The role of ego-identity status in mating preferences.
Dunkel, Curtis S. ; Papini, Dennis R.
"One could make a point for an evolutionary rationale which
would explain why sexual differences should not fully divide the sexes
until competence and fidelity permit their division to be one of
polarization, that is, one of mutual enhancement of experience and of
distribution of labor within a stylized pattern of love and care. Such a
rationale of human development would also suggest that the sexes are
less different in regard to the capacities and virtues which further
communication and cooperation; while the differences are greatest where
divergence is of the essence, that is, in the counterpoints of love life
and the divided functions of procreation. One could say, then, that the
sexes are most similar in the workings of the ego, which-being closest
to consciousness, language and ethics-must serve both to integrate the
fact of sexual mutuality and bipolarity" (Erikson, 1964; p. 129).
This quote from Erik Erikson suggests that he recognized that both
ego-identity and sex play important roles in many areas of life
including mating preferences, and that at the level of mating
preferences, these two factors most likely function independently. The
purpose of the current study is to examine this possibility while
juxtaposing two other views on mating preferences: Sexual Strategies
Theory (SST) and Social Role Theory (SRT).
Although there have been a number of negative critiques (e.g., most
recently van Hoof, 1999), the identity statuses (Marcia, 1966) continue
to be the most utilized model in identity research. The status model is
composed of two criteria: identity exploration and identity commitment.
Exploration is defined as actively investigating one's identity
options. Commitment is deciding to pursue a particular option and
investing in it.
Individuals can be further classified into statuses based on these
two criteria. Individuals are classified as identity achieved if they
exhibit exploration and commitment, identity moratorium if they exhibit
exploration without commitment, identity foreclosed if they exhibit
commitment without exploration, and identity diffused if they do not
exhibit exploration or commitment. Research has shown that the statuses
vary along a number of important social and personal dimensions (Marcia,
1980). One area that is pertinent to the development of sexuality that
has received some attention from identity researchers is menarche.
Both Berzonsky and Lombardo (1983) and Papini, Sebby, and Clark
(1989) found links between pubertal maturation, the affective quality of
the family environment, and identity. The convergence of pubertal
maturation and identity during the early adolescent years permits the
suggestion that later sexual strategies may be tempered by the
exploration and commitment processes that underlie the identity
statuses. Despite the abundance of research on status differences across
a wide range of topics including identity and menarche, no research has
been conducted on ego-identity and mating preferences and very little on
ego-identity and any aspect of sexuality.
One notable exception to the paucity of research is an unpublished
Master's thesis by King (1993), in which the relationship between
identity status and self-reported sexual behavior was examined. King
(1993) hypothesized that because an array of troublesome behaviors
(e.g., drug use, delinquency) is associated with identity diffusion,
identity diffusion would also be positively associated with risky sexual
behavior (e.g., having multiple partners). She also predicted that
because identity foreclosure is associated with conservative/traditional
attitudes, identity foreclosure should be negatively correlated with
risky sexual behavior. Empirical support was found for both hypotheses.
Although demographic information (including sex) on the participants was
reported, no statistical tests examining possible identity status x sex
interactions were reported.
Because the statuses differ on the amount of risky sexual behavior
exhibited, the results suggest that identity status may play a role in
mating preferences. Additionally, the research suggests that identity
diffusion and identity foreclosure adolescents should exhibit the
clearest differences. Identity diffusion should be positively associated
with a desire for a greater number of partners and a penchant for
short-term mating. The opposite trend should be found for identity
foreclosure. However, because the role played by sex was not examined,
the possible independence of, or interaction between, sex and identity
status on mating preferences remains unexamined.
Buss and Schmitt (1993) proposed that differences in the number of
sexual partners desired, interest in short-term mating, and desired
partner characteristics exhibited by men and women reflect evolved
mating strategies. Although, the differences between the sexes are not
categorical, cross-cultural research has shown that these differences
are likely to be universal (Buss, 1989; Schmitt, 2003).
SST is founded on the biologically based reproductive differences
between males and females. According to SST, each sex faces unique
challenges for reproductive success and therefore different strategies
for meeting these challenges have evolved. Sex differences in the amount
of parental investment are at the heart of these differences. Men
produce a vastly larger number of gametes than do women and they need to
and do exhibit (Low, 1989) lower investment to produce viable offspring.
Thus, because it was advantageous for reproduction, males evolved a
greater desire for short-term mating and desire more sexual partners
than do women. An additional tenet of SST, is that men place greater
emphasis on physical beauty than do women because beauty is correlated
with age and health and is therefore an indicator of reproductive
fitness.
Conversely, the female's investment in producing viable
offspring is high because it includes pregnancy, labor, and nursing. On
top of the increased investment in offspring by women, they are much
more limited in the number of offspring they can produce. To pass on
their genes, women need to make sure that the relatively smaller number
of children they can birth survive. Therefore, SST states that women are
more selective in their choice of mating partners and prefer partners
who can help provide resources to assist in the raising of offspring. In
summary, SST predicts that women are less likely to exhibit interest in
short-term mating, desire fewer sexual partners, and show greater
preference for a mate who can provide resources. Research has
consistently found support for the sex differences in mating preferences
hypothesized by SST (e.g., Buss, 1989; Buss & Schmitt, 1993;
Schmitt, 2003).
SST does not even remotely address ego-identity. Evolutionary
psychologists have viewed ego-identity as a cultural phenomenon
(Bjorklund & Pellegrini, 2002; McDonald, 1988). For example,
MacDonald (1988) notes, "From an evolutionary perspective identity
formation affects parent-child transmission of culture" (pp.
161-162). However, SST does not exclude the possible role ego-identity
may play in mating preferences. Given that sex differences explain only
a portion of the variance on mating preference variables, it seems
reasonable to examine other factors that may explain some of the
variance. And because adolescence is an important time for both sexual
and identity development, ego-identity may prove a useful explanatory
variable in understanding gender differences in mate selection
strategies. However, as also suggested by Erikson (1964) in the opening
quote, if ego-identity does play a role it is most likely independent of
the differences due to sex.
SRT (Eagly & Wood, 1999; Wood & Eagly, 2002) recognizes the
sex differences in mating preferences that have been found, but
attributes the differences to the gender roles proscribed by society and
adopted by individuals. Men and women guide or script their behavior so
that it matches the gender roles promoted by society. Therefore, sex
differences in the number of partners desired (Eagly, 2004), interest in
short-term mating (Eagly, 2004), and the idealistic characteristics of a
potential mate are also a function of gender roles--not specific evolved
adaptations (Eagly & Wood, 1999).
SRT (Eagly & Wood, 1999) predicts that because gender roles
vary across cultures, in those cultures with more traditional and
prescribed gender roles, sex differences in mating preferences should be
more pronounced. Results have shown that in less sexually egalitarian
societies, sex differences in what characteristics were desired in a
mate were more pronounced (Eagly & Wood, 1999; Kasser & Sharma,
1999).
Additionally, because individuals within any given culture vary in
the degree to which they identify with and have been socialized to
accept traditional gender roles, sex differences within a given culture
should also be of importance. Johannesen-Schmidt and Eagly (2002) tested
this proposition by examining the preferred mate characteristics in
American college students as a function of sex and the endorsement of
the traditional female role. The general pattern of the results
conformed to SRT because the sex differences in mate preference
characteristics were most pronounced in individuals who endorsed a
traditional female role.
If the prescription of behavior by the culture is the primary cause
of sex differences in mating preferences, it follows that those who
actively identity with the culture should be more likely to exhibit the
sex differences. Thus, individuals who have not questioned the
prevailing cultural attitudes or explored alternative points of view
should exhibit greater sex differences in mating preferences. Lack of
exploration of the traditional cultural view combined with a commitment
to it may be especially important in the examination of sex differences
in that these individuals should exhibit more sex-typed preferences.
The identity foreclosed status is defined by the commitment to
beliefs and values without exploration. Identity foreclosure is
associated with a number of variables that suggest adherence to a
traditional view. Identity foreclosure is associated with greater
obedience to authority (Podd, 1972), greater incidence of conventional
moral reasoning (Podd, 1972), high levels of authoritarianism (Marcia,
1966), a lack of openness (Clancy & Dollinger, 1993; Schwartz,
2001), high parental identification (MacDonald, 1988) and foreclosure
status individuals are described by experts in the field as
conservative, conventional, and exhibiting sex-appropriate behavior
(Mallory, 1989).
Thus from a social-role perspective ego-identity should interact
with sex to predict mating preferences. Specifically, because identity
foreclosure represents an identity closely tied with the traditional
culture, the sex differences in mating preferences should be most
pronounced in identity foreclosed individuals.
Restating the hypotheses:
(1) Ego-identity hypothesis: Both ego-identity and sex should
independently act as significant predictors of mating preferences with
identity diffusion being associated with a desire for a greater number
of sexual partners and greater interest in short-term mating.
(2) Sexual strategies hypothesis: Sex should predict mating
preferences. Ego-identity may or may not account for additional
variance.
(3) Social-role hypothesis: Ego-identity and sex should interact to
predict mating preferences with the greatest sex differences being
exhibited by those who are identity foreclosed.
METHOD
Participants
Based on a demographic questionnaire, potential participants were
screened for marital status, sexual orientation, and age. Only
participants who were single, heterosexual, and under 25 were included
in the study. After screening, 231 (136 female) undergraduate students
from a Midwestern community college and a Midwestern university (Age: M
= 19.41, SD = 1.34) participated in the study for extra class credit.
The sample included 208 Whites, 10 Blacks, 8 Hispanics, 4
Asian-Americans, and 1 student who marked "other" on the
demographics questionnaire and supplied no further information.
Materials and Procedure
Ego-identity. The Ego-Identity Process Questionnaire (EIPQ;
Balistreri, Busch-Rossnagel, & Geisinger, 1995) was used to measure
identity status. The EIPQ is made up of items designed to tap the eight
identity domains of occupation, religion, politics, values, family,
friendships, dating, and sex roles. The EIPQ is a 32-item Likert-type
measure with subscales pertaining to identity exploration (e.g., I have
undergone several experiences that made me change my views on men's
and women's roles) and identity commitment (e.g., My ideas about
men's and women's roles will never change). The coefficient
alpha using this sample was: exploration: ([alpha] = .72), commitment
([alpha] = .73).
The EIPQ scoring system for making status assignments was followed.
Median splits for both the exploration and commitment scores were
computed and individuals who scored below the median on each scale were
placed in the diffusion category (n = 52), those scoring above the
median on commitment and below in exploration were placed in the
foreclosure category (n = 70), those scoring below on commitment and
above on exploration were placed in the moratorium category (n = 63),
and those scoring above the median on each scale were placed in the
achievement category (n = 46).
Mating preferences. Three aspects of mating preferences were
examined: (1) total number of sexual partners desired in a lifetime,
(2), extent to which the participant is actively seeking a short-term
partner, and (3) preferred mate characteristics (i.e., physical
attractiveness and earning capacity).
The total number of sexual partners desired in a lifetime was
measured by an open-ended question taken from the "number of
partners" measure originally used by Buss and Schmitt (1993).
Participants were simply asked "Over your entire lifetime, how many
sexual partners would you ideally like to have?"
The extent to which a participant is seeking a short-term partner
was measured by a one-item, seven-point (1--not currently seeking,
7--strongly currently seeking), Likert-type scale (Buss & Schmitt,
1993; Schmitt, 2003), which asks participants the extent to which they
are actively seeking a short-term mating partner (e.g., one-night
stand).
Following Buss (1989), a compilation of characteristics (e.g.,
religious, exciting personality) of a potential mate was administered to
participants who were instructed to rate the importance of each
characteristic in a potential mate on a scale from 0 (not important at
all) to 3 (extremely important). A total of 26 characteristics were on
the list including the two target characteristics (good earning capacity
and physically attractive). Because the characteristics deemed most
desirable can vary according to the level of commitment in the
relationship (Buss & Schmitt, 1993; Kendrick, Sadalla, Groth, &
Trost, 1990), the list was presented twice. The participants were
instructed to rate the importance of the characteristics for both a
potential dating partner and a potential marriage partner.
RESULTS
Number of Partners Desired
Three participants responded to the question in a non-quantitative
manner and their data is not included in the analysis. A 2 (sex) x 4
(identity status) between subjects analysis of variance (ANOVA) was
performed with the number of partners desired acting as the dependent
variable. The assumption of homogeneity of variance was violated,
inflating the likelihood that the null hypothesis would be rejected.
Log-transformation of the data did not resolve the violation; therefore,
as suggested by Stevens (1996) the probability for rejection of the null
hypothesis was changed to p < .01 (1). When the probability was
changed to p < .01 the main effects were not impacted, but the
interaction term was no longer significant. As seen in Table 1, the
ANOVA resulted in a main effect for sex, F(1, 220) = 13.92, p < .001,
[[eta].sup.2] = .06, with males expressing a desire for a greater number
of partners than females. There was also a main effect for identity
status, F(3, 220) = 5.96, p < .005, [[eta].sup.2] = .08. A
Tukey's HSD post hoc test (p < .05) revealed that the diffusion
status participants expressed a desire for a greater number of partners
than did foreclosure status and achievement status participants, and
moratorium status participants expressed a desire for a greater number
of partners than did achievement status participants.
Although the change in the probability impacted the significance of
the interaction, due to its significance to the hypotheses, the results
are reported and the reader is asked to judge the findings with this
knowledge in mind. The interaction term resulted in a F(3, 220) = 2.68,
p < .05, [[eta].sup.2] = .04. An examination of simple effects
revealed that significant gender differences where apparent only in the
diffusion and foreclosure status groups.
Penchant for Short-term Mating
A 2 (sex) x 4 (identity status) between subjects ANOVA was
performed with the degree to which a short-term mate is sought acting as
the dependent variable. The homogeneity of variance assumption was
violated, but a transformation of the data did not clear up the matter,
therefore probability was adjusted to p < .01 (Stevens, 1996). As
seen in Table 1, the ANOVA resulted in a significant main effect for
sex, F(1, 223) = 15.98, p < .001, [[eta].sup.2] = .07, and a main
effect for identity status, F(3, 223) = 4.47, p < .005, [[eta].sup.2]
= .06. Males reported a higher level of seeking a short-term
relationship than did females and a Tukey's HSD post hoc test (p
< .05) revealed that identity diffusion participants reported a
higher level of short-term relationship seeking than did identity
foreclosure and identity achieved participants. The interaction between
sex and identity status was not significant.
Preferred Mate Characteristics
Data from two participants is not included in the analyses because
they failed to complete the mate characteristics questionnaire. Two 2
(sex) x 2 (commitment level: dating and marriage) x 4 (identity status)
mixed ANOVAs were performed with the rating of physical beauty acting as
the dependent variable for one analysis and earning potential acting as
the dependent variable on the second analysis. Sex and identity status
were between-subjects variables and commitment level was a
within-subjects variable.
For good earning capacity, there were significant main effects for
sex x commitment level. However, the main effects were superseded by a
significant sex x commitment level interaction, F(1,221) = 9.38, p <
.005, [[eta].sup.2] = .04. An examination of the simple effects revealed
that for both levels of commitment, females placed greater importance on
earning capacity than did males and that both sexes placed greater
importance on earning capacity in a marriage partner. The interaction
appears to be a result of the greater shift in importance with females
increasing the importance of earning capacity for a marriage partner
more than did males (see Table 2). The main effect for identity status
and all other interactions were not significant. The only significant
result for physical attractiveness was sex, F(1,221) = 15.48, p <
.001, [[eta].sup.2] = .07, with males (across level of commitment; M =
2.36, SD = .60) placing more importance on physical attractiveness than
did females (across level of commitment; M = 2.03, SD = .74).
DISCUSSION
Three contrasting hypotheses were tested in this study: the
ego-identity hypothesis proposes that ego-identity and sex play
independent roles in determining mating preferences, the sexual
strategies hypothesis suggests sex plays a role in mating preferences
and ego-identity may or may not play a role, and the social-role
hypothesis suggests that sex and ego-identity interact to determine
mating preferences. While the results were mixed, the majority of the
findings lend support to the ego-identity and sexual strategies
hypotheses.
As predicted by the ego-identity hypothesis, both sex and identity
status explained a significant amount of variance in the number or
partners desired, with ego-identity actually accounting for more
variance than sex. Consistent with a multitude of former findings, men
expressed a desire for a greater number of partners (e.g., Schmitt,
2003). Post hoc analyses also revealed a pattern in which participants
in the status groups defined by lack of commitment (diffusion and
moratorium) expressed a desire for a greater number of partners than did
those in the status groups defined by the presence of commitment
(achievement and foreclosure). Consistent with the findings of King
(1993), the diffusion status group exhibited a desire for the greatest
number of partners. Differing from King (1993), it was found that the
moratorium group desired a greater number of partners than did the
achievement group. The findings suggest that a desire for a relatively
large number of partners it not just associated with identity diffusion,
but with a general lack of commitment.
Depending on how one wants to interpret the statistics, there may
also have been a significant status by sex interaction on the number of
partners desired, and this interaction would qualify the main effects.
The simple effects showed that, as predicted by social role theory, sex
differences were more pronounced in the foreclosure status group.
However, the diffusion status group also exhibited a significant sex
difference. It may be that while identity foreclosure represents an
active commitment to the roles put forth by the larger culture, identity
diffusion represents a passive adoption of expectations. The orientation
of passive acceptance of norms is seen as a hallmark of the information
processing style of the diffusion status (Berzonsky & Sullivan,
1992). Of course, this is a speculative post hoc analysis of a
statistically questionable interaction. Additional research would be
needed to clarify this possibility.
The penchant for short-term mating also showed effects of identity
status and sex. Consistent with a multitude of former findings, men
expressed a desire for a greater number of partners (e.g., Schmitt,
2003). Consistent with what was found with the number of partners
desired, identity diffusion was associated with a greater penchant for
short-term mating. This lends further support for the ego-identity
hypothesis.
The results of the analyses for the preferred mating
characteristics strongly support the sexual strategies hypotheses
because sex was the only significant predictor of preference for
physical attractiveness and good earning capacity in a mate, with men
placing more emphasis than women on the former characteristic and women
placing more emphasis than men on the former. This finding has been
consistently found by researchers emphasizing both an evolutionary and
social role perspective. The findings of the current study suggest an
important caveat to the general sex differences. Sex differences are
moderated by the commitment level of the relationship (Buss &
Schmitt, 1993; Kendrick et al., 1990). In this study, greater level of
commitment increased the sex difference in the importance of good
earning capacity.
In past research (Buss, 1989; Johannesen-Schmidt & Eagley,
2002) participants have been instructed to rate the characteristics they
desire in a mate. The term mate is ambiguous and could be interpreted
any number of ways. The current findings girder past research showing
that the characteristics emphasized in a potential mate vary according
to the level of commitment. It could be, for example, that the influence
of the attitudes toward women's roles on sex differences varies
according to the level of commitment in the specified relationship.
CONCLUSIONS
Psychoanalytic and evolutionary psychology are not necessarily at
odds (Badcock, 1998; Surbey, 1998), and this research represents one
manner in which a traditional psychoanalytic domain of study
(ego-identity) helped explain variance in variables that are at the
heart of evolutionary psychology. The inclusion of ego-identity in the
prediction of the number of partners desired and the penchant for
short-term mating added explanatory power and was used to address an
important controversy in evolutionary psychology. Further research could
continue to explore the possibility that the expression of evolutionary
sexual strategies may be moderated by identity processes which serve as
the conduit through which the tension between sexual desires and
socialization pressures are resolved.
REFERENCES
Badcock, C. R. (1998). Psycho Darwinism: The new synthesis of
Darwin and Freud. In C. Crawford and D. Krebs (Eds.), Handbook of
evolutionary psychology: Ideas, issues, and applications. Mahwah, NJ:
LEA.
Balistreri, E., Busch-Rossnagel, N. A., & Geisinger, K. F.
(1995). Development and preliminary validation of the Ego Identity Process Questionnaire. Journal of Adolescence, 18, 179-192.
Berzonsky, M. D., & Lombardo, J. P. (1983). Pubertal timing and
identity crisis: A preliminary investigation. Journal of Early
Adolescence, 3, 239-246.
Berzonsky, M. D., & Sulllivan, C. (1992). Social-cognitive
aspects of identity style: Need for cognition, experiential openness,
and introspection. Journal of Adolescent Research, 7(2), 140-155.
Bjorklund, D. F., & Pellegrini, A. D. (2002). The origins of
human nature: Evolutionary developmental psychology. Washington, DC:
American Psychological Association.
Buss, D. M. (1989). Sex differences in human mate preferences:
Evolutionary hypotheses tested in 37 cultures. Behavioral and Brain
Sciences, 12, 1-49.
Buss, D. M., & Schmitt, D. P. (1993). Sexual strategies theory:
An evolutionary perspective on human mating. Psychological Review, 100,
204-232.
Clancy, S. M., & Dollinger, S. J. (1993). Identity, self, and
personality: I. Identity status and the five-factor model of
personality. Journal of Research on Adolescence, 3(3), 227-245.
Eagly, A. H. (2004, April). On the flexibility of human mating
preferences: A social role analysis. Invited address presented at the
annual meeting of the Midwestern Psychological Association.
Eagly, A. J., & Wood, W. (1999). The origins of sex differences
in human behavior: Evolved dispositions versus social roles. American
Psychologist, 54, 408-423.
Erikson, E. H. (1964). Insight and responsibility. New York:
Norton.
Johannesen-Schmidt, M. C., & Eagly, A. H. (2002). Another look
at sex differences in preferred mate characteristics: The effects of
endorsing the traditional female gender role. Psychology of Women
Quarterly, 26, 322-328.
Kasser, T., & Sharma, Y. S. (1999). Reproductive freedom,
educational equality, and females' preference for
resource-acquisition characteristics in mates. Psychological Science,
10, 374-377.
Kendrick, D. T., Sadalla, E. K., Groth, G., & Trost, M. R.
(1990). Evolution, traits, and the stages of human courtship: Qualifying
the parental investment model. Journal of Personality, 58(1), 97-116.
King, P. K. (1993). Adolescent sexual behavior and identity
development. Unpublished master's thesis, Utah State University,
Logan, Utah.
Low, B. S. (1989). Cross-cultural patterns in the training of
children: An evolutionary perspective. Journal of Comparative
Psychology, 103(4), 311-319.
MacDonald, K. B. (1988). Social and personality development: An
evolutionary synthesis. New York: Plenum Press.
Mallory, M. E. (1989). Q-sort definition of ego-identity status.
Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 18(4), 399-412.
Marcia, J. E. (1966). Development and validation of ego identity
status. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 3, 551-558.
Marcia, J. E. (1980). Identity in adolescence. In J. Adelson (Ed.),
Handbook of adolescent psychology. New York: Wiley.
Papini, D. R., Sebby, R. A., & Clark, S. (1989). Affective
quality of family relations and adolescent identity exploration.
Adolescence, 24, 457-466.
Pedersen, W. C., Miller, L. C., Putcha-Bhagavatula, A. D., &
Yang, Y. (2002). Evolved sex differences in the number of partners
desired? The long and the short of it. Psychological Science, 13(2),
157-161.
Podd, M. H. (1972). Ego identity status and morality: The
relationship between two developmental constructs. Developmental
Psychology, 6(3), 497-507.
Schmitt, D. P. (2003). Universal sex differences in the desire for
sexual variety: Tests from 52 nations, 6 continents, and 13 islands.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85(1), 85-104.
Schwartz, S. J. (2001). The evolution of Eriksonian and
neo-Eriksonian identity theory and research: A review and integration.
Identity: An International Journal of Theory and Research, 1(1), 7-58.
Stevens, J. (1996). Applied multivariate statistics for the social
sciences (3rd ed.). Mahwah, NJ: LEA.
Surbey, M. K. (1998). Developmental psychology and modern
Darwinism. In C. Crawford and D. Krebs (Eds.), Handbook of evolutionary
psychology: Ideas, issues and applications. Mahwah, NJ: LEA.
van Hoof, A. (1999). The identity status field re-reviewed: An
update of unresolved and neglected issues with a view on some
alternative approaches. Developmental Review, 19, 497-556.
Wood, W., & Eagly, A. H. (2002). A cross-cultural analysis of
the behavior of women and men: Implications for the origins of sex
differences. Psychological Bulletin, 128, 699-727.
FOOTNOTE
(1) The use of mean-based statistics for the number of sexual
partners desired measure has been critized (Pedersen, Miller,
Putcha-Bhagavatula, & Yang, 2002). When the number of partners
variable was categorized into three equal (n) groups (high/medium/low
based on the number of partners desired) and Chi-square analyses were
computed for the variables of sex, identity status, and number of
partners desired, the sex differences, [chi square] (2, N = 226) =
18.59, p < .001, and identity differences, [chi square] (6, N = 226)
= 21.35, p < .005, remained.
Dennis R. Papini, Psychology Department, Box 87, Middle Tennessee
State University, Murfreesboro, TN 37132
Reprint requests should be sent to Curtis S. Dunkel, Social
Sciences, Illinois Central College, One College Drive, East Peoria, IL
61635. E-mail, cdunkel@icc.edu
Table 1
Means for the Number of Partners Desired
and Penchant for Short-term Mating by Sex and
Identity Status
Sex Diffusion Foreclosure Moratorium
Number of partners desired
Male 10.14 (8.70) 5.33 (6.86) 5.19 (6.79)
Female 4.05 (4.56) 2.79 (3.52) 4.88 (5.50)
Total 6.61 (7.22) 3.84 (5.29) 5.02 (5.20)
Penchant for short-term mating
Male 3.14 (1.01) 2.13 (1.61) 2.14 (1.53)
Female 2.17 (1.64) 1.38 (1.00) 1.85 (1.33)
Total 2.58 (1.85) 1.70 (1.34) 1.94 (1.39)
Sex Achievement Total
Number of
partners desired
Male 3.57 (4.73) 5.99 (6.80)
Female 1.88 (1.55) 3.54 (4.33)
Total 2.68 (3.52) --
Penchant for
short-term mating
Male 2.32 (1.84) 2.41 (1.77)
Female 1.17 (0.64) 1.66 (1.27)
Total 1.72 (1.46) --
Note. Standard deviations are in parentheses.
Table 2
Preference for a Mate with Good Earning Capacity
by Sex and Level of Commitment
Commitment Male Female
Dating 1.51 (0.88) 1.75 (0.81)
Marriage 1.70 (0.85) 2.25 (0.76)
Note. Standard deviations are in parentheses.