GENDER DIFFERENCES IN PSYCHOLOGICAL WELL-BEING OF MEXICAN EARLY ADOLESCENTS.
Benjet, Corina ; Hernandez-Guzman, Laura
ABSTRACT
This study examined gender differences in the effects of menarche in females and voice change in males, specifically with regard to
depression, self-esteem, body image, and externalizing problems (i.e.,
behavioral disturbances). In addition, possible modifying variables
(relationship with parents, social-emotional adjustment, level of
parental education, and menstrual attitudes) were assessed. Participants
were 1,102 Mexican youths aged 9 to 14. Analyses indicated that there
were no gender differences among prepubertal youths in depression, body
image, or self-esteem, but prepubertal males had more externalizing
problems than did premenarcheal females. Females increased in
depression, externalizing problems, and negative body image
postmenarche, while males showed no change in depression, a trend toward
fewer externalizing problems, and felt better about their bodies
following voice change. Relationship with parents, social-emotional
adjustment, parental education, and menstrual attitudes did not modify
the r elation between menarche and body image or depression. Based on
these findings, suggestions for designing interventions aimed at early
adolescent females are presented.
Early adolescence is a time of rapid physical changes and new
social demands, which in turn have an impact on psychological
development. It is also during this transitional period that gender
differences in depression, self-esteem, and body image begin to emerge.
Despite findings indicating either no gender difference or a slight
propensity for greater depression in prepubescent males than females,
from middle adolescence through adulthood, depression becomes more
frequent in females (Katragadda & Tidwell, 1998; Petersen,
Sarigiani, & Kennedy, 1991). Numerous cross-cultural studies have
reported that there is a substantial gender difference in adult
depression, with an average female-to-male ratio of two to one (McGrath,
Keita, Strickland, & Russo, 1990). In early adolescence females also
begin to feel less satisfied with themselves and their bodies. For
example, Tobin-Richards, Boxer, and Petersen (1983) found that body
image became increasingly more negative with pubertal maturation for
females and more po sitive for males. Similarly, Mexican females
reported greater self-esteem than did males in third through fifth
grades, and then in sixth grade the self-esteem of females plummeted
below that of males, who simultaneously had a surge in self-esteem
(Verduzco Alvarez-Icaza, Lara-Cantu, Lancelotta, & Rubio, 1989).
Other researchers have found all three--depression, body image, and
self-esteem--to be worse in early adolescent females than males (Keel,
Fulkerson, & Leon, 1997).
Studies investigating precursors of eating disorders during the
transition from childhood to adolescence have pointed to an interaction
of gender, depression, self-esteem, and body image. For example, among
Chinese adolescents, females were found to have more negative body image
as compared to males, but they did not differ in terms of self-esteem
(Davis & Katzman, 1997). The relationship between body satisfaction
and depression was significant for females only. Veron-Guidry,
Williamson, and Netemeyer (1997) identified depression and low
self-esteem, among other variables, as risk factors for eating disorders
in prepubertal females. Kostanski and Gullone (1998) indicated that
dissatisfaction with body image in early adolescence was related to
gender, self-esteem, and depression.
What might account for the adverse consequences of physical changes
occurring during puberty? In particular, what role does pubertal
maturation play in undermining the emotional well-being of females?
There are several possible explanations. For example, females may face
greater challenges during puberty than do males. Females reach puberty,
on average, two years prior to males (Tanner, 1955), when they may be
emotionally and cognitively less prepared to handle such changes, and
when they face the simultaneous transition to middle school. Menarche,
in particular, is a concrete symbol of the passage from child to woman,
and has been shown, in studies with Anglo American females, to be
related to changes in self-concept and increased negative affect. Ruble
and Brooks-Gunn (1982) reason that because of the ties between
menstruation, femininity, and sexuality, more generalized aspects of a
girl's self-concept might be affected by menarche.
Further, female maturation is more visible than is male maturation.
While only the adolescent female (and those she chooses to tell) knows
her menarcheal status, breast development is noticeable to all. There is
also an increase in the percentage of body fat and a decrease in the
proportion of shoulder to waist width accompanying menarche, changes
that are incongruent with societal ideals for female beauty, whereas
males' bodies change in a direction that is more congruent with
societal standards for male attractiveness (i.e., they become larger and
stronger).
Finally, menarche, a symbol of the female adolescent's
emerging womanhood and sexuality, may stimulate an intensification of
gender socialization. Hill et al. (1985) have suggested that parents
allow adolescent sons more autonomy and try to increase their control of
daughters postpuberty. An increase in parental control at a time when
the adolescent female is seeking greater autonomy might undermine her
sense of competence.
The findings from empirical research into the impact of menarche on
females have been mixed. Koff, Rierdan, and Silverstone (1978) and
Rierdan and Koff (1980) found that postmenarcheal females were more
satisfied with feminine body parts (these researchers did not examine
overall body image and their measures were projective). Garwood and
Allen (1979) noted that postmenarcheal females had a more positive
overall self-concept than did premenarcheal females, though their scores
on the Satisfaction with Self subscale of the Tennessee Self-Concept
Scale were lower, and they reported having more problems. On the other
hand, Simmons, Burgeson, Carlton-Ford, and Blyth (1987), using a
longitudinal design, found self-esteem, as well as academic achievement
and involvement in extracurricular activities, to be adversely affected
by menarche (even more so if the adolescent simultaneously had to
confront multiple challenges).
Menarche, in combination with other factors, has been found to be
related to depression. Brooks-Gunn and Warren (1989) reported that
levels of depression rose during times of greatest hormonal increase,
but hormones explained 4% of the variance, whereas menarche and age at
menarche in combination with negative life events explained 17% of the
variance in depression above and beyond that attributable to hormonal
levels. Along the same lines, Brooks-Gunn (1988) and Rierdan and Koff
(1991) detected greater depression in females who experience menarche
early as compared to on-time females. Menarche, in conjunction with poor
ego development, has also been found to be associated with greater
depression (Rierdan & Koff, 1993). Patton et al. (1996), using a
multivariate model, reported that depression and anxiety in females were
positively correlated with year in school, however menarcheal status was
a stronger predictor. It also has been suggested that coping with
stressful life events exacerbates adolescent female s' depressive symptoms, especially when self-esteem is low (Hoffmann & Su, 1998).
The majority of these studies used a simple
premenarche-postmenarche paradigm. Yet, Hill et al. (1985) detected
increases in mother-daughter conflict six months postmenarche, a return
to premenarcheal levels one year postmenarche, and increases again more
than one year postmenarche. This illustrates the importance of
considering the time lapsed since menarche in order to understand its
precise impact.
Not all females experience such adverse consequences postmenarche
and thus it is important to explore variables that might either
exacerbate or buffer this transition. Risk factors might lie at a
contextual-societal level (e.g., parental education), at the level of
the parent-child dyad (e.g., the adolescent's perception of
parental control, affect, and communication), or at the individual level
(e.g., attitudes toward menstruation and the adolescent's
social-emotional adjustment). For example, adolescent females from less
educated families might have less access to information regarding
menstruation, thus affecting their ability to cope and their attitudes
toward menstruation (Cuevas Fournier, 1969). Further, Caspi and Moffitt
(1991) proposed that transitions such as puberty magnify preexisting individual differences rather than changing social-emotional adjustment.
They found that females with behavior problems in childhood and who
subsequently reached maturity early had greater difficulty adjusting to
ado lescence than did the early maturing females without preexisting
problems.
Programs that attempt to prevent psychological maladjustment in
adolescents require accurate information about associated variables and
the ways in which they interact to promote or hinder well-being. The
purpose of the present study was to replicate and clarify previous
findings regarding the impact of puberty (specifically menarche in
females and voice change in males) on the depression levels,
self-esteem, and body image of adolescents. It included the length of
time since puberty as a variable. It also explored whether there is a
similar process for males and females, that is, whether pubertal changes
also undermine males' emotional well-being. One of the possible
explanations for the greater postpubertal maladjustment found in females
than in males involves the variables chosen for analysis. Males'
maladjustment following pubertal changes may be manifested externally
(i.e., aggression and conduct disturbances), while females'
maladjustment postmenarche may be manifested internally (i.e.,
depression). Th is research tested that hypothesis by evaluating the
impact of menarche for females and voice change for males on aggression
and conduct problems as well as depression, self-esteem, and body image.
The final objective was to assess possible modifying variables, such as
relationship with parents, level of parental education, social-emotional
adjustment, and menstrual attitudes, to see whether they are related to
level of risk during this transitional time.
METHOD
Participants
The participants were 1,102 Mexican early adolescents, of whom 576
were female and 526 were male (38 had been eliminated from the sample
either for answering at least 10 items outside of the range of
possibilities or because of a nonserious attitude during the
administration of the measures). They were in the fifth (n = 241), sixth
(n = 321), and seventh (n = 540) grades at three private and four public
schools in Mexico City. The students ranged in age from 9 to 14 years,
with an average age of 11.8. Their pubertal status, divided into four
categories, is presented in Table 1.
Seventy-six percent of the participants lived in two-parent
families. The educational level of their parents was varied: 2% of the
fathers and 2% of the mothers had no formal education, 12% of the
fathers and 12% of the mothers had a primary school education, 16% of
the fathers and 18% of the mothers had a secondary school education, 2%
of the fathers and 5% of the mothers had received vocational training,
8% of the fathers and 13% of the mothers had a high school education,
and 60% of the fathers and 50% of the mothers reported a university
education. Family socioeconomic level was also varied: 2% of the fathers
and 46% of the mothers were unemployed, 16% of the fathers and 6% of the
mothers were unskilled laborers, 12% of the fathers and 6% of the
mothers were skilled laborers, 14% of the fathers and 6% of the mothers
were business owners, 2% of the fathers and 7% of the mothers were
teachers, and 54% of the fathers and 29% of the mothers were
professionals.
Procedure
The principal of each school, and in some cases other school
administrators as well, reviewed and agreed to participate in the
project. Informed consent was obtained from each student, with only a
few who did not wish to participate. The students were told of the study
in their classrooms and were asked to respond to the questionnaire
during class time. Results of the study were later provided to each
school.
Measures
Pubertal status. For females, two items assessed whether they had
experienced menarche and at what age. Self-reports of menarcheal status
have been shown to be fairly accurate over time (Brooks-Gunn, Warren,
Rosso, & Gargiulo, 1987). For males, four items assessed whether
their voice had deepened and the growth of pubic hair had begun and at
what age. Voice change in males occurs in the same stages of pubertal
development as menarche in females (i.e., Tanner stages three and four).
In the final analyses, voice change alone was employed as the measure of
pubertal maturation in males due to its being a relatively more concrete
event than pubic hair growth and thus easier for the males to locate in
time.
Depression. Depression was measured using a Spanish language version of the Center for Epidemiological Studies Depression Scale
(CESD; Radloff, 1977). The Spanish version has been validated for
Mexican early adolescents (Benjet, Hernandez-Guzman,
Tercero-Quintanilla, Hernandez-Roque, & Chartt-Leon, 1999). Twenty
items assess the frequency of depressive symptomatology in the previous
week, including depressed affect, feelings of guilt and worthlessness,
psychomotor retardation, loss of appetite, and sleep difficulties.
Internal consistency for this sample was good (Cronbach's alpha =
.85).
Self-esteem. A shortened Spanish language version of the Five-Scale
Test of Self-Esteem for Children (Pope, McHale, & Craighead,
1988)--translated via a backward translation method and reduced via
factor analysis by Hernandez-Guzman and Caso-Niebla (1996)--was employed
to measure self-esteem. It assesses self-worth in five domains: global,
academic, body, family, and social. Internal consistency = .82.
Body image. A combination of the Body scale of the Five-Scale Test
of Self Esteem for Children and the Body Image scale of the Self-Image
Questionnaire for Young Adolescents (Petersen, 1980) was translated into
Spanish via a backward translation method for use in this study. The
items assess satisfaction with one's body, attractiveness, weight,
and physical condition. Internal consistency = .94.
Social-emotional adjustment. Social-emotional adjustment was
measured using the Psychological Maladaptive Problems scale of the
Health, Lifestyles and Behavior Inventory (Sanchez-Sosa &
Hernandez-Guzman, 1991). Developed for the Mexican population, it
assesses various maladaptive difficulties: anxiety, somatization,
motoric retardation, conduct disturbances, and aggression.
Cronbach's alpha = .92.
Relationship with parents. The adolescents' perception of
parents' degree of control was evaluated using the Relation with
Mother and Relation with Father subscales of the Drug Risk Scale
(Climent, Aragon, & Plutchick, 1989). Each subscale consists of 15
items (an additional item was incorporated) that assess how frequently
each parent relates with the adolescent in various ways. Three factors
have emerged for Mexican adolescents (Gonzalez-Forteza, 1992): affect,
communication, and control, with internal consistencies ranging between
.89 and .93 (Gonzalez-Forteza, 1996). For this sample, a factor
reflecting communication regarding sexuality emerged.
Attitudes toward menarche and menstruation. In order to measure
attitudes toward menarche and menstruation, a Likert-type scale was
developed based on a focus group with early adolescent females, items
from the Menstrual Attitudes Questionnaire (Brooks-Gunn & Ruble,
1980), and items from the Attitudes Questionnaire (Morelos Flores,
1994). Seventeen items assess feelings of pride, shame, and fear
associated with menarche, and perceptions of menstruation as natural,
bothersome, or shameful. A positive score reflects positive attitudes.
Cronbach's alpha = .55.
Sociodemographics. Eight items assessed the marital status,
educational level, and type of employment of parents, family size, and
birth order of the adolescent.
RESULTS
Overall Gender Differences
Gender differences were explored using two-tailed t-tests for
independent samples. Females reported greater depressive symptomatology
(t = 3.2, p = .001), a more negative body image (t = 4.6, p = .000), a
more positive relationship with mother (t = 3.9, p = .000), and a less
positive relationship with father (t = 2.5, p = .011) than did males.
There were no significant differences between females and males in terms
of self-esteem or social-emotional adjustment. However, males reported
greater maladjustment in the areas of conduct disturbances (t = 2.9, p =
.004), aggression (t = 4.5, p = .000), psychomotor retardation (t = 2.8,
p = .005), and somatization (t = 3.0, p = .003), while females reported
greater maladjustment in regard to anxiety (t = 4.1, p = .000).
Pubertal Status and Gender Effects
Separate ANCOVAs were conducted to explore the effect of pubertal
status (pre-post) and gender (female-male), covarying for age, on each
of the dependent variables (depression, body image, self-esteem, and
externalizing problems). The changes in the dependent variables were
then plotted against time since puberty (prepubertal, less than six
months postpubertal, between six and twelve months postpubertal, and
more than one year postpubertal) for females and males.
Depression. There was a significant effect for pubertal status,
F(1, 1090) = 9.0, p [less than] .01, gender, F(1, 1092) = 12.0, p =
.001, and their interaction, F(1, 1092) = 7.0, p [less than] .01. There
was no effect for age. Comparing the means, using the Bonferroni
technique, showed that females' depressive symptomatology increased
from premenarche to postmenarche (p [less than] .001), but that
depression did not vary by pubertal status for males. Figure 1
illustrates how levels of depression varied with time since puberty for
males and females.
Body image. Though there was no significant main effect for
pubertal status, there was a main effect for gender, F(1, 1098) = 18.1,
p [less than] .001, and a significant interaction between pubertal
status and gender, F(1, 1098) = 8.7, p [less than] .01. There was no
effect for age. Gender differences in body image emerged after puberty,
when females had a more negative body image in comparison to males (p
[less than].001), whereas there was no difference between prepubertal
females and males. Figure 2 shows how body image varied with time since
puberty for females and males.
Self-esteem. Neither pubertal status nor gender demonstrated
significant main effects, though age did, F(1, 1096) = 4.2, p = .04, and
the interaction between pubertal status and gender was marginally
significant, F(1, 1096) = 3.5, p = .06. Females marginally, though not
significantly, decreased in self-esteem, and males marginally, though
not significantly, increased in self-esteem. Figure 3 shows that the
gender differences in self-esteem emerged more than one year after
puberty.
Externalizing problems (behavioral disturbances). First, aggression
and conduct disturbance scores were combined. There was a marginal main
effect for pubertal status, F(1, 1037) = 3.2, p = .07, a main effect for
gender, F(1, 1037) = 18.3, p = .000, and an interaction between the two,
F(1, 1037) = 6.0, p = .01. While prepubertal females were better
adjusted with respect to externalizing problems than were prepubertal
males (p = .000), their adjustment declined (to a marginally significant
degree) from premenarche to postmenarche (p = .07). There was no
significant change in externalizing problems for males. Figure 4
illustrates how adjustment varied with time since puberty for females
and males.
Possible Modifying Variables
Menstrual attitudes, relationship with parents, parental level of
education, and social-emotional adjustment were converted into
categorical variables and entered, along with gender and pubertal
status, in ANOVAs for each of the dependent variables. No significant
interactions were found, although there were many significant main
effects. Thus, regression equations were calculated for depression,
self-esteem, and body image.
Regression analyses for depression. A simultaneous multiple
regression including pubertal status, social-emotional adjustment,
menstrual attitudes, maternal and paternal educational levels, and
relation with mother and father (specifically, control, affect and
communication regarding sexuality) was conducted for depression in
females. Pubertal status was entered as a continuous variable using time
since puberty (prepubertal, less than six months postpubertal, between
six and twelve months postpubertal, and more than one year
postpubertal). These variables explained 35% of the variance in
depressive symptomatology (R = .60, [R.sup.2] = .35, p [less than]
.001). The variables that significantly predicted depression were
menarcheal status ([beta] = .12, p [less than] .00), social-emotional
adjustment ([beta] = - .40, p [less than] .00), menstrual attitudes
([beta] = -.07, p [less than] .02), maternal affect ([beta] = -.10, p =
.05), maternal communication regarding sexuality ([beta] = .18, p [less
than] .00), a nd, marginally, paternal control ([beta] =.09, p = .06).
A similar regression analysis was conducted for males (menstrual
attitudes was excluded). Pubertal status, social-emotional adjustment,
and paternal and maternal education, control, affect, and communication
regarding sexuality explained 23% of the variance in depression for
males (R = .48, [R.sup.2] = .23, p [less than] .001), considerably less
than that for females. Only social-emotional adjustment significantly
predicted depression ([beta] = -.38, p [less than] .00), while paternal
affect was marginally significant ([beta] = -.10, p = .07).
Regression analyses for body image. As with depression, a
simultaneous multiple regression including pubertal status,
social-emotional adjustment, menstrual attitudes, maternal and paternal
educational levels, and relation with mother and with father (control,
affect, and communication regarding sexuality) was conducted for body
image in females. These variables explained 22% of the variance in body
image (R = .47, [R.sup.2] = .22, p [less than] .001). The variables that
contributed significantly to body image scores were social-emotional
adjustment ([beta] = .18, p [less than] .00), menstrual attitudes
([beta] = .16, p [less than] .00), maternal educational level ([beta] =
.16, p [less than] .01), and maternal affect ([beta] = .19, p [less
than].01).
A similar multiple regression analysis was conducted for males
(with the exception of menstrual attitudes). The independent variables
explained 20% of the variance in body image for males (R = .45,
[R.sup.2] = .20). The variables that contributed significantly to body
image scores were social-emotional adjustment ([beta] = .25, p [less
than] .001) and paternal affect ([beta] = .18, p [less than] .01).
Regression analyses for self-esteem. A simultaneous multiple
regression to explore the contributions of pubertal status,
social-emotional adjustment, menstrual attitudes, and paternal and
maternal educational level, control, affect, and communication regarding
sexuality revealed that these variables accounted for 39% of the
variance in females' self-esteem (R = .62, [R.sup.2] = .39, p [less
than] .001). The variables that significantly predicted self-esteem for
females were social-emotional adjustment ([beta] = .32, p [less than]
.001), maternal educational level ([beta] = .13, p [less than] .03),
menstrual attitudes ([beta] = .09, p [less than] .02), maternal affect
([beta] = .25, p [less than] .001), paternal control ([beta] = -.11, p
[less than] .03), and paternal affect ([beta] = .11, p = .03).
A similar multiple regression was computed for males (without the
menstrual attitudes variable). The independent variables accounted for
31% of the variance in self-esteem scores for males (R = .56, [R.sup.2]
= .31, p [less than] .001). The variables that significantly predicted
self-esteem for males were social-emotional adjustment ([beta] = .25, p
[less than] .001) and paternal affect ([beta] = .29, p [less than]
.001), and maternal control was marginally significant ([beta] = -.09, p
[less than] .07).
DISCUSSION
Results from this investigation of the changes over time in
depression, body image, self-esteem, and externalizing problems
contribute to our understanding of the impact of puberty. The findings
replicate those of previous studies, namely that menarche does have an
adverse effect on adolescent females, above and beyond the effect of
age, in terms of greater depression and, marginally, a more negative
body image. While prepubertal females and males reported roughly the
same degree of depressive symptomatology, postpubertal females gradually
and steadily increased in depression while males remained approximately
the same. Females' body image showed a minimal but steady decline,
while males felt better about their bodies immediately following voice
change, suffered a slight drop between six and twelve months after voice
change, and then rebounded more than a year post-puberty.
Previous research on the effects of puberty has mainly reported
increases in female adolescents' depression and negative body
image. However, the present investigation also found a relationship
between menarche and externalizing problems. Postmenarcheal females
became more aggressive and reported more conduct disturbances, equaling
that of males.
The results did not confirm the changes in self-esteem that had
previously been reported in several studies, but they did agree with
findings by Davis and Katzman (1997). In the present study, self-esteem
varied, though ever so slightly, with age rather than pubertal status.
Small gender differences in self-esteem were observed more than one year
after puberty.
Overall, it was found that the increase in depressive
symptomatology, externalizing problems, and negative body image of
females is gradual and linear, and becomes particularly evident six
months postmenarche. In the present study, almost 26% of the females
between six and twelve months postmenarche could be classified as
depressed (using one standard deviation above the mean on the CES-D as
the cutoff criterion), whereas only 14% of the males six to twelve
months postpuberty could be similarly classified. In other words,
menarche is the point at which females' well-being begins to
plummet in comparison to males, and it continues to do so more than a
year postmenarche.
Though the difference in depression between pre- and postmenarcheal
females was considerable (one-third standard deviation), the difference
in body image was minimal. The small effect of menarche on body image
may be due to different evaluations of the various pubertal changes. For
example, the rapid increase in body fat might be evaluated negatively by
the adolescent female, whereas breast growth might be seen as a
desirable occurrence. In fact, the mixed results of the studies in this
area might be a reflection of the different physical changes in puberty,
some of which are congruent and others incongruent with social standards
of beauty. Overall, however, this study suggests that the tendency is
toward a more negative general body image. Future research should
examine which aspects of body image are influenced by specific pubertal
changes.
The findings of this study also suggest the absence of a parallel
process for males in which pubertal maturation undermines their
well-being. Whereas prepubertal females and males did not differ in
terms of depression, body image, or self-esteem, postpubertal females
increased in depressive symptomatology and had a less positive body
image postmenarche, and males reported less depression and a more
positive body image. Concerning behavior problems, postpubertal males,
in contrast to females, demonstrated a slight tendency toward better
adjustment.
Several possible explanations are offered for the failure to find a
significant relationship between pubertal status and depression,
externalizing problems, or self-esteem for males. Pubertal status may
not have the same adverse consequences for males as for females. Females
face greater challenges in puberty, at a younger age, compared with
males. These more abrupt and concrete changes are not necessarily
received positively. Another explanation involves the measurement of
puberty itself. Voice change, which was chosen because it occurs at a
similar stage of development as menarche and because of its relative
ease of identification, may be a more positive event for males compared
with other physical changes. In addition, there is undoubtedly less
precision in the measurement of puberty in males than in females
(menarche). Males may also have been more likely to overestimate pubertal maturation. There was almost the same percentage of
postpubertal males as females in the sample. Given that there was no age
d ifference between females and males, and females mature roughly two
years earlier than males, proportionally more postpubertal females than
postpubertal males would have been expected.
This study also explored possible risk factors: social-emotional
adjustment, parental level of education, menstrual attitudes, and
relationship with parents (perceived control, affect, and communication
regarding sexuality). No interaction effects were found; none of the
variables appeared to modify the relation between menarche and
depression or body image, suggesting that this transition is uniformly
difficult for females. However, there were main effects of some of these
variables for depression, self-esteem, and body image. For example, the
females with the highest levels of depression had been postpubertal
longer, were more poorly adjusted, had more negative menstrual
attitudes, and perceived less positive maternal affect, greater paternal
control, and more maternal communication regarding sexuality. Those with
the most positive body image were better adjusted, expressed more
positive menstrual attitudes, had a more highly educated mother, and
perceived more positive maternal affect. Finally, those with the highest
self-esteem were better adjusted, had more positive menstrual attitudes,
were younger, had a more educated mother, and perceived more positive
maternal affect, more positive paternal affect, and less paternal
control.
In the case of males, greater adjustment and more positive paternal
affect contributed to less depressive symptomatology. Being more
socially and emotionally adjusted and perceiving more positive paternal
affect predicted a more positive body image. Being better adjusted and
perceiving more positive affect from father and less control from mother
predicted more positive self-esteem in males.
It is interesting that, in terms of psychological well-being,
perceived affect from the same-sex parent and perceived control from the
opposite-sex parent were significant aspects of the parent-adolescent
relationship. Further research should more thoroughly test these
relationships.
Greater communication with regard to sexuality contributed to
higher levels of depression among females. It is conjectured that the
items dealing with this variable in reality assess control over
sexuality instead of communication (for example, "Does your
mother/father tell you that you should wait until you're an adult
to have sexual relations?").
Overall, these results suggest that early adolescence is a
particularly difficult time for females and that the physical changes
during puberty do not similarly undermine the well-being of males. Most
research has been conducted with Anglo American youths, and the present
study confirmed many of the previous findings using a sample of Mexican
early adolescents. Future research in this area would be greatly
enhanced by the use of longitudinal methods, following the same females
before and after their pubertal changes, with measurements at several
points in time, ideally into early adulthood in order to examine how
lasting the effects are. In addition, future gender comparisons should
explore variables specific to males' response to this transition,
including more precise measures of pubertal status. These comparisons
will have to take into account psychological and social variables, as
well as the effects of hormonal differences.
The results of the present study point to possible directions for
the design and implementation of prevention and intervention programs.
Such programs, aimed at both preadolescent and adolescent females and
their parents, should address mother-daughter and father-daughter
relationships, specifically paternal/maternal demonstrations of affect
and less controlling parenting practices. Overall social-emotional
adjustment, which has previously been found to be associated with
parent-child relationships (Dumka, Roosa, & Jackson, 1997; Shaw,
Keenan, Vondra, Delliquadri, & Giovanelli, 1997; Rapee, 1997; Sato
et al., 1997; Taris & Bok, 1996), is also an important factor.
Further longitudinal research is necessary to determine how best to
improve adjustment prior to menarche in order to make the transition
through puberty less challenging.
Coping strategies also should be investigated and incorporated into
prevention and intervention programs. Coping strategies have been found
to distinguish between depressed and nondepressed adolescent females
(Bouteyre, 1997). Broderick (1998) studied the coping styles of early
adolescents and reported that females' coping responses were more
ruminative and self-focused compared to those of males. Avoidance as a
form of coping has been found to be related to anxiety and depression
(Gomez, 1998). Satija, Advani, and Nathawat (1997) documented more
avoidance and the use of fewer problem solving strategies in depressed
subjects. Thus, future research should explore whether interventions
aimed at improving social skills, coping strategies, and assertiveness
might prevent early adolescent females from experiencing increases in
depression and negative body image. Finding ways to ease the pubertal
transition in early adolescence may help keep more serious and related
disturbances from arising in later adolescence a nd adulthood, such as
clinical depression, anorexia and bulimia, and sub stance abuse.
This research was supported in part by the Program for Support to
Research and Innovative Technology Projects--National University of
Mexico. The authors gratefully acknowledge the assistance of Raquel
Tinajero in collecting and entering the data, and Emilio Sacristan in
processing the data.
Laura Hernandez-Guzman, School of Psychology, National Autonomous
University of Mexico.
Reprint requests to Coma Benjet, Division of Epidemiological and
Psychosocial Research, National Institute of Psychiatry, Calzada
Mexico-Xochimilco 101, Mexico City, 14370 Mexico. Electronic mail may be
sent to cbenjet@imp.edu.mx.
REFERENCES
Benjet, C., Hernandez-Guzman, L., Tercero-Quintanilla, G.
Hernandez-Roque, A., & Chartt-Leon, R. (1999). Validity and
reliability of the CES-D in children and early adolescents. Revista
Mexicana de Psicologia, 16(1), 175-185.
Bouteyre, E. (1997). Strategies of coping, depression levels and
school performance in children of immigrants. Psychologie Francaise,
42(3), 211-216.
Broderick, P. C. (1998). Early adolescent gender differences in the
use of ruminative and distracting coping strategies. Journal of Early
Adolescence, 18(2), 173-191.
Brooks-Gunn, J. (1988). Antecedents and consequences of variations
in girls' maturational timing. Journal of Adolescent Health Care,
9, 365-373.
Brooks-Gunn, J., & Ruble, D. (1980). Menstrual Attitude
Questionnaire. Psychosomatic Medicine, 42, 504-512.
Brooks-Gunn, J., & Warren, M. (1989). Biological and social
contributions to negative affect in young adolescent girls. Child
Development, 60, 40-55.
Brooks-Gunn, J., Warren, M., Rosso, J., & Gargiulo, J. (1987).
Validity of self-report measures of girls' pubertal status. Child
Development, 58, 829-841.
Caspi, A., & Moffitt, T. (1991). Individual differences are
accentuated during periods of social change: The sample case of girls at
puberty. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 61(1), 157-168.
Climent, C., Aragon, L., & Plutchick, R. (1989). Prediction of
risk for drug use in high school students. Boletin de la Oficina
Sanitaria Panamericana, 107(6), 568-576.
Cuevas Fournier, L. E. (1969). Psychological aspects of menarche.
Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Universidad Nacional Autonoma de
Mexico, Mexico City, Mexico.
Davis, C., & Katzman, M. (1997). Charting new territory: Body
esteem, weight satisfaction, depression, and self-esteem among Chinese
males and females in Hong Kong. Sex Roles, 36(7-8), 449-459.
Dumka, L. E., Roosa, M. W., & Jackson, K. M. (1997). Risk,
conflict, mothers' parenting, and children's adjustment in
low-income, Mexican immigrant, and Mexican American families. Journal of
Marriage and the Family 59(2), 309-323.
Garwood, G., & Allen, L. (1979). Self-concept and identified
problem differences between pre- and postmenarcheal adolescents. Journal
of Clinical Psychology, 35(3), 528-537.
Gomez, R. (1998). Locus of control and avoidant coping: Direct,
interactional and mediational effects on maladjustment in adolescents.
Personality and Individual Differences, 24(3), 325-334.
Gonzalez-Forteza, C. (1992). Psychosocial stress and coping
mechanisms: The impact on the emotional state of adolescents.
Unpublished master's thesis, Universidad Nacional Autonoma de
Mexico, Mexico City, Mexico.
Gonzalez-Forteza, C. (1996). Buffering and risk factors for
depression and suicide attempts in adolescents. Unpublished doctoral
dissertation, Universidad Nacional Autonoma de Mexico, Mexico City,
Mexico.
Hernandez-Guzman, L., & Caso Niebla, J. (1996). Does
child-rearing style influence child self-esteem? Presentation at the
annual meeting of the National Council on Teaching and Research in
Psychology, Mexicali, Mexico.
Hill, J. P., Holmbeck, G., Marlow, L., Green, T., & Lynch, M.
E. (1985). Menarcheal status and parent-child relations in families of
seventh-grade girls. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 14(4), 301-316.
Hoffmann, J. P., & Su, S. (1998). Stressful life events and
adolescent substance use and depression: Conditional and gender
differentiated effects. Substance Use and Misuse, 33(11), 2219-2262.
Katragadda, C. P., & Tidwell, R. (1998). Rural Hispanic
adolescents at risk for depressive symptoms. Journal of Applied Social
Psychology, 28(20), 1916-1930.
Keel, P. K., Fulkerson, J. A., & Leon, G. R. (1997). Disordered
eating precursors in pre- and early adolescent girls and boys. Journal
of Youth and Adolescence, 26(2), 203-216.
Koff, E., Rierdan, J., & Silverstone, E. (1978). Changes in
representation of body image as a function of menarcheal status.
Developmental Psychology, 14, 635-642.
Kostanski, M., & Gullone, E. (1998). Adolescent body image
dissatisfaction: Relationship with self-esteem, anxiety, and depression
controlling for body mass. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry
and Allied Disciplines, 39(2), 255-262.
McBroom, W. (1981). Parental relationships, socioeconomic status,
and sex role expectations. Sex Roles, 7, 1027-1033.
McGrath, E., Keita, P. G., Strickland, B., & Russo, N. (1990).
Women and depression: Risk factors and treatment issues. Washington, DC:
American Psychological Association.
Morelos Flores, M. (1994). Attitudes and knowledge regarding the
feminine menstrual cycle of premenopausal women and girls beginning to
menstruate. Unpublished bachelor's thesis, Universidad Nacional
Autonoma de Mexico, Mexico City, Mexico.
Patton, G. C., Hibbert, M. E., Carlin, J., Shao, Q., Rosier, M.,
Caust, J., & Bowes, G. (1996). Menarche and the onset of depression
and anxiety in Victoria, Australia. Journal of Epidemiology and
Community Health, 50(6), 661-666.
Petersen, A. (1980). The Self-Image Questionnaire for Young
Adolescents. Chicago, IL: Laboratory for the Study of Adolescence,
Michael Reese Hospital and Medical Center.
Petersen, A., Sarigiani, P., & Kennedy, R. (1991). Adolescent
depression: Why more girls? Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 20(2),
247-271.
Pope, A., McHale, S., & Craighead, W. E. (1988). Self esteem
enhancement with children and adolescents. Boston, MA: Allyn and Bacon.
Radloff, L. (1977). The CES-D Scale: A self-report depression scale
for research in the general population. Applied Psychological
Measurement, 1, 385-401.
Rapee, R. M. (1997). Potential role of childrearing practices in
the development of anxiety and depression. Clinical Psychology Review,
17(1), 47-67.
Rierdan, J., & Koff, E. (1980). The psychological impact of
menarche: Integrative versus disruptive changes. Journal of Youth and
Adolescence, 9(1), 49-58.
Rierdan, J., & Koff, E. (1991). Depressive symptomatology among
very early maturing girls. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 20,
415-425.
Rierdan, J., & Koff, E. (1993). Developmental variables in
relation to depressive symptoms in adolescent girls. Development and
Psychopathology, 5, 485-496.
Ruble, D., & Brooks-Gunn, J. (1982). The experience of
menarche. Child Development, 53, 1557-1566.
Sanchez-Sosa, J., & Hernandez-Guzman, L. (1993). Health,
Lifestyles and Behavior Inventory, Third Edition. Mexico City, Mexico:
Universidad Nacional Autonoma de Mexico.
Satija, K., Advani, G. B., & Nathawat, S. S. (1997). Is
depression a result of faulty coping strategies? Indian Journal of
Clinical Psychology, 24(1), 65-69.
Sato, T., Uehara, T., Sakado, K, Nishioka, K, Ozaki, N., Nakamura,
M., & Kasahara, Y. (1997). Dysfunctional parenting and a lifetime
history of depression in a volunteer sample of Japanese workers. Acta
Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 96(4), 306-310.
Shaw, D. S., Keenan, K., Vondra, J. L., Delliquadri, E., &
Giovannelli, J. (1997). Antecedents of preschool children internalizing
problems: A longitudinal study of low-income families. Journal of the
American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 36(12), 1760-1767.
Simmons, R., Burgeson, R., Carlton-Ford, S., & Blyth, D.
(1987). The impact of cumulative change in early adolescence. Child
Development, 58, 1220-1234.
Tanner, J. M. (1955). Growth at adolescence. Oxford, England:
Blackwell Scientific.
Taris, T. W., & Bok, I. A. (1996). Effects of parenting style
upon psychological well-being of young adults: Exploring the relations
among parental care, locus of control and depression. Early Child
Development and Care, 132, 9-104.
Tobin-Richards, M., Boxer, A., & Petersen, A. (1983). The
psychological significance of pubertal change: Sex differences in
perceptions of self during early adolescence. In J. Brooks-Gunn & A.
Petersen (Eds.), Girls at puberty: Biological and psychological
perspectives. New York: Plenum Press.
Verduzco Alvarez-Icaza, M. A., Lara-Cantu, M. C., Lancelotta, G.,
& Rubio, S. (1989). A study of self-esteem in school-aged children:
Normative data. Salud Mental, 12(3), 50-54.
Veron-Guidry, S., Williamson, D. A., & Netemeyer, R. G. (1997).
Structural modeling analysis of body dysphoria and eating disorder symptoms in preadolescent girls. Eating Disorders: The Journal of
Treatment and Prevention, 5(1), 15-27.
Table 1. Distribution of Pubertal Status by Sex
Pubertal Status Females Males
Prepubertal 319 (55.7%) 259 (49.8%)
Less than 6 months postpubertal 89 (15.5%) 156 (30.0%)
Between 6 and 12 months postpubertal 73 (12.7%) 44 (8.5%)
More than 1 year postpubertal 92 (16.1%) 61 (11.7%)
[Graph omitted]
[Graph omitted]
[Graph omitted]
[Graph omitted]