. Perceived stress .22 *** .22 *** --
13. Depressive symptomatology .23 *** .24 *** .77 *** --
Note. Variables are standardized.
* p < .05, ** p < .01, *** p < .001
Table 2
Unstandardized Parameter Estimates, Standard Errors,
and Significance Levels
Variables Factor Loadings
Communication with Mother
Open communication 1.123 ***
(0.064)
Avoidant communication -0.722 ***
(0.049)
Offensive communication -1.00 (a)
Communication with Father
Open communication 1.115 ***
(0.079)
Avoidant communication 0.634 ***
(0.057)
Offensive communication -1.00 (a)
Teacher's Perception
Personal adjustment 1.087 ***
(0.052)
Social integration at school 0.932 ***
(0.047)
Relationship with the teacher 1.00 (a)
Violent Behavior at School 1.00 (a)
Peer Victimization 1.00 (a)
Psychological Distress
Depressive symptomatology 0.957 ***
(0.039)
Perceived stress 1.00 (a)
Note. Robust statistics. Standard errors are in parentheses.
(a) Fixed to 1.00 during estimation.
*** p < .001 (two-tailed test)
REFERENCES
Ary, D. V., Duncan, T. E., Biglan, A., Metzler, C. W., Noell, J.
W., & Smolkowski, K. (1999). Development of adolescent problem
behavior. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 27(2), 141-150.
Alasker, F. D., & Oweus, D. (1992). Parental relationships,
peer relationships, and the development of depressive tendencies in
adolescence. Paper presented at the biennial meeting of the Society for
Research on Adolescence, Washington, DC.
Angold, A., & Costello, E. J. (1993). Depressive comorbidity in
children and adolescents: Empirical, theoretical and methodological
issues. American Journal of Psychiatry, 150, 1779-1791.
Barnes, H. L., & Olson, D. H. (1982). Parent-adolescent
communication scale. In H. D. Olson (Ed.), Family inventories:
Inventories used in a national survey of families across the family life
cycle (pp. 33-48). St. Paul, MN: Family Social Science, University of
Minnesota.
Barrera, M. J., & Li, S. A. (1996). The relation of family
support to adolescents' psychological distress and behavior
problems. In G. R. Pierce & I. G. Sarason (Eds.), Handbook of social
support and the family (pp. 313-343). New York: Plenum Press.
Beam, M. R., Gil-Rivas, V., Greenberger, E., & Chen, C. (2002).
Adolescent problem behavior and depressed mood: Risk and protection
within and across social contexts. Journal of Youth and Adolescence,
31(5), 343-357.
Begotti, T., Borca, G., Calandri, E., Cateelino, E., &
Ingoglia, S. (2004). Family relationships and risk: An analysis of
circularity. IX Conference of the European Association for Research on
Adolescence. Porto.
Bentler, P. M. (1995). EQS structural equations program manual.
Encino, CA: Multivariate Software.
Birch, S. H., & Ladd, G. W. (1998). Children's
interpersonal behaviors and the teacher-child relationship.
Developmental Psychology, 34(5), 934-946.
Blankemeyer, M., Flannery, D. J., & Vazsonyi, A. T. (2002). The
role of aggression and social competence in children's perceptions
of the child-teacher relationship. Psychology in the Schools, 39(3),
293-304.
Buist, K. L., & Dekovic, M. (2004). Family positive and
negative affective quality and adolescent problem behavior. IX
Conference of the European Association for Research on Adolescence.
Porto.
Cohen, S., Kamarck, T., & Mermelstein, R. (1983). A global
measure of perceived stress. Journal of Health and Social Behavior, 24,
385-396.
Coie, J. D., & Koeppl, G. K. (1990). Adapting intervention to
the problems of aggressive and disruptive children. In S. R. Asher &
J. D. Coie (Eds.), Peer rejection in childhood (pp. 309-337). New York:
Cambridge University Press.
Crawford-Brown, C. (1999). The impact of parenting on conduct
disorder on Jamaican male adolescents. Adolescence, 34(134), 417-436.
Cummings, M. E., & DAvies, P. (1994). Children and marital
conflict: The impact of family dispute and resolution. New York:
Guilford Press.
Cummings, M. E., Goeke-Morey, M. C., & Papp, L. M. (2003).
Children's responses to everyday martial conflict tactics in the
home. Child Development, 74(6), 1918-1929.
Dadds, M. R., Sanders, M. R., Morrison, M., & Rebgetz, M.
(1992). Childhood depression and conduct disorder II. An analysis of
family interaction patterns in the home. Journal of Abnormal Child
Psychology, 101(3), 505-513.
Eisenberg, N., Fabes, R. A., Shepard, S. A., Guthrie, I. K.,
Murphy, B. C., & Reiser, M. (1999). Parental reactions to
children's negative emotions: Longitudinal relations to quality of
children's social functioning. Child Development, 70(2), 513-534.
Emler, N., & Reicher, S. (1995). Adolescence and delinquency.
Oxford: Blackwell.
Ferdinand, R. F., Stijnen, T., Verhulst, F. C., & van der
Reijden, M. (1999). Associations betweren behavioral and emotional
problems in adolescence and maladjustment in young adulthood. Journal of
Adolescence, 22, 123-136.
Flouri, E., & Buchanan, A. (2002). Life satisfaction in teeage
boys: The moderating role of father involvement and bullying. Aggressive
Behavior, 28, 126-133.
Formoso, D., Gonzales, N. A., & Aiken, L. S. (2000). Family
conflict and children's internalizing and externalizing behavior:
Protective factors. American Journal of Community Psychology, 28(2),
175-199.
Fry, P. S. (1983). Process measures of problem and non-problem
children's classroom behavior: The influence of teacher behavior
variables. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 53(1), 79-88.
Garber, J. (1996). The development of depression project. Peabody
Journal of Education, 71, 99-117.
Garnefski, N., & Diekstra, R. F. W. (1997). Comorbidity of
behavioral, emotional, and cognitive problems in adolescence. Journal of
Youth and Adolescence, 26, 321-338.
Ge, X., Best, K. M., Conger, R. M., & Simons, R. L. (1996).
Parenting behaviors and the occurrence and co-occurrence of adolescent
depressive symptoms and conduct problems. Developmental Psychology, 32,
717-731.
Gerard, J. M., & Buehler, C. (1999). Multiple risk factors in
the family environment and youth problem behaviors. Journal of Marriage
and the family, 61, 343-361.
Gil-Rivas, V., Greenberger, E., Chen, C., & Montero Lopez-Lena,
M. (2003). Understanding depressed mood in the context of a
family-oriented culture. Adolescence, 38(149), 93-109.
Guterman, N. B., Hahm, H. C., & Cameron, M. (2002). Adolescent
victimization and subsequent use of mental health counselling services.
Journal of Adolescent Health, 30, 336-345.
Hawker, D. S. J., & Boulton, M. J. (2000). Twenty years'
research on peer victimization and psychosocial maladjustment: A
meta-analytic review of cross-sectional studies. Journal of Child
Psychology and Psychiatry, 41, 441-455.
Hawley, P., & Vaughn, B. (2003). Aggression and adaptation: The
bright side to bad behavior. Introduction to special volume.
Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 49, 239-244.
Jack, S. L., Shores, R. E., Denny, R. K., Gunter, P. L., DeBriere,
T., & DePaepe, P. (1996). An analysis of the relationships of
teachers' reported use of classroom management strategies on types
of classroom interactions. The Journal of Behavioral Education, 6,
67-87.
Juvonen, J., Niahina, A., & Graham, S. (2000). Peer harassment,
psychological adjustment, and school functioning in early adolescence.
Journal of Educational Psychology, 92(2), 349-359.
Kenny, M. E., Gallagher, L. A., Alvarez-Salvat, R., & Silsby,
J. (2002). Sources of support and psychological distress among
academically successful inner-city youth. Adolescence, 37(145), 161-182.
Kumpulainen, K., Rasanen, E., & Puura, K. (2001). Psychiatric
disorders and the use of mental health services among children involved
in bullying. Aggressive Behavior, 27, 102-110.
Kupersmidt, J. B., Coie, J. D., & Dodge, K. A. (1990).
Predicting disorder from peer social problems. In S. R. Asher & J.
D. Coie (Eds.), Peer rejection in childhood. New York: Cambridge
University Press.
Little, T., Brauner, J., Jones, S., Nock, M., & Hawley, P.
(2003). Rethinking aggression: A typological examination of the
functions of aggression. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 49, 343-369.
Liu, Y. (2003). Parent-child interaction and children's
depression: The relationship betweren parent-child interaction and
children's depressive symptoms in Taiwan. Journal of Adolescence,
26(4), 447-457.
Loeber, R., Drinkwater, M., Yin, Y., Anderson, S. J., Schmidt, L.
C., & Crawford, A. (2000). Stability of family interaction from ages
6 to 18. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 28(4), 353-369.
Meehan, B. T., Hughes, J. N., & Cavell, T. A. (2003).
Teacher-student relationships as compensatory resources for aggressive
children. Child development, 74(4), 1145-1157.
Olsson, G. I., Nordstrom, M. L., Arinell, H., & von Knorring,
A. L. (1999). Adolescent depression: Social network and family
climate--A case-control study. Journal of Child Psychology and
Psychiatry, 40(2), 227-237.
Overbeek, G., Biesecker, G., Stattin, H., Engels, R., & Meeus,
W. (2002). Co-occurrence of depressive moods and delinquency in early
adolescence: Longitudinal associations with parent, teacher, and peer
relationships. VIII Conference of the European Association for Research
on Adolescence. Oxford.
Radloff, L. S. (1977). The CES-D scale: A self-report depression
scale for research in the general population. Applied Psychological
Measurement, 1, 385-401.
Reddy, R., Rhodes, J. E., & Mulhall, P. (2003). The influence
of teacher support on student adjustment in the middle school years: A
latent growth curve study. Development and Psychopathology, 15, 119-138.
Sheeber, L., Hops, H., Alpert, A., Davis, B., & Andrews, J.
(1997). Family support and conflict: Prospective relations to adolescent
depression. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 25, 333-345.
Smith, P. K., Bowers, L., Binney, V., & Cowie, H. (1993).
Relationships of children involved in bully/victim problems at school.
In S. Duck (Ed.), Learning about relationships, understanding
relationship processes. (pp. 183-212). London: Sage Publications.
Stevens, V., De Bourdeaudhuij, I., & Van Oost, P. (2002).
Relationship of the family environment to children's involvement in
bully/victim problems at school. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 31,
419-428.
This research was supported by grant SEJ2004-01742 from the
Ministry of Education and Science of Spain.
Gonzalo Musitu, Universidad de Valencia, Spain.
Juan Herrero, Universidad de Oviedo, Spain.
Reprint requests to Estefania Estevez, Departamento de Psicologia
Social, Universidad de Valencia, Av. Blasco Ibanez 21, 46010 Valencia,
Spain.