Correlates of adolescent pregnancy in La Paz, Bolivia: findings from a quantitative-qualitative study.
Lipovsek, Vaija ; Karim, Ali Mebryar ; Gutierrez, Emily Zielinski 等
Adolescence is a transitional period characterized by opportunities
for healthful development as well as risks for adverse health and social
outcomes. It is now widely recognized that an adolescent's
prospects for healthful development are influenced by myriad social,
psychological, economic, and cultural factors (Kirby, 1999; Leffert et
al., 1998; Resnick et al., 1997). Some of these (e.g., poverty,
affiliation with peers who engage in high-risk behaviors, low
self-esteem) appear to be risk factors for adverse health and social
outcomes, while others (e.g., high parental educational expectations,
"connectedness" with family and school, high self-efficacy)
appear to be protective in that they tend to be associated with more
favorable outcomes. Recent adolescent health research has assigned high
priority to better understanding the relative importance of such factors
so that youth programs might be developed to better reinforce the
protective factors and mediate the risk factors that may be present in a
given setting (Leffert et al., 1998; Resnick et al., 1997).
The present study was undertaken to address the question of why
some female adolescents in La Paz, Bolivia, become pregnant while others
in similar circumstances avoid early pregnancy. To investigate this
question, the present study combined a nearest-neighbor case-control
design with qualitative research methods (focus-group discussions).
Study subjects for the case-control design consisted of matched pairs of
female adolescents of the same age and residing in the same
neighborhood, one of whom had experienced a pregnancy. Study subjects
for the focus-group discussions were adolescent girls from the same
population. The study focused on the following factors as possible
explanations of the observed discordant outcomes: family structure,
parental relationships, partner relationships, knowledge of pregnancy
risk behaviors, self-esteem, and locus of control (internal versus
external). The last three of these factors fall under the heading of
what Kirby (1999) has termed "sexual psychosocial antecedents." Altho ugh our primary interest is in causal
factors/antecedents of adolescent pregnancy, in many cases it is not
possible to establish the direction of causality between factors and
outcomes from the available data. For this reason, we use the term
"correlates" instead of "antecedents."
Acquiring a better understanding of the correlates of pregnancy
among Bolivian adolescents is important for several reasons. Bolivia,
similar to many developing countries, has a very young population.
Estimates from the 1998 Demographic and Health Survey indicate that 51%
of the Bolivian population was 19 years of age or younger, including
slightly over 10% between ages 15 and 19 (Republica de Bolivia, 1998).
Most (63%) Bolivian females reach the secondary level of education,
although by age twenty roughly 30% become pregnant (Republica de
Bolivia, 1998). Recent estimates indicate a teen birth rate in Bolivia
of 79 births per 1,000 females aged 15-19, well above the regional
average (including North America) of 68 births (Republica de Bolivia,
1998; UNICEF, 1998). WHO/PAHO (1998) projections suggest that in the
year 2000, 13% of all births in Bolivia will be to adolescents. Abortion
in Bolivia is legal only if the health of the mother is in danger or in
the case of rape, and thus voluntary termination of pre gnancies is not
well documented and occurs under nonregulated conditions (Population
Reference Bureau, 1994).
METHOD
The target population for the study was females 13-19 years of age
residing in a peri-urban area of La Paz--the First Health District,
known as La Garita de Lima. The total population of this area is
estimated to be 250,000. There are 14 primary health care centers in the
First Health District, all of which were sites for data collection.
A combination of quantitative and qualitative methods was used in
gathering data for the study. The quantitative study design was a
nearest-neighbor case-control design frequently used in epidemiologic
research. Adolescents presenting at a primary health care center during
the designated study period who were either currently pregnant or had
been pregnant in the previous 12 months constituted the
"cases." Adolescents of the same age (plus/minus one year) who
resided in the same neighborhood as the cases, but who had never been
pregnant, served as "controls." Study investigators familiar
with the area, using the case's address as provided during her
interview, visited residences nearest to the case's home until
finding a suitable control (without revealing the identity of the
control). Due to the intensity of the effort, one-to-one matching was
used. Matching on neighborhood of residence has been shown in other
studies to be effective in controlling for differences in socioeconomic
status (Mock et al., 1993). The qualitative design consisted of
focus-group interviews with adolescents from the same population. Girls
who agreed to participate in the quantitative aspect (i.e., consented to
a questionnaire interview) were also asked to participate in the
focus-group discussions.
A structured questionnaire was administered to adolescents who
agreed to be interviewed. Data were gathered in May-June 1999 by
research assistants trained in interviewing techniques and provided
written instructions. By the end of a six-week data collection period,
190 completed questionnaires had been obtained--95 cases and 95
controls. As adolescent pregnancy is a sensitive topic in Bolivia, the
refusal rate among potential cases was high. The extent to which this
may have biased study findings is unknown.
The structured questionnaire covered the following topics: marital
status and living arrangements, family background, sexual experience,
parental and partner relationships, knowledge of risk of pregnancy,
self-esteem, and locus of control. The questionnaire was pretested at a
maternity hospital in La Paz among adolescents presenting for a prenatal check-up and among adolescent girls of similar age and socio-demographic
characteristics at a nearby secondary school.
After preliminary analysis of the data from the structured
questionnaires, cases and controls associated with four of the sites
(Chamoco Chico, El Tejar, Cuidadela Ferroviaria, and La Portada) were
recruited to participate in focus-group discussions. However, due to the
sensitivity of the subject matter, none of the adolescents who were, or
had been, pregnant agreed to participate in the focus groups. Three
focus-group discussions were conducted with girls who had never been
pregnant, stratified by age (13-14 years, 15-16 years, and 17-18 years).
Each focus group had 6-8 participants and lasted approximately 90
minutes.
The focus-group discussions centered around perceived risks and
benefits of early pregnancy, its prevention, and its consequences. As
the focus groups took place after the completion of the questionnaire
data collection and preliminary analysis, they were intended to validate
and further inform the quantitative results.
Of the initial 95 case-control pairs, four pairs were subsequently
dropped due to age discrepancies of more than one year, as were three
pairs that did not match sufficiently well with regard to place of
residence. This left 88 matched pairs available for analysis which,
although limited, is supported by other research as sufficient to employ
the case-control design (Caton et al., 2000).
All variables measured were discrete, and most were analyzed as
such: variables pertaining to marital status, living arrangements,
family background, sexual experience, parental and partner
relationships, and locus of control. In addition, continuous scales were
created to measure two constructs, self-esteem and knowledge of risk of
pregnancy. A scale with 12 items pertaining to perceptions and feelings
about relationships with peers and partners was created to measure
self-esteem (Cronbach's [alpha] .73), and a scale with 9 items was
created to measure knowledge regarding pregnancy (Cronbach's
[alpha] = .43). The alpha coefficient of the knowledge scale should be
interpreted with some caution, as inconsistent responses may be
indicative of inconsistent knowledge rather than low scale reliability
(Zimet, 1992). All the items used for the two scales had binary
responses. Accordingly, scales were developed using latent trait
analysis (McDonald, 1999), which assumes that there is a single latent
attribute per s cale and that all items in the scale measure that
attribute. Classical item analysis (item-total score correlation and
item-rest correlation) was used to select items for each scale
(McDonald, 1999). Items for locus of control did not scale well using
the classical item analysis method and therefore a scale was not
constructed to measure this attribute. The items composing each scale
and percentage of positive responses (i.e., positive selfesteem,
internal locus of control, and correct knowledge of risk of pregnancy)
are presented in Table 1.
Conditional logistic regression was used to test bivariate associations between the self-esteem index and pair wise-matched cases
and controls. Odds-ratios for all other factors were estimated using the
Mantel-Haenszel method. Matching was accounted for using the
nonparametric McNemar chi-square statistic. Multivariate analyses were
not conducted due to the differing missing values in the independent
variables, which caused the number of case-control pairs to drop
significantly (from the initial 88 pairs). Therefore, the low power of
the sample size precluded meaningful multivariate results.
The focus-group interviews were audio recorded and transcribed by
the Bolivian researcher. They were reviewed and synthesized around the
main themes of interest: perceived risks and benefits of early
pregnancy, its prevention, and its consequences.
RESULTS
Before turning to the quantitative findings of the study, it is
informative to consider some of the consequences of early pregnancy
perceived by focus-group participants, none of whom had been pregnant.
These were overwhelmingly negative. Younger focus-group participants
expected punishment from family ("they would hit you,"
"father would reject you because you made him look bad"), from
peers ("friends would not spend time with you anymore"), and
from the community ("you would be kicked out of school; people
would talk badly about you"). A girl who gets pregnant, it was
stated, would "not want to leave the house" or "would
want to kill herself."
The perceptions of the older focus-group participants (ages 15-18)
were comparable--they expected to be punished by their family and spoken
of badly by the community. They stated that the family might support
them, but that more often a young woman would face serious pressure from
her family to marry the father of the child. They expected the community
to look badly upon them ("people don't want their daughters to
be friends with you"), but they also stated that their friends
would stand by them and help them. As with the younger girls, these
young women felt that getting pregnant would be a terrible outcome for
them: "girls first think about abortion or suicide or of going
away." One girl said, "if you can't talk to your parents,
then you look for marriage to get out of the house."
Some of the consequences anticipated by focus-group participants,
none of whom had been pregnant, are borne out by the data obtained from
the structured questionnaires (Table 2). Two-thirds of cases were either
married or cohabiting, compared with only 8% of controls. Early
pregnancy and entry into marriage/union thus appear to be inextricably bound in this area of La Paz. Adolescent pregnancy also has significant
consequences with regard to school attendance and employment. Less than
one-third of cases attended school full time as compared with 76% of
controls, and over three times as many cases as controls were employed.
Nearly one-third of cases were neither attending school nor working
(they likely were caring for infants/children), compared with only 3% of
the controls.
It should be noted, however, that the direction of causation between adolescent pregnancy and marriage, school matriculation, and
employment status is uncertain. It is possible that adolescents in La
Paz who get pregnant early are self-selected with regard to their
aspirations toward education, marriage, and children. However, these
were not mentioned in the focus-group discussions as a rationale
for/benefit of early pregnancy.
It may also be observed in Table 2 that there were no differences
between cases and controls in age at first boyfriend and number of
lifetime boyfriends. However, it is not possible to discern the
differences in these relationships that contributed to diverging pregnancy outcomes, since the controls were not asked if they had ever
engaged in sexual intercourse.
Findings regarding the association between adolescent pregnancy and
family background factors (place of birth, parents' place of birth,
and number of male or female siblings) are presented in Table 3. As may
be observed, cases and controls did not differ significantly with regard
to any of the family characteristics considered.
Associations between parental relationship factors and adolescent
pregnancy are displayed in Table 4. Significant differences are observed
between cases and controls on three of the six factors considered. Cases
were 2.44 times as likely as controls to have reported substantial
fighting with their parents, 0.48 times as likely to have said that
their parents were affectionate, and 0.39 times as likely to have said
that their parents were supportive (p < .05 for all three factors).
As adolescent pregnancy in a conservative society such as Bolivia may
well affect the quality of child-parent relationships, at least in some
cases a pregnancy may have been the cause instead of the outcome of
adverse relationships. However, a number of prior studies in which the
causal sequencing is clear have shown positive parental relationships to
be important protective factors for adolescent pregnancy (Kirby, 1999;
Leffert et al., 1998; Resnick et al., 1997).
The combination of these findings and the results from the focus
groups suggest that Bolivian adolescents do not tend to view parents as
a key source of information or support for matters concerning sexuality,
contraception, and pregnancy. When asked where they would turn for
support on such matters, focus-group participants named friends as their
first choice. Some also suggested going to the health center, although
some acknowledged that contact with health facilities carried the risk
of stigmatization--"if you go there, everyone will think you are
pregnant."
With regard to relationships with partners, cases and controls
differed significantly on only one of the seven factors considered-
whether respondents had talked with their partner about getting pregnant
(see Table 5). Cases were 3.5 times more likely to have talked with
their partner about pregnancy than were controls (p <.01). However,
as such discussions may well have occurred after they became pregnant,
this factor cannot necessarily be viewed as an antecedent of adolescent
pregnancy.
Only two items measured the association between peer influence and
adolescent pregnancy. One of them--whether the respondent saw her
friends often--was found to be protective regarding early pregnancy (p
<.01). However, it is possible that, as with parental-relationship
variables, relationship with peers was substantially altered as a result
of an early pregnancy.
It is noteworthy that there were no significant differences on the
other partner dimensions considered, particularly given the emphasis on
partner communication and negotiation in many adolescent reproductive
health programs. The focus-group discussions shed some light on this
topic, highlighting the importance of partner (and peer) pressure in
decisions to have sex. Regarding partner pressure, older focus-group
participants stated: "if you do it, he won't leave you,"
"if you refuse, he may look for another girl," and "if
you refuse, other girls will make fun of you." On the other hand,
one participant observed that "if a girl refuses to have sex, her
partner may leave her, but he may also lose interest and leave her after
he's had sex with her." In the event of pregnancy, they saw
two possibilities: "either he leaves you, or he's made to
marry you."
In addition, while most girls believed that it was the
responsibility of both partners to think about protection, they felt
that it was really up to them to take care of themselves. However, they
identified embarrassment and the fear of being perceived as promiscuous
by their partners if they demanded the use of contraception. They stated
that most girls use rhythm as a method to avoid pregnancy, and although
most knew of other methods, they did not use them.
Findings regarding associations between adolescent pregnancy and
two sets of factors falling under the general heading of "sexual
psychosocial" factors are presented in Table 6. This table shows
the association between self-esteem and knowledge regarding pregnancy
indices and the outcome of interest (pregnancy). Although the alpha
score for the pregnancy knowledge index is low, this is not seen as
problematic in the case of knowledge because, as mentioned previously,
inconsistent responses may be indicative of inconsistent knowledge as
opposed to low scale reliability (Zimet, 1992).
As may be observed, there was a strong inverse linear relationship
between level of self-esteem and likelihood of adolescent pregnancy (p
< .01). That is, as the score on the self-esteem index increased, the
likelihood of pregnancy decreased. As with other correlates, it is
unclear whether diminished self-esteem is a cause or an effect of
unwanted pregnancy. In the focus-group discussions, the participants
spoke at length of negative consequences of unwanted pregnancy,
including rejection by the partner, peers, and even the broader
community, as well as possible punishment from parents. In light of
this, it would seem that pregnant girls in La Paz are very likely to
have lowered self-esteem as a consequence of the response of their
immediate environment to their pregnancy.
The index measuring knowledge regarding pregnancy was not found to
be associated with the outcome. As cases and controls did not differ
significantly on any of the items, knowledge alone does not seem to be
an explanatory factor for the disparities in pregnancy outcomes.
However, the focus-group discussions yielded important insight into the
difficulties adolescents in La Paz have in obtaining accurate
information on reproduction and contraception. A number of the younger
girls reported that they did not know anything about sexual relations,
expressing that they were too young to talk about it, and that they had
not yet been taught about it in school. They had, however, heard of
condoms, and gave various sources of information for questions
pertaining to sex and contraception, naming (in order of preference):
friends, older sister or cousin, mothers, and the physical education
teacher at school.
Older girls said they were taught the biological aspects of sex in
school. When asked how to prevent pregnancy, they mentioned using
condoms, going to the pharmacy, going to the health clinic, and waiting
until marriage. When asked about where information about sex or
contraceptives could be obtained, the girls listed (in order of
preference): friends (a comfortable and easily accessible source of
information, although some girls expressed concern about the accuracy of
the information obtained); media; school (although they reported that
the information given was often in unfamiliar terms, was confusing and
limited, and was not in tune with their reality); and family
(information obtained from family was also seen as limited and not
always relevant, and girls reported being uncomfortable asking parents
about these topics).
In addition to self-esteem and knowledge regarding pregnancy, there
were also three items that attempted to measure locus of control. Little
correlation was found between these three items (through item-total
score correlation and item-rest correlation). For this reason, a scale
was not formed, but each of the items was tested individually, and no
association was found between these items and the outcome (the
individual items are listed in Table 1).
While the focus-group discussions did not address locus of control
specifically, the (perceived or real) inability of girls to demand the
use of contraceptives from their partners, or obtain them from
pharmacies or health centers, points to the low level of control
Bolivian girls exert upon such important decisions. This should not,
however, be mistaken for lack of awareness or desire to act, as the
focus-group participants clearly noted they knew that contraceptive methods existed and where they could be obtained.
DISCUSSION
The present study sought to identify some of the factors
responsible for discordant pregnancy outcomes among matched pairs of
female adolescents in La Paz, Bolivia. One important factor was parent
relationships, which were found to be strongly correlated with early
pregnancy: never-pregnant girls were more likely than girls who were or
had been pregnant to have reported being shown affection and to feel
supported by their parents. Conversely, ever-pregnant girls reported
higher levels of fighting in their parents' home than did controls.
These findings are consistent with recent literature indicating that
"connectedness" with parents is a powerful protective factor
against a broad range of negative health and development outcomes among
adolescents, and that strained parent-child relations and a coercive parenting style are often antecedents of adolescent risk behaviors and
negative outcomes (Caton et al., 2000; Bearman et al., 1999; Cole,
1998).
Interpreting this result in the light of focus-group discussions
suggests that a serious strain on the parent-child relationship is
nearly unavoidable following an unintended pregnancy. Of equal
importance, however, were the focus-group findings that communication
with parents regarding sexuality is generally difficult for all Bolivian
girls. Focus-group participants stated that it was difficult to discuss
issues of sexuality and contraceptives with parents due to
embarrassment, and that information received from them was seen as
limited and not relevant to their lives. In addition, fear of punishment
and rejection was expressed very strongly regarding parental
relationships, particularly among the younger girls. Rejection could
take the form of physical punishment or disappointment of parents or, in
a more direct way, the girl could be rejected by the family by being
forced to marry the father of the child. These results suggest that
adolescents do not see their family as a source of support in a
situation o f need. Improved parent-child communication relating to matters of sexuality and contraception would thus seem an important
focus of future adolescent health initiatives in Bolivia.
This study also found that never-pregnant girls were more likely to
have had high self-esteem than those who were or had been pregnant.
Self-esteem was measured through a series of statements concerning
perceptions and feelings about relationships with peers and partners.
The statements revolved around sexuality and sexual decision-making in
which the fear of rejection by both peers and partner figured
prominently. The finding of a protective effect of self-esteem is
consistent with much of the research literature (Kirby, 1999;
Modrcin-Talbott et al., 1998; Connelly, 1997; Cole, 1997; Mullis &
Mullis, 1997; Morgan, Chapar, & Fisher, 1995), although it should be
noted that the literature is not entirely consistent on the relationship
between self-esteem and sexual risk-taking. Some studies have found no
relationship between self-esteem and pregnancy status after controlling
for key background factors (e.g., age and socioeconomic status) and
others have observed higher levels of self-esteem among adolescents wh o
practiced risky sexual behaviors and had more sexual partners. A recent
study of secondary school students in urban Peru found that the effects
of self-esteem on sexual risk-taking behavior varied by gender--among
boys, high self-esteem was associated with higher likelihood of sexual
activity, but with a lower likelihood of sexual activity among girls
(Magnani, Zielinski Gutierrez, Seiber, & Vereau, 2000). While the
issue of causality must be approached with some caution, the statements
that measured self-esteem in the present study generally revolved around
perceptions and beliefs that would not necessarily be fundamentally
changed with an unintended pregnancy. A plausible explanation is that
low self-esteem among girls is in fact both an antecedent and an outcome
of unwanted pregnancy; that is, girls with low self-esteem may be
predisposed to sexual risk-taking, but girls who get pregnant in an
environment that subsequently ostracizes them might also exhibit lowered
self-esteem.
The finding that girls who spend more time with their friends were
less likely to be pregnant is of some interest. While the negative
influence of peers has been explored in the literature (Bearman et al.,
1999), less is known about the detrimental effects of peer isolation.
One possible interpretation of these findings is that girls with some
form of peer network are more likely to be protected against adverse
health outcomes, including unplanned pregnancy. Such an interpretation
is supported by the focus-group results, where friends were cited as an
important, albeit not entirely reliable, source of reproductive health
information. The most reliable sources of such information cited in the
focus groups were female siblings. Based on these data, it is possible
to hypothesize that girls who were more isolated (that is, had no female
siblings and did not spend much time with friends) were more likely to
have become pregnant.
No clear effects of variations in partner relationships on the
likelihood of adolescent pregnancy were observed in the quantitative
data gathered for the study. This might be attributable to the limited
power Bolivian adolescent females appear to have in such relationships.
The information extracted from the focus groups indicates that there is
little communication between adolescent partners in Bolivia about
pregnancy and contraception. Focus-group participants reported an
unwillingness to talk about contraception with their partners or request
that condoms be used due to embarrassment and fear of appearing
promiscuous. Nonetheless, they stated it was up to them to protect
themselves against pregnancy, and most used the rhythm method to do so.
However, given the relatively low use/effectiveness of the rhythm method
and its inability to protect girls against sexually transmitted
diseases, this is a less than optimal safeguard.
The fear of being rejected by partners also figured prominently in
the focus-group discussions. Participants reported fearing that if they
did not consent to sexual relations, their partners would leave them.
Paradoxically, they also stated that a partner might leave a girl after
he has had sex with her (he might lose interest in her), and that a
partner often leaves a girl after she has become pregnant. This leaves a
girl in a situation of having to balance her own interests, her
family's expectations of her, and her desire for an intimate
relationship.
Unlike in other recent studies, no significant differences were
observed between cases and controls with regard to locus of control. It
is clear from the focus-group discussions, however, that control issues
figured prominently in decision-making concerning sex and contraception.
Focus-group participants identified pressure from peers and partners as
powerful factors; for example, that a partner would pressure a girl into
having sex to show love for him and that even other girls would look
down upon a girl in their group who did not have sexual relations.
Overall, the combination of these different findings make it
unclear whether controls had avoided pregnancy by luck, by protecting
themselves with contraceptives, or by a strong sense of self that
allowed them to not be influenced by peer and/or partner pressure to
engage in sexual intercourse. Nevertheless, the findings do suggest that
adolescent females in La Paz lack trustworthy support networks-with
their families, peer groups, and partners-that would enable them to seek
information regarding sex and contraception and act upon that
information. The findings further suggest that while individual factors
such as high self-esteem are associated with being able to avoid
unwanted pregnancy, structural factors also contribute to the health and
development status of adolescent females. The focus-group discussions
revealed a complex web of such factors: girls are encouraged (sometimes
pressured) to have sexual relations by partners and peers, but strict
social norms preclude open discussion about sexuality and c ontraception
between parents and youth, as well as between adolescent partners. At
the same time, transgression of norms (getting pregnant) elicits
punishment and ostracism from family, peers, partners, and the broader
community.
Interventions seeking to reinforce protective factors for Bolivian
adolescents might, therefore, focus on improving communication and
dialogue between parents and children, between partners, as well as in
the larger community about sexuality, sexual risks, and ways to protect
oneself against unwanted pregnancy. Since sex education exists in
Bolivian schools, this can be an opportunity to discuss not only
biological aspects of reproduction, but also relationships,
communication, and negotiation with partners. Myths regarding
promiscuity and use of contraception could be dispelled, and joint
responsibility for the use of contraception among sexually active
adolescents could be encouraged. A successful school- and mass
media-based peer promotion initiative recently implemented in Paraguay
provides a model for such an intervention (Magnani, Robinson, Seiber,
& Avila, 2000).
This study illustrates the pressing need for more in-depth research
into the factors that may influence the likelihood of pregnancy among
young women living under similar circumstances. The biases that affected
this study are, in some ways, difficult to overcome in a study limited
by data collection only after pregnancy occurs. Nevertheless, the study
findings do highlight the combination of structural and personal factors
that interact to produce health and development risks for adolescent
females in Bolivia. The findings suggest that targeting only the
behavior of adolescent girls is not likely to be sufficient. In order to
be successful, it is likely that parents, partners, and the community at
large have to be involved in the process of reducing risks and creating
opportunities for healthful development of Bolivian adolescents. The
potential benefits of being able to closely target programs to reach
young women at risk, and to offer programs capable of mediating the
risks, are of interest to all in the f ield of adolescent reproductive
health.
Table 1
Percentage of Positive/Correct Responses to Items Constituting the
Self-Esteem, Locus of Control, and Knowledge of Pregnancy Indices by
Cases and Controls
Items Cases Controls
Self-esteem
I feel very attractive when my 76.5 88.4
partner asks me for sex, and that's
why I accept *
Girls who have sex are more mature 75.6 72.4
It would be terrible for me if I 62.4 81.0
could not satisfy my partner
sexually **
If I ask questions about sex, my 51.8 64.7
friends and family will think badly of me
If I don't want to lose my partner, 81.2 89.8
I have to have sex
Being capable of having sex makes me 88.0 94.2
feel more important
In this day and age, it's only the shy 70.6 77.9
and insecure girls who don't dare to
have sex
When I'm with my group, I don't dare 50.6 64.0
to say what I think about sex because
they might criticize me
I am afraid my group will make fun 86.9 94.2
of/reject me, and that's why I have
sex like the other girls in the group
Girls who can't hold on to a boyfriend 84.7 90.7
are losers
I may be afraid of getting pregnant, 60.7 81.4
but I'm more afraid of being rejected
by my partner **
To give in to the "proof of love" is 64.7 88.1
the only way my partner can be sure
of me **
Locus of control
I follow my friends' advice regarding 90.7 86.2
sex, because that way they respect me
more
Following the rules of my religion 60.0 49.4
protects me from any danger
My boyfriend knows how to 41.2 30.9
direct/control our relationship, and
that's why I trust him
Knowledge of pregnancy
A woman can get pregnant...
When the man puts the penis in the 91.9 90.7
vagina and leaves his milk
Only when the man puts his penis 43.9 54.9
in the vagina
When she has sex without protection 95.3 86.9
When she's taking pills and she 69.1 64.1
forgets a day
When the man hugs and kisses her 98.9 95.2
strongly
The first time she has sex with 76.3 71.8
a man without protection
When she is menstruating 27.7 26.5
When the penis enters only a little 73.4 61.3
and leaves the milk
When she protects herself with 3.5 10.7
birth control
Significant difference (two-tailed) between cases (n = 88) and controls
(n = 88)
* p < .05
** p < .01
Table 2
Percentage Distribution of Cases and Controls by Marital Status,
Occupation, and Dating.
Factor Category Cases Controls
Marital status Married *** 18.8 3.4
Cohabiting ** 47.1 4.5
Single** 34.1 92.0
Total respondents (n) 85 88
Occupation Full-time student ** 32.9 76.1
Working ** 21.2 5.7
Part-time student 14.1 14.8
Neither ** 31.8 3.4
Total respondents (n) 85 88
Age at first boyfriend 8-12 10.6 6.2
13-14 34.1 42.0
15-16 48.2 45.7
17-20 7.1 6.2
Total respondents (n) 85 81
Number of lifetime 1 21.3 15.8
boyfriends 2 18.7 26.3
3 25.3 17.1
4 + 34.7 40.8
Total respondents (n) 75 76
Significant difference (two-tailed) between cases and controls
** p < .01
*** p < .001
Table 3
Associations Between Family Background Factors and Adolescent Pregnancy
95%
OR confidence McNemar's
Factors Categories (adjusted) interval [chi square]
Birthplace Other 1.00
This city 0.65 0.30 - 1.38 1.29
Parents' birthplace Other 1.00
This city 0.88 0.49 - 1.57 0.20
Male siblings None 1.00
At least one 0.83 0.36 - 1.92 0.18
Female siblings None 1.00
At least one 0.47 0.21 - 1.05 3.57
No. No. of
of discordant
Factors pairs pairs
Birthplace
77 28
Parents' birthplace
82 45
Male siblings
88 22
Female siblings
88 28
Table 4
Associations Between Parental Relationship Factors and Adolescent
Pregnancy
95%
OR confidence McNemar's
Factors Categories (adjusted) interval [chi square]
Relationship with Regular/bad 1.00
parents is... Good 0.64 0.33 - 1.24 1.78
Is communication No 1.00
with parents good? Yes 1.07 0.52 - 2.22 0.03
Is there much No 1.00
fighting in your Yes 2.44 1.13 - 5.31 5.45 *
home?
Are your parents No 1.00
affectionate? Yes 0.48 0.23 - 0.98 4.24 *
Do you trust your No 1.00
parents? Yes 0.47 0.20 - 1.09 3.24
Are your parents No 1.00
very supportive? Yes 0.39 0.16 - 0.93 4.84 *
No. of
No. of discordant
Factors pairs pairs
Relationship with
parents is... 82 36
Is communication
with parents good? 77 29
Is there much
fighting in your 72 31
home?
Are your parents
affectionate? 75 34
Do you trust your
parents? 71 25
Are your parents
very supportive? 68 25
* p < .05 (two-tailed)
Table 5
Associations Between Partner Relationship and Peer Influence Factors and
Adolescent Pregnancy
95%
OR confidence McNermar's
Factors Categories (adjusted) interval [chi square]
Partner relationship
Overall, Regular/bad 1.00
Relationship with Good 0.73 0.34-1.60 0.62
partner is...
Have you ever No 1.00
talked with Yes 3.50 1.41-8.67 8.33 **
partner about
pregnancy?
Is communication No 1.00
with partner Yes 1.00 0.47-2.10 <0.01
good?
Do you fight No 1.00
often with your Yes 0.87 0.41-1.82 0.14
partner?
Is your partner No 1.00
affectionate? Yes 0.60 0.18-1.82 1.00
Do you trust your No 1.00
partner? Yes 1.36 0.43-2.96 0.62
Is your partner No 1.00
very supportive? Yes 0.77 0.34-1.75 0.39
Peer Influences
Any of your No 1.00
friends drink Yes 0.59 0.27-1.23 2.31
alcohol regularly?
Do you get No 1.00
together with Yes 0.29 0.12-0.62 12.10 **
friends often?
No. No. of
of discordant
Factors pairs pairs
Partner relationship
Overall,
Relationship with 72 26
partner is...
Have you ever
talked with 67 27
partner about
pregnancy?
Is communication
with partner 67 28
good?
Do you fight
often with your 64 28
partner?
Is your partner
affectionate? 66 16
Do you trust your
partner? 67 26
Is your partner
very supportive? 65 23
Peer Influences
Any of your
friends drink 82 35
alcohol regularly?
Do you get
together with 83 40
friends often?
** p < .01 (two-tailed p value)
Table 6
Associations Between Self-Esteem and Knowledge Regarding Pregnancy
Indices and Adolescent Pregnancy
95% confidence
Factors OR (adjusted) interval p value
Self-esteem index 0.03 <0.01 - 0.27 .002
(range:0-1;alpha:0.73)
Knowledge regarding 0.87 0.24 - 3.12 .833
pregnancy index
(range:0-1;alpha:0.43)
REFERENCES
Bearman, P., Bruckner, H., Brown, B. B., Theobald, W., &
Philliber, S. (1999). Peer potential: Making the most of how teens
influence each other. Washington, DC: The National Campaign to Prevent
Teen Pregnancy.
Caton, C. L., Hasin, D., Shrout, P. E., Opler, L. A., Hirshfield,
S., Dominguez, B., & Felix, A. (2000). Risk factors for homelessness
among indigent urban adults with no history of psychotic illness: A
case-control study. American Journal of Public Health, 90(2), 258-263.
Cole, F. L. (1997). The role of self-esteem in safer sexual
practices. Journal of the Association of Nurses in AIDS Care, 8(6),
64-70.
Connelly, C. D. (1998). Hopefulness, self-esteem, and perceived
social support among pregnant and nonpregnant adolescents. Western
Journal of Nursing Research, 20(2), 195-209.
Kirby, D. (1999). Antecedents of adolescent sexual risk-taking,
pregnancy, and childbearing: Implications for research and programs.
Washington, DC: National Campaign to Prevent Teen Pregnancy.
Leffert, N., Benson, P., Scales, P., Sharma, A., Drake, D., &
Blythe, D. (1998). Developmental assets: Measurement and prediction of
risk behaviors among adolescents. Applied Development Science, 2(4),
209-230.
Magnani, R. J., Robinson, A., Seiber, E., & Avila, G. (2000).
Impact of a health communication program on adolescent reproductive
health knowledge, attitudes, and behaviors in three cities in Paraguay.
Washington, DC: FOCUS on Young Adults.
Magnani, R. J., Zielinski Gutierrez, E., Seiber, E., & Vereau,
D. (2000). Antecedents of sexual activity and condom use among secondary
school students in urban Peru. Washington, DC: FOCUS on Young Adults
Program/Pathfinder International.
McDonald, R. P. (1999). Test theory: A unified treatment (pp.
231-249). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Mock, N. B., Magnani, R. J., Dikassa, L., Rice, J. C., Abdoh, A.,
& Bertrand, W. E. (1993). The utility of case-control methods for
health policy and planning analysis: An illustration from Kinshasa,
Zaire. Evaluation and Program Planning, 16(2), 199-205.
Modrcin-Talbott, M. A., Pullen, L., Zandstra, K, Ehrenberger, H.,
& Muenchen, B. (1998). A study of self-esteem among well adolecents:
Seeking new direction. Issues in Comprehensive Pediatric Nursing, 21(4),
229-241.
Morgan, C., Chapar, G. N., & Fisher, M. (1995). Psychosocial
variables associated with teenage pregnancy. Adolescence, 30(118),
277-289.
Mullis, A. K., & Mullis, R. L. (1997). Vocational interest of
adolescents: Relationships between self-esteem and locus of control.
Psychological Reports, 81(3), 1363-1371.
Population Reference Bureau. (1994). The world's youth 1994: A
special focus on reproductive health.
Republica de Bolivia, Ministerio de Hacienda e Instituto Nacional
de Estadistica and Macro International. (1998). Encuesta Nacional de
Demografia y Salud, 1998. Calverton, MD, USA. Demographic and Health
Surveys.
Resnick, M. D., Bearman, P. S., Blum, R. W., Bauman, K E., Harris,
K M., Jones, J., Tabor, J., Beuhring, T., Sieving, R. E., Shew, M.,
Ireland, M., Bearinger, L. H., & Udry, J. R. (1997). Protecting
adolescents from harm: Findings from the National Longitudinal Study of
Adolescent Health. Journal of the American Medical Association, 278(10),
823-832.
UNICEF. (1998). Progress of nations. New York: UNICEF. WHO/PAHO.
(1998). Health in the Americas, 1998 Edition (Volume 2). Washington, DC:
Pan American Health Organization.
Zimet, G. D. (1992). Reliability of AIDS Knowledge Scales:
Conceptual issues. AIDS Education and Prevention, 4(4), 338-344.
This research was supported by funds provided by USAID/Bolivia to
the FOCUS on Young Adults Program/Pathfinder International under
Cooperative Agreement No. CCP-A-00-96-90002-00.
The coordination and logistical assistance of Gladys Pozo,
Pathfinder/Bolivia, in carrying out the study is gratefully
acknowledged, as are the helpful comments of FOCUS on Young Adults
Program colleague Nancy Murray on an earlier version of this article.
Vaija Lipovsek, Ali Mehryar Karim, Emily Zielinski Gutierrez,
Robert J. Magnani, FOCUS on Young Adults Program, Tulane University Medical Center, School of Public Health & Tropical Medicine, New
Orleans, Louisiana; Maria del Carmen Castro Gomez, independent
consultant, La Paz, Bolivia.
Reprint requests to Varja Lipovsek, Tulane University Medical
Center, School of Public Health & Tropical Medicine, Department of
International Health & Development, 14-40 Canal Street, Suite 2200,
New Orleans, Louisiana 70112. Electronic mail may be sent to
vlipovs@tulane.edu.