Relational factors of vulnerability and protection for adolescent pregnancy: a cross-sectional comparative study of Portuguese pregnant and nonpregnant adolescents of low socioeconomic status.
Pereira, Ana I.F. ; Canavarro, Maria C. ; Cardoso, Margarida F. 等
Adolescent pregnancy has been the target of various studies because
it elicits innumerable questions. In developed societies it is
considered a serious social problem that is associated with low
schooling, precarious employment, and poverty. Reproductive health remains a priority issue for European Union (EU) member states even at a
time when pregnancy-related mortality rates are at historically low
levels. Although the mean age of mothers at the time they give birth, an
important determinant of maternity outcome, is increasing throughout the
EU, there is a worrisome persistent increase or stability in teenage
pregnancy (Ferrinho, Bugalho, & Pereira, 2004).
Portugal has seen a sustained downward trend in teenage pregnancies
since the mid-eighties when specific health service measures were
introduced for teenagers and improvement in family planning reduced the
number of unwanted pregnancies (Mendonca & Calado, 2002).
Nevertheless, in 2003 approximately 6,144 Portuguese infants were born
to adolescent mothers and 5.5% of all births were from mothers under age
20 (INE, 2003). Portugal has the second highest teenage birth rate in
the former 15 European Union member states (UNICEF, 2001).
In the era of information and availability of contraceptive
methods, why do adolescents get pregnant? Consistent with pluralist and
multi-factorial development theory and models, adolescent pregnancy is
seen as a multidetermined phenomenon with different developmental
outcomes. Similar to what is postulated in the principles of
equifinality and multifinality of general system theory (von
Bertalanffy, 1968), different courses can lead to pregnancy during
adolescence and different trajectories are drawn from there. Therefore,
in lieu of looking for the determinants of adolescent pregnancy, it is
preferable to study the different risk and protective factors involved.
Several authors (Corcoran, 1999; Corcoran, Franklin, & Bennett,
2000; Pistole, 1999; Scaramella, Conger, Simons, & Whitbeck, 1998)
have claimed the importance of a relational approach to studying risk
for adolescent pregnancy. The present study assumes both a relational
and ecological perspective. Bronfenbrenner (1979, 1987) believes that
comprehension of human development requires "the analysis of
multi-personal interractional systems, that are not restricted to only
one environment" (p. 40). Although an ecological perspective
considers the influence of contexts in which children do not
participate, this perspective also predicts that the relationships that
form a direct part of the child's and adolescent's life are
those that have a most profound influence (Cauce, Reid, Landesman, &
Gonzales, 1990). A series of studies identified three main "support
systems" in adolescence: family, friends, and school (Cauce, Mason,
Hiraga, & Liu, 1994). The following is a brief review of the
relational factors of familial and extra-familial contexts that are
antecedents to and associated with adolescent pregnancy.
Family Factors
Concerning the familial context, most studies focus on
sociodemographic characteristics and family structure. These studies
(Coley & Chase-Lansdale, 1998; Hardy & Zabin, 1991; Marlove,
1997; Miller, 2002; Woodward, Fergusson & Horwood, 2001;
Yampolskaya, Brown, & Greenbaum, 2002) support the view that there
is a large incidence of pregnancy in adolescents raised by single
parents, by parents with low educational attainment and low income, and
in adolescents from larger families. The existence of adolescent
pregnancy/ maternity models in the family is another variable that seems
to have influence. The pregnancy in this life cycle period occurs more
frequently in daughters of mothers with an adolescent pregnancy history
(Manlove, 1997; Woodward et al., 2001) and in sisters of adolescent
mothers (Records, 1993). This suggests the existence of
intergenerational patterns in teenage motherhood.
With regard to psychological and relational variables of the family
context, investigation results are not consistent. Some studies suggest
as predictors of early pregnancy a family environment characterized by
stress, depression, and conflict (Ravert & Martin, 1997; Woodward,
et al., 2001), more dysfunctionality and rigidity (Garrett &
Tidwell, 1999), physical, sexual, and emotional abuse (Garrett &
Tidwell, 1999; Kellogg, Hoffman, & Taylor, 1999; Woodward et al.,
2001), and a lower relationship quality between parents and daughters
(Landy, Schubert, Cleland, Clark, & Montgomery, 1983). Nevertheless,
other studies (Townsend & Worobey, 1987) revealed the absence of
significant differences in family relations when pregnant adolescents
are compared with nonpregnant adolescents. Limitations inherent to some
of these studies are the cross-sectional design, small selected samples
of pregnant/mother adolescents, sample collection methods, and the
utilization of instruments with nonevaluated psychometric properties.
Few studies examined the mediating mechanisms that explain how
family variables influence adolescent sexual behavior and pregnancy. In
an exceptional study using a prospective longitudinal design, Scaramella
et al. (1998) followed a cohort of rural adolescents from 7th to 12th
grade. Results of this study show that relationships with parents
characterized by warmth and involvement during early adolescence are
significantly and negatively associated with risk behavior during middle
adolescence. This kind of family environment is also negatively
correlated with deviant-peer affiliations and positively correlated with
academic competence. The effect of parental warmth and involvement
during early adolescence in pregnancy status during middle and late
adolescence was examined by structural equation analysis controlling for
parents' education. Results show that the effects of parenting were
indirect and mediated by academic competence, deviant-peer affiliations,
and risk-taking behavior. In summary, this study suggests that family
relationships indirectly influence the risk for adolescent pregnancy,
affecting risk and protective factors within other contexts: peers and
schools.
Relations Outside the Family
Relationship with peers is also associated with teenage pregnancy.
Some studies (Scaramella et al., 1998; Woodward et al., 2001) found an
association between affiliation with deviant peers and teenage pregnancy
risk. Also, larger interpersonal difficulties, a restricted number of
friends, and a lower quality of friendships characterize the pregnant
adolescent's peer relations. The pregnant/mother adolescents,
compared to nonpregnant adolescents have fewer friends (Alvarez,
Burrows, Zvaighat, & Muzzo, 1987; Passino et al., 1993), more often
consider their peer relations as less important than family links, are
more submissive, and exhibit poorer interpersonal communication abilities in their relations with other adolescents (Hardy & Zabin,
1991). These findings suggest overall unsatisfactory peer relations.
However, other studies (Connelly, 1998; Corcoran et al., 2000; Orshan,
1999) that compare social support perceived by pregnant and nonpregnant
teenagers report no differences, which lead us to conjecture that girls
who become pregnant may compensate for these unsatisfactory relations
with support received from other persons (e.g., boyfriend).
Little is known about the romantic relations of the pregnant teen.
Most studies gather information only from the mothers, and studies that
include the adolescent's partner consider only the sociodemographic
characteristics of the child's father and the objective data of the
relationships (e.g., longevity). Some studies (Bull & Hogue, 1998;
Coley & Chase-Landsdale, 1998; Males, 1993) show that the partner
tends to be 2 to 4 years older and have low educational achievement.
Dating at an earlier age is a factor that distinguishes pregnant
teenagers from adolescents without a history of early pregnancy
(Furstenberg, 1976). Romantic relationships of pregnant teens are
relatively steady, although they have a lower mean length of
relationship with the partner prior to pregnancy (compared with the
relationship preceding marriage for married women without a history of
early pregnancy) (Schamess, 1993). Also, the pregnancy tends to occur
most often within an exclusive relationship (De Anda, 1983). However,
emotional motivations in romantic relationships research tend to be
disregarded in studies of teen pregnancy (Pistole, 1999).
School Context
Pregnant adolescents, in comparison to nonpregnant adolescents,
have lower levels of education, lower educational aspirations and
performance, and higher levels of school dropout (Alvarez et al., 1987;
Coley & Chase-Lansdale, 1998; Garrett & Tidwell, 1999; Harris,
1998; Ireson, 1984; Manlove, 1998; Olson & Worobey, 1984; Townsend
& Worobey, 1987; Yampolskaya et al., 2002). Nevertheless, this does
not mean that those who get pregnant during adolescence have lower
cognitive capabilities compared to their nonpregnant peers. Most often,
these adolescents attend schools that have fewer resources along with
students with lower capabilities, provide less stimulation, and subject
them to negative cataloguing (Manlove, 1998).
METHOD
Subjects
Two groups of young women: pregnant (n = 57) and without a
pregnancy history (n = 81), aged 14 through 18 years, comprised the
convenience sample population. Both groups were from a low socioeconomic
status. Data from the pregnant adolescents were collected in three
national health services from different geographic areas. For those
without a history of pregnancy, data were collected in public schools. A
balance between the two groups with regard to socioeconomic status and
geographic area guided school selection. All those in the pregnancy
group were primiparous: 18% in the first trimester, 35% in the second
trimester, and 47% in the third trimester. The majority (85%) of these
pregnancies were unplanned.
Procedure
The pregnant adolescents were contacted while they were waiting for
an obstetrics consultation in a public service facility. There the study
objectives were explained, confidentiality guaranteed, and oral consent
obtained. The adolescents were interviewed individually and then
completed the study instruments that are described in the next section.
Before the instruments were administered to the nonpregnant
adolescents during class time, confidentiality was guaranteed and the
voluntary nature of participation was made clear. All adolescents
approached in both groups agreed to participate. All adolescents who
were not from a low socioeconomic status were excluded. With regard to
the nonpregnant adolescent group, those younger than 14 and those who
had a previous pregnancy history, were excluded. Adolescents in the
pregnant group who had a previous pregnancy were also excluded.
Measures
Sociodemographic, Personal, and Familial Antecedents Questionnaire.
This questionnaire was composed of 61 questions for both groups and 16
additional questions for the pregnant adolescents. The questions were
organized in five different thematic parts: personal data
(sociodemographic and antecedents); family data (sociodemographic and
antecedents); school antecedents and professional and school future;
sexual life and contraceptive methods; life events and preoccupations.
Three additional topics were addressed to the pregnant adolescents:
pregnancy, child's father's sociodemographic data and
relationship with the child's father, and future projects for the
adolescent and her baby.
Perceived Parental Child Rearing (EMBU). Evaluation of the parental
child rearing was accomplished through administration of the Portuguese
shortened version of EMBU (Perils, Jacobson, Lindstorm, von Knorring,
& Perils, 1980), developed by Canavarro (1996). This instrument was
designed to assess memories of both maternal and parental rearing
behavior, measured separately on three dimensions: Rejection, Emotional
Warmth, and Overprotection. The items are scored on a four-point Likert
scale for each parent.
Following a personal contact with M. Eisemann (November, 1999), a
few changes to the Portuguese version (Canavarro, 1996, 1999) were made
to ensure its appropriateness for the adolescent sample of the present
study.
EMBU is one of the most widely used retrospective measures of
perceived parental child rearing during childhood and adolescence. This
instrument was selected among others that evaluate perceived parental
rearing based on its psychometric qualities which were supported by
various studies in different countries (Arrindell, Perris, Eisemann, van
der Ende, Gasgner, Iawawaki, Maj, & Zhang, 1994; Arrindel & van
der Ende, 1984; Gerlsma, Emmelkamp, & Arrindel, 1990; Ross,
Campbell, & Clayer, 1982; Ruiter, 1994; Vandereycken, 1994;
Winefield, Tiggermann, & Winefield, 1994). The Portuguese
psychometric studies (Canavarro, 1996) also revealed that this version
has good validity and reliability.
Convoy model. Social support was measured by administering a part
of the Portuguese version (Araujo, 1997) of the measure developed by
Kahn and Antonucci (1980). The Convoy model evaluates the
respondent's social networks through a spatial representation
technique. Respondents are presented with a set of three concentric
circles with the word "you" in the center. Respondents are
asked to name the "people who, in your life, are closer, in
function of the support (affective, familial, professional ...) received
from and given to them," in each of three levels as depicted by the
three concentric circles. They are asked to place in the inner circle
those persons who are "so close and important" to them that
they "can't imagine life without them." Those considered
less close but still important are placed in the middle and outer
circles. These persons are identified according to relationship (e.g.,
father, mother, friend, boyfriend, neighbor).
Different support providers are given a score indicating the
importance of their position in the three circles, ranging from 0 to 3.
The persons placed in the first circle, the circle of the highest
emotional proximity, are given three points; those placed in the second
circle are given two points; and those placed in the third circle are
given one point. A score of 0 indicates that the type of relation is
nonexistent or if it did exist the girl did not include it in her social
support network.
The respondents then have to evaluate the satisfaction with the
support received from others and the satisfaction with the support given
to others. The evaluation is done on a five-point Likert scale, from
"Not satisfied" to "Very much satisfied." This
measure was chosen for the evaluation possibilities it offers; namely,
the simultaneous evaluation of the structural aspects and evaluation of
the adequacy of the identified social network. Furthermore, it has the
advantage of being a less restrictive procedure in the social network
identification because respondents are allowed to identify as many
people as they wish (Antonucci, 1985).
Statistical Methodology
In order to investigate possible predictors of adolescent
pregnancy, the first stage of the analysis was to establish which of the
factors were significantly associated with the pregnancy. Comparisons
were made between the pregnant and nonpregnant adolescent groups using
chi-square tests for association of categorical variables (such as area
of residence or emotional proximity to peers) and independent t-tests
for continuous variables (such as emotional support or overprotection
from father/mother). The second stage of this analysis was to determine
which of the antecedent factors to pregnancy were significantly and
independently associated with adolescent pregnancy. A multifactorial
analysis using logistic regression was employed. Results are reported as
adjusted odds ratio of adolescent pregnancy and the corresponding 95%
confidence interval. The effect of each factor was obtained controlling
for all the others in the model, and nonsignificant factors (p >
0.05) were dropped from the final model, with the exception of age and
area of residence. Interactions between variables were also
investigated.
RESULTS
Family Factors of Pregnant and Nonpregnant Adolescents
As expected, some associations between family factors and
adolescent pregnancy were found. All family factors assessed through the
Socio-Demographic and Familial Antecedents Questionnaire were
significantly different for the two groups (Table 1). Pregnant
adolescents were more likely to have more siblings, to belong to a
non-intact family, to have parents with a history of previous
psychological/psychiatric intervention, and to have mothers with a
history of adolescent pregnancy. Concerning Perceived Parental Child
Rearing assessed through EMBU, significant differences between the
groups were also found (Table 2). Pregnant young girls perceived less
emotional support and less overprotection from both parents. There were
no statistical significant differences concerning the rejection
dimension.
Relations with Peers and Partners and Social Support in Pregnant
and Nonpregnant Adolescents
A considerable percentage of adolescents from the pregnant group
reported having no friends (31.6%), while only one adolescent of the
nonpregnant group did not include any friend in her social network.
Also, teenagers from the pregnant group reported significantly lower
levels of emotional proximity to peers (Table 3).
In regard to the romantic partner, there are also significant
differences. While the great majority (89.3%) of pregnant teens include
romantic partners in their social network, only a minority (38.3%) of
the nonpregnant adolescents include the boyfriend. Further, pregnant
adolescents report significantly higher levels of emotional proximity to
romantic partners (Table 3). It is important to note that for the
majority of teens, the child's father is the first sexual partner,
and that their relationship has considerable longevity (M = 1.66 years,
SD = 1.06). Finally, no significant differences were found concerning
the global evaluation of perceived and received support.
School Context in Pregnant and Nonpregnant Adolescents
In the pregnant adolescent group, only a minority attend school
(12.3%), and for the majority of them, dropout occurred before the
pregnancy (52.6%). All the girls in the nonpregnant sample attend school
(a consequence of the sample procedures). However, a Portuguese study
(Lourenco, 1996) that collected the sample of both pregnant and
nonpregnant adolescents in health centers found similar differences
between the two groups concerning school dropout.
Also, there is a significant difference (t = 6.61, p < 0.001)
between the two groups concerning grade retention. The pregnant
adolescents have a higher number of retentions in grade (M = 1.54, SD =
1.21) than the nonpregnant adolescents (M = 0.38, SD = 0.64). Again,
these results should be interpreted with caution. The sample of
nonpregnant adolescents was collected in schools and does not include
nonpregnant adolescents who dropped out of school. This fact can lead to
a greater difference in the number of retentions between pregnant and
nonpregnant adolescents than is found in the general population.
Logistic Regression Considering Family, School and Peers Factors
The comparison of pregnant and nonpregnant adolescents was also
made through a logistic regression method. Results are shown in Table 4.
Variables were selected among those factors found to be significant at
the 5% level in the bivariate analyses, with the exception of emotional
proximity to boyfriend. Although this variable can contribute to an
understanding of the factors that promote teenage pregnancy, we chose
not to include it in the analysis for two reasons: this variable is more
sensitive to changes associated with the occurrence of pregnancy, and
only 38% of the nonpregnant adolescents had a boyfriend. Among the
possible predictor variables included in the multifactorial model, five
factors remain significantly and independently associated with
adolescent pregnancy after adjusting for the effects of age, area of
residence, and the remainder in the model. This main effects model fit
the data adequately and no interactions were deemed necessary.
After adjusting for the effects of the other variables in the
model, lower overprotection from mother and lower emotional support from
father were positively associated with teenage pregnancy. Also,
pregnancy was more common in girls whose mother had a history of
adolescent pregnancy. Concerning school factors, for each year of school
failure, the risk of pregnancy increased significantly. Finally, lower
emotional proximity to peers was also positively associated with
pregnancy. In sum, the most effective predictors of adolescent pregnancy
were family, school, and peer level factors.
DISCUSSION
The results of the present study confirm the importance of the
simultaneous study of different relational contexts for the
understanding of adolescent pregnancy. Factors belonging to different
levels of analysis--family, school, peer relations--make independent
contributions toward characterization of the two adolescents groups,
pregnant and without a pregnancy history.
In spite of the transverse nature of the study, we can infer how
the various factors studied, assumed as antecedent variables from a
theoretical point of view, can create vulnerabilities for the occurrence
of pregnancy in this developmental period. The following describes some
hypothetical mechanisms that can explain how these variables could lead
the adolescents to desire the pregnancy or, in the case of nonplanned
pregnancy, the factors that caused the adolescent to choose the
maternity when she became aware that she was pregnant.
Concerning the adolescent's family, the results showed that
pregnant adolescents perceive less emotional warmth from father and less
overprotection from mother in comparison with the nonpregnant
adolescents. The importance of parental child rearing in understanding
early pregnancy leads us to search for the different mechanisms that
explain how these variables act.
A young woman who perceives less emotional support from her parents
may seek this support in romantic relationships (Pereira, 2001; Pereira,
Canavarro, Mendonca, & Cardoso, 2002). Some of the data collected
during the interviews reveal that many of the adolescents who planned
the pregnancy did it to please the boyfriend, who was older and wanted
to have a child. Others did it because they wanted to begin a new life
with their partners.
Less supervision/overprotection from parents is associated with
earlier sexual activity and more risk behaviors, including nonprotected
sex (Donoghue, 1992-93; Peterson, Sripada, & Barglow, 1982;
Scaramella et al., 1998; Tobas & Ricer, 1998). Frequently these
adolescents grow up on their own, under the influence of street culture
and without well-defined limits to their behavior. Concerning
adolescents who live in poor communities, with little resources and few
adult role models, parents' supervision emerges as an important
factor of protection from deviant and risky behaviors (Pereira, 2001;
Pereira et al., 2002). Poor supervision and involvement by parents also
affect the school context and are associated with lack of academic
competence and school dropout (Scaramella et al., 1998). In sum, greater
emotional support and adequate supervision seem to be protective factors
for adolescents of low socioeconomic status. Miller (2002), in a recent
literature review, points two family relations variables--parent/child
connectedness, closeness and warmth, and parental regulation,
monitoring, and supervision, as important predictors of adolescent
sexual behavior and pregnancy. Warm and supportive relations with
parents promote sensitive environments attuned to adolescents'
needs and provide a general sense of competence and responsibility
(Scarmella et al., 1998). This environment also has a positive effect on
adolescents in other contexts; e.g., adherence to school demands and
avoidance of deviant peers.
Consistent with other empirical studies (Manlove, 1997; Woodward et
al., 2001) another factor that differentiated adolescent pregnant and
nonpregnant groups was the existence of a history of adolescent
pregnancy in the adolescent's mother. The mother's early
pregnancy is associated with other risk factors for being pregnant
during adolescence, among them, single parenthood and larger families.
Further, the model provided by the mother can lead to more
permissiveness in the adolescent regarding early sexual activity and
easy acceptance of adolescent pregnancy.
School is equally a reference context for understanding early
pregnancy (Alvarez et al., 1987; Coley & Chase-Lansdale, 1998;
Garrett & Tidwell, 1999; Harris, 1998; Ireson, 1984; Manlove, 1998;
Olson & Worobey, 1984; Townsend & Worobey, 1987; Yampolskaya et
al., 2002). School failure and low success expectancy in association
with difficult economic conditions of the families of our sample, can
lead the adolescents to drop out and early entrance into the work world.
Maternity is one more adult role the adolescent is forced to play
(Pereira, 2001). From a different perspective, the teenager with a
history of school failure may see maternity as entailing less costs in
terms of future projects, leading to easier acceptance of pregnancy
(Manlove, 1998). School dropout is also associated with the loss of a
potential source of social support and friendship.
In line with findings of other studies (Alvarez et al., 1987;
Passino et al., 1993) young pregnant women perceive less emotional
proximity to peers and some do not have friends in their social support
network. This can be another factor in producing vulnerability for early
pregnancy. Isolation from peers can lead the adolescent to seek the
support and care of others through becoming pregnant--a guarantee of a
"more secure" relation with the boyfriend, or even that her
child can fulfil the need for companionship (Canavarro & Pereira,
2001). This kind of compensating mechanism seems to be supported by the
results concerning emotional proximity to the partner and social
support. A higher emotional proximity to the romantic partner, and a
steady relationship may lead to greater social support. Thus, it seems
that a multicontextual approach to the study of early pregnancy
contributes to a better understanding of the process.
Although the present study contributes to some additional
understanding of early pregnancy, there are some study limitations. The
cross-sectional design and retrospective assessment of the variables
make it difficult to ascribe causality to relationships between
pregnancy and some variables. Another limitation concerns the difference
between the contexts of data collection for the two groups. The pregnant
group was recruited in health centers, while the nonpregnant group was
recruited in schools. Given the fact that approximately half the sample
of pregnant adolescents was not attending school, the differences found
between the two groups may simultaneously reflect school dropout and
pregnancy factors.
Nevertheless important contributions can be drawn from this study.
The low socioeconomic adolescent sample is one of its main strengths
since some previous studies did not take this into account. The focus
here on adolescents from low socioeconomic status provided some
understanding of the risk and protective factors for adolescent
pregnancy in this high-risk group. Finally, the use of psychometrically
sound instruments and the simultaneous consideration of different
relational contexts through multivariate procedures are other assets of
the present work.
REFERENCES
Alvarez, M. L., Burrows, R., Zvaighat, A., & Muzzo, S. (1987).
Sociocultural characteristics of pregnant and nonpregnant adolescents of
low socioeconomic status: A comparative study. Adolescence, 22, 149-156.
Antonucci, T. (1985). Personal characteristics, social support, and
social behavior. In R. H. Binstock & E. Shanas (Eds.), Handbook of
aging and social sciences (2nd ed., pp. 94-128). New York: Van Nostrand
Reinhold.
Arrindell, W. A., Perris, C., Eisemann, M., van der Ende, J. F.,
Gasgner, P., Iawawaki, S., Maj, S., & Zhang, J. (1994). Parental
rearing behavior from a cross-cultural perspective: A summary of data
obtained in 14 nations. In C. Perris, W. Arrindell, & M. Eisemann
(Eds.), Parenting and Psychopathology (pp. 145-171). New York: John
Wiley & Sons.
Arrindell, W. A., & van der Ende, J. (1984). Replicability and
invariance of dimensions of parental rearing behavior: Further Dutch
experiences with the EMBU. Person. Individ. Diff., 5, 671-682.
Bronfenbrenner, U. (1987). La ecologia del desarrollo humano. [The
ecology of human development]. Barcelona: Ediciones Paidos Iberica.
(Original work published in 1979.)
Bull, S., & Hogue, C. J. R. (1998). Exploratory analysis of
factors associated with teens' repeated childbearing. Journal of
Health Care for the Poor and Undeserved, 9(1), 42-61.
Canavarro, M. C. (1996). Avaliacao das praticas educativas
parentais atravees do EMBU: Estudos psicometricos [Evaluation of
parental rearing behavior with the EMBU: Psychometric studies].
Psychologica, 16, 5-18.
Canavarro, M. C. (1999). Relacoes afectivas e saude mental. Urea
abordagem ao longo da vida [Affective relations and mental health].
Coimbra: Quarteto Editora.
Canavarro, M. C., & Pereira, A. I. (2001). Gravidez e
maternidade na adolescencia: Perspectivas teoricas [Pregnancy and
maternity in adolescence: Theoretical perspectives]. Coimbra: Quarteto
Editora.
Cauce, A. M., Mason, C., Hiraga, Y., & Liu, G. (1994). Social
support during adolescence: Methodological and theoretical
considerations. In F. Nestmann & K. Hurrelmann (Eds.), Social
networks and social support in childhood and adolescence (pp. 89-108).
Berlin: Walter de Gruyter.
Cauce, A. M., Reid, M., Landesman, S., & Gonzales, N. (1990).
Social support in young children: Measurement, structure and behavioral
impact. In B. R. Sarason, I. G. Sarason, & G. R. Pierce (Eds.),
Social support: An interactional view (pp. 64-95). New York: Wiley.
Coley, R. L., & Chase-Lansdale, P. L. (1998). Adolescent
pregnancy and parenthood: Recent evidence and future directions.
American Psychologist, 53, 152-166.
Connelly, C. D. (1998). Hopefulness, self-esteem, and perceived
social support among pregnant and nonpregnant adolescents. Western
Journal of Nursing Research, 20, 195-209.
Corcoran, J. (1999). Ecological factors associated with adolescent
pregnancy: A review of the literature. Adolescence, 34(135), 603-620.
Corcoran, J., Franklin, C., & Bennett, P. (2000). Ecological
factors associated with adolescent pregnancy and parenting. Social Work
Research, 24(1), 29-40.
De Anda, D. (1983). Pregnancy in early and late adolescence.
Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 12(1), 33-42.
Donoghue, E. (1992-93). Sociopsychological correlates of teen-age
pregnancy in the United States Virgin Islands. International Journal of
Mental Health, 21(4), 39-49.
Ferrinho, P., Bugalho, A., & Pereira M. J. (Eds.). (2004). For
better health in Europe. Report with the support of the European
Commission. Instituto de Medicina Preventiva da Faculdade de Medicina,
Universidade Lisboa, Portugal.
Furstenberg, F. F., Jr. (1976). Unplanned parenthood: The social
consequences of teenage childbearing. New York: Free Press.
Garrett, S. C., & Tidwell, R. (1999). Differences between
adolescent mothers and nonmothers: An interview study. Adolescence, 34,
91-105.
Gerlsma, C., Emmelkamp, P. M. G., & Arrindell, W. A. (1990).
Anxiety, depression, and perception of early parenting: A meta-analysis.
Clinical Psychological Review, 10, 251-277.
Hardy, J. B., & Zabin, L. S. (1991). Adolescent pregnancy in an
urban environment: Issues, programs, and evaluation. Baltimore: Urban
& Schwarzenber.
Harris, J. L. J. (1998). Urban African American adolescent parents:
Their perceptions of sex, love, intimacy, pregnancy, and parenting.
Adolescence, 33, 833-844.
Instituto Nacional de Estatistica (INE, 2003). Estatisticas
Demograficas [Demografic Statistics]. Lisboa: INE.
Ireson, C. J. (1984). Adolescent pregnancy and sex roles. Sex
Roles, 11, 189-201.
Kahn, R. L., & Antonucci, T. (1980). Convoys over the life
course: Attachment, roles and social support. In P. B. Baltes & O.
B. Brim (Eds.), Life span development and behavior: Vol. 3 (pp.
253-368). New York: Academic Press.
Kellogg, N. D., Hoffman, T. J., & Taylor, E. R. (1999). Early
sexual experiences among pregnant and parenting adolescents.
Adolescence, 34, 293-303.
Landy, S., Schubert, J., Cleland, J. F., Clark, C., &
Montgomery, J. S. (1983). Teenage pregnancy: Family syndrome?
Adolescence, 18, 781-794.
Lourenco, M. M. (1996). Textos e contextos da gravidez na
adolescencia. A adolescente, a familia e a escola [Texts and contexts of
adolescent pregnancy: Adolescent, family and school]. Coimbra: Faculdade
de Psicologia e de Ciencias da Educacao da Universidade de Coimbra.
Males, M. (1993). School-age pregnancy: Why hasn't prevention
worked? Journal of School Health, 63(10), 429432.
Manlove, J. (1997). Early motherhood in an intergenerational
perspective: The experiences of a British cohort. Journal of Marriage
and the Family, 59, 263-279.
Manlove, J. (1998). The influence of high school dropout and school
disengagement on the risk of school age pregnancy. Journal of Research
on Adolescence, 8, 187-220.
Mendonca, D., & Calado, B. P. (2002). Situacao da Satide
Reprodutiva em Portugal. In I. Craveiro, & P. Ferrinho, Saude
reprodutiva nos paises e territorios de lingua Portuguesa ]Reproductive
health in countries and territories of Portuguese language] (pp.
153-170). Lisboa: Associacao para o Desenvolvimento e Cooperacao Garcia
da horta.
Miller, B. C. (2002). Family influences on adolescent sexual and
contraceptive behavior. Journal of Sex Research, 39(1), 22-27.
Olson, C., & Worobey, J. (1984). Perceived mother-daughter
relations in a pregnant and nonpregnant adolescent sample. Adolescence,
19, 679-694.
Orshan, S. A. (1999). Acculturation, perceived social support and
self-esteem and pregnancy status among Dominican adolescents. Health
Care for Women International, 20, 245-238.
Passino, A. W., Whitman, T. L., Borkowski, J. G., Schellenbach, C.
J., Maxwell, S. E., Keogh, D., & Rellilnger, E. (1993). Personal
adjustment during pregnancy and adolescent parenting. Adolescence, 28,
97-122.
Peterson, C., Sripada, B., & Barglow, P. (1982). Psychiatric
aspects of adolescent pregnancy. Psychosomatics, 23, 723-733.
Pereira, A. I. F., Canavarro, M. C., Mendonqa, D. V., &
Cardoso, M. F. (2002). O mundo interpessoal da gravida adolesclente [The
interpersonal world of the pregnant adolescent]. Psicologia, Teoria,
Investigacao e Pratica, 7(2), 19-36.
Pereira, A. I. (2001). Contexctos relacionais de vulnerabilidade e
protect(to para a gravidez ns adolescencia [Relational contexts of
vulnerability and protection for adolescent pregnancy]. Unpublished
master's thesis. University of Coimbra, Coimbra, Portugal.
Perris, C., Jacobsson, L., Lindstrom, H., yon Knorring, L., &
Perris, H. (1980). Development of a new inventory assessing memories of
parental rearing behavior. Acta Psychiatr. Scand, 61, 265-274.
Pistole, M. C. (1999). Preventing teenage pregnancy: Contributions
from attachment theory. Journal of Mental Health Counseling, 21(2),
93-113.
Ravert, A. A., & Martin, J. (1997). Family stress, perception
of pregnancy, and age of first menarche among pregnant adolescents.
Adolescence, 32, 261-269.
Records, A. A.., & Martin, J. (1997). Family stress, perception
of pregnancy, and age of first menarche among pregnant adolescents.
Adolescence, 32, 261-269.
Records, K. A. (1993). Life events of pregnant and nonpregnant
adolescents. Adolescence, 28(110), 325-338.
Ross, M. W., Campbell, R. L., & Clayer, J. R. (1982).
Adolescent attachment and psychopathology. Journal of Consulting and
Clinical Psychology, 64, 244-253.
Ruiter, C. (1994). Anxious attachment in agoraphobia and
obsessive-compulsive disorder: A literature review and treatment
implications. In C. Perris, W. A. Arrindell, & M. Eisemann (Eds.),
Parenting and psychopathology (pp. 281-307). New York: John Wiley and
Sons.
Scaramella, L. V., Conger, R. D., Simons, R. L., & Whitbeck, L.
B. (1998). Predicting risk for pregnancy by late adolescence: A social
contextual perspective. Developmental Psychology, 1233-1245.
Schamess, S. (1993). The search for love: Unmarried adolescent
mothers' views of and relationship with men. Adolescence, 28(110),
425-438.
Tobias, B. B., & Ricer, R. E. (1998). Counseling adolescents
about sexuality. Primary Care, 25(1), 49-70.
Townsend, J. K., & Worobey, J. (1987). Mother and daughter
perceptions of their relationship: The influence of adolescent pregnancy
status. Adolescence, 12, 487-496.
UNICEF (2001, July). A league table of teenage births in rich
nations. (Innocenti Report Card, No. 3). Florence: UNICEF Innocenti
Research Centre.
Vandereycken, W. (1994). Parental rearing behavior and eating
disorders. In C. Perris, W. A. Arrindell, & M. Eisemann (Eds.),
Parenting and psychopathology (pp. 219-234). New York: John Wiley and
Sons.
von Bertalanffy, L. (1968). General systems theory: Foundations,
developments and applications. New York: Braziller.
Winefield, H. R., Tiggermann, M., & Winefield, A. H. (1994).
Parental rearing, behavior, attributional style and mental health. In C.
Perris, W. A. Arrindell, & M. Eisemann (Eds.), Parenting and
psychopathology (pp. 33-54). New York: John Wiley and Sons.
Woodward, L., Fergusson, D. M., & Horwood, L. J. (2001). Risk
factors and life processes associated with teenage pregnancy: Results of
a prospective study from birth to 20 years. Journal of Marriage and the
Family, 63, 1170-1185.
Yampolskaya, S., Brown, E. C., & Greenbaum, P. E. (2002). Early
pregnancy among adolescent females with serious emotional disturbances:
Risk factors and outcomes. Journal of Emotional & Behavioral
Disorders, 10, 108-126.
This research was supported in part by a grant from the Portuguese
Science and Technology Ministry, Technology and Science Foundation,
provided to the first author. The authors gratefully acknowledged the
participation of the adolescents, schools, and national health services
in the project.
Ana I. F. Pereira, School of Education Joao de Deus, Lisbon,
Portugal.
Maria C. Canavarro, Faculty of Psychology and Sciences of
Education, Coimbra University, Portugal.
Margarida F. Cardoso and Denisa Mendonqa, Institute for Biomedical Sciences Abel Salazar, ICBAS, Oporto University, Portugal.
Requests for repents should be sent to Aria I. F. Pereira, School
of Education Joao de Deus, Av. Alvares Cabral, 69, 1269-094 Lisboa,
Portugal. E-mail: aifreitaspereira@sapo.pt
Table 1
Family Factors
Pregnant Nonpregnant
Adolescents Adolescents
Factor (n = 57) (n = 81)
Number of siblings: M (SD) 3.09 (2.92) 1.44 (1.10)
Intact family: n (%) 30 (55.6) 66 (89.2)
Parent psychological/ 19 (33.3) 10 (12.3)
psychiatric intervention: n (%)
Mother adolescent 17 (29.8) 5 (6.2)
pregnancy: n (%)
Factor Statistical Tests
Number of siblings: M (SD) t = 4.06, p < 0.001
Intact family: n (%) [chi square] (1) = 18.84, p < 0.001
Parent psychological/ [chi square] (1) = 9.27, p < 0.01
psychiatric intervention: n (%)
Mother adolescent [chi square] (1) = 12.60, p < 0.001
pregnancy: n (%)
Table 2
Perceived Parental Rearing Behavior: Results from EMBU
Pregnant Nonpregnant
Adolescents Adolescents
Parental Behavior (n = 57) (n = 81)
Emotional support-father: 18.12 (5.44) 21.20 (4.73)
M (SD)
Overprotection-father: 12.65 (2.69) 14.97 (3.40)
M (SD)
Rejection-father: M (SD) 9.65 (2.79) 9.80 (2.23)
Emotional support-mother: 20.83 (4.71) 22.31 (3.77)
M (SD)
Overprotection-mother: 13.45 (2.79) 15.61 (3.44)
M (SD)
Rejection-mother: M (SD) 11.14 (6.67) 11.45 (3.18)
Parental Behavior Statistical Tests
Emotional support-father: t = -3.40, p < 0.001
M (SD)
Overprotection-father: t = -4.09, p < 0.001
M (SD)
Rejection-father: M (SD) ns
Emotional support-mother: t = -2.01, p < 0.05
M (SD)
Overprotection-mother: t = -3.98, p < 0.001
M (SD)
Rejection-mother: M (SD) ns
Table 3
Emotional Proximity to Peers and Boyfriend and Satisfaction
with Received and Given Social Support: Results from Convoy Model
Pregnant Nonpregnant
Emotional Proximity and Adolescents Adolescents
Satisfaction with Support (n = 57) (n = 81)
High emotional proximity to 23 (40.4) 74 (91.4)
peers *: n (%)
High emotional proximity to 49 (87.5) 28 (34.6)
boyfriend/partner*: n (%)
Satisfaction with received 3.81 (1.09) 3.88 (0.71)
support: M (SD)
Satisfaction with given 3.84 (0.73) 3.94 (0.73)
support: M (SD)
Emotional Proximity and
Satisfaction with Support Statistical Tests
High emotional proximity to [chi square] (1) = 41.68, p < 0.001
peers *: n (%)
High emotional proximity to [chi square] (1) = 37.69, p < 0.001
boyfriend/partner*: n (%)
Satisfaction with received ns
support: M (SD)
Satisfaction with given ns
support: M (SD)
* Low emotional proximity (0, 1), high emotional proximity (2, 3).
Table 4
Potential Risk Factors for Adolescent Pregnancy:
Results from Logistic Regression Analysis
Adjusted 95% Confidence
Predictor Variables Odds Ratio Interval p
Age 1.593 0.72-3.53 0.251
Area of residence 0.190
Rural 1 (a) --
Urban 3.026 0.58-15.87
Emotional proximity to peers 0.001
High 1 (a) --
Low 23.979 4.26-135.09
Number of school failures 7.401 2.18-25.10 0.001
Overprotection-mother 0.658 0.50-0.88 0.004
Emotional support-father 0.808 0.69-0.95 0.011
Mother adolescent pregnancy 0.037
No 1 (a) --
Yes 8.115 1.14-57.80
(a) Reference group.