TARGETING ADOLESCENT MOTHERS WITH DEPRESSIVE SYMPTOMS FOR EARLY INTERVENTION.
Field, Tiffany ; Pickens, Jeffrey ; Prodromidis, Margarita 等
ABSTRACT
Infants of mothers with depressive symptoms show developmental
delays if symptoms persist over the first 6 months of the infant's
life, thus highlighting the importance of identifying those mothers for
early intervention. In Study 1, mothers with depressive symptoms (n =
160) and mothers without depressive symptoms (n = 100) and their infants
were monitored to identify variables from the first 3 months that
predict which mothers would still be symptomatic at 6 months. A
"dysregulation" profile was noted for the infants of depressed
mothers, including lower Brazelton scores, more indeterminate sleep, and
elevated norepinepbrine, epinephrine, and dopamine levels at the
neonatal period, and greater right frontal EEG activation, lower vagal tone, and negative interactions at the 3- and 6-month periods. A group
of maternal variables from the neonatal and 3-month assessments
accounted for 51% of the variance in the mothers' continuing
depressive symptoms. These variables included greater right frontal EEG
activatio n, lower vagal tone, and less positive interactions at 3
months, and elevated norepinephrine, serotonin, and cortisol levels at
the neonatal stage. In Study 2, a similar sample of mothers with
depressive symptoms (n = 160) and without depressive symptoms (n = 100)
was recruited and followed to 3 months. Those symptomatic mothers who
had values above (or below) the median (depending on the negative
direction) on the predictor variables identified in Study 1 (taken from
the first 3 months) were then randomly assigned to an intervention or a
control group at 3 months. These groups were then compared with each
other, as well as with the group without depressive symptoms, at 6 and
12 months. The intervention, conducted from 3 to 6 months, consisted of
free day care for the infants and a rehab program (social, educational,
and vocational) plus several mood induction interventions for the
mothers, including relaxation therapy, music mood induction, massage
therapy, and mother-infant interaction coaching. Although th e mothers
who received the intervention continued to have more depressive symptoms
than did the nondepressed mothers, their interactions significantly
improved and their biochemical values and vagal tone normalized. Their
infants also showed more positive interations, better growth, fewer
pediatric complications, and normalized biochemical values, and by 12
months their mental and motor scores were better than those of the
infants in the control group.
Longitudinal studies on mothers with depressive symptoms have
examined various kinds of depression, such as postpartum depression,
dysthymia, and major depressive disorder, and various degrees of
chronicity (Campbell, Cohn, & Meyers, 1995; Field, Healy, Goldstein,
& Guthertz, 1990; Lyons-Ruth, Zoll, Connell, & Grunebaum, 1986;
Murray, 1992). Irrespective of the type of depression, mothers'
depressive mood states appear to affect infants' development
negatively (Beardslee, Bemporad, Keller, & Klerman, 1983; Field,
1984; Orvaschel, 1983; Zuckerman & Beardslee, 1987). Findings
indicate that infants and children of depressed mothers are more likely
to have problems, including sleep disorders, accidents, growth failure,
and psychosomatic complaints. For example, in a prospective study,
Radke-Yarrow, Cummings, Kuzynski, and Chapman (1985) noted that
clinically depressed mothers displayed very little affection, and their
toddlers showed greater sadness, talked less often, and engaged in less
exploratory behavior.
The typical paradigm has been to study the face-to-face
interactions of depressed mothers and their infants. In this context,
depressed mothers have been described as having flat affect or depressed
mood, and as being less vocal and less responsive to their infants
(Cohn, Campbell, Matias, & Hopkins, 1990; Field, Healy, Goldstein,
& Guthertz, 1990). This, in turn, may affect their infants'
language development, problem-solving ability, mastery motivation, and
social competence.
The type of depression may be less critical than whether the mother
has chronic depressive symptoms (Campbell et al., 1995; Sameroff &
Seifer, 1983). Most studies on postpartum depression have found that the
majority of mothers experience remission in the first few months after
childbirth. However, Lyons-Ruth et al. (1986) reported that only 16%
experienced remission in the first year, and Field et al. (1990) found
that only 20% of the mothers who had depressive symptoms when their
infants were 3 months old were symptom-free when they were 6 months old.
Further, Field (1992) noted that infants whose mothers' depression
went into remission showed significant improvement in their
interactional behavior and developmental performance. In contrast,
infants whose mothers still had symptoms when they were 6 months old
showed developmental delays at 12 months. Because intervention is
probably needed most by mothers who have chronic symptoms (Campbell et
al., 1995), it is important to find measures that can identify those
mothers.
Behavioral remission (e.g., the abatement of such things as slow
speech, slouched posture, and a slow gait) is often noted even when a
person continues to report depressed feelings. On the other hand, some
physiological variables appear to be stable even when behavioral
symptoms are not present. For example, Henriques and Davidson (1990)
have reported that depressed subjects show greater relative right
frontal EEG activation, which remains after they go into remission and
no longer show behavioral symptoms, suggesting that this might be a
physiological characteristic of chronic depression. Similarly, Field,
Fox, Pickens, Nawrocki, and Soutulla (1995) documented greater relative
right frontal EEG activation in chronically depressed mothers and their
3-month-old infants. Heart rate is another potential predictor variable,
inasmuch as high, stable resting heart rate has been reported for
chronically depressed subjects. For example, Field, Pickens, Fox,
Nawrocki, and Gonzalez (1995) found lower vagal tone (and h igher heart
rate) in mothers with chronic depressive symptoms and their infants.
Field (1995) also documented elevated cortisol and catecholamines in
mothers with chronic depression.
The purpose of the following two studies was, first, to identify
variables from the first 3 months of an infant's life that predict
the mother's continuing depression at 6 months (Study 1). Second,
in Study 2, those predictor variables were used to identify a sample of
high-risk mothers, and the effectiveness of an intervention program was
tested.
STUDY 1
Study 1 was designed to identify variables that predict which
mothers would show continuing depressive symptoms at 6 months
postdelivery. Behavioral, psychophysiological, and biochemical
assessments were made at the neonatal, 3-month, and 6-month periods.
Variables from the neonatal and 3-month assessments were entered into a
regression model to determine their contribution to the variance in the
mothers' depressive symptoms at 6 months.
METHOD
Sample
The sample consisted of 260 mothers (70% of them adolescents) of
normal, full-term infants. Approximately 63% were African American, 22%
Hispanic, and 15% non-Hispanic White. They were recruited at the
hospital postdelivery (mothers of preterm infants and infants with
prenatal complications were excluded). The mothers, on average, were
17.8 years old, had 10.3 years of education, and their socioeconomic
status was 4.4 on the Hollingshead Index. Adolescent and young adult
mothers were recruited because it was thought that they would be more
compliant with the intervention if they were still in high school and if
the intervention and day-care program were situated in their high
school. In addition, it was felt that the intervention might have more
impact with younger women by helping them complete high school and begin
career training. They were not breast-feeding their infants (as is
typical of this population). The mothers were the primary caregivers
(based on their reported plan to be the ones spending the greatest
number of hours per day with their infants). The refusal rate was
extremely low (7 mothers).
The Beck Depression Inventory (BDI) was used to classify the
mothers according to depressive symptoms (dysphoria). While not
providing a diagnosis of depression, self-report measures such as the
BDI are typically used when the symptom of dysphoria is of primary
interest (Seifer, 1995). Further, such dimensional measures, using
continuous scales, typically offer more information and have better
psychometric properties, making them more powerful in statistical tests
(Cohen, 1990). Although dysphoric mood is only one of the symptoms in
the depression complex, it is the primary symptom and the one thought to
affect infants most. As Seifer (1995) has pointed out, another advantage
of a single-symptom measure is that "it defines more precisely an
individual characteristic that may be implicated in poor developmental
outcomes" (p. 421). Thus, mothers with BDI scores greater than 12
(the cutoff for depression in most research protocols) were assigned to
the depressed group (n = 160). Mothers with scores less than 9 on the
BDI (and matched on socioeconomic status and age) were assigned to the
nondepressed group (n = 100). Mothers with scores in the 9-12 range were
not included, in order to ensure a large enough difference between the
nondepressed and depressed groups. Others have noted that only a small
proportion of women with depression seek treatment (Leaf, Bruce,
Tischler Freeman, Weisman, & Myers, 1988; O'Hara, Zekoski,
Philipps & Wright, 1990), and this held true for the mothers in the
present study (not one was in treatment or taking antidepressant
medication).
Neonatal Assessment
The mothers were interviewed, the infants were assessed, and
mother-infant interactions were observed in the mothers' hospital
rooms. Brazelton assessments, sleep/wake behavior, and urine sampling
were conducted by a research associate who was blind to the group status
of the subjects. Medical records were reviewed for standard birth
measures (gestational age, birth weight, birth length, head
circumference, and 5-minute Apgar scores). In addition, the ponderal
index (ratio of weight to length, providing a measure of intrauterine growth deprivation) and the Obstetric and Postnatal Complications Scales
(Littman & Parmelee, 1978) were used.
Two instruments were employed to assess maternal depression: the
Beck Depression Inventory (Beck, Ward, Mendelson, Mock, & Erbaugh,
1961) and the Diagnostic Interview Schedule for Children (Costello,
Edelbrock, & Costello, 1985). The BDI is a 21-item questionnaire,
with each item scored on a 4-point scale indicating the presence and
severity of depressed feelings/behaviors/symptoms. It is commonly
employed in research on nonclinically depressed samples, and it has
reasonably good psychometric properties (Beck, 1976). The Diagnostic
Interview Schedule for Children (DISC) addresses specific symptoms as
well as their chronology and duration. This standardized diagnostic
interview has a step structure that minimizes interviewing time.
Reliability and validity of the DISC have been found to be as good as or
better than other structured diagnostic interviews (Costello, Edelbrock,
& Costello, 1985). The DISC was used to diagnose dysthymia
(prevalent in earlier samples; see Field, 1995) and major depression
(rarely diagnosed in previous samples of depressed adolescent mothers).
The Maternal Stress Interview (Field, 1980) consists of 52
questions concerning socioeconomic status, marital status, other family
members, and home/life variables, such as crowding stress. The alpha
coefficient for internal consistency was .82 for this scale.
The Problem Oriented Screening Instrument for Teenagers (POSIT;
National Institute on Drug Abuse, 1987) was administered to the mothers
to screen for social, emotional, and behavioral problems. This 139-item
questionnaire measures various social, familial, physical, and mental
stressors. Items are grouped into ten subscales in order to identify
specific problem areas: Substance Use/Abuse, Physical Health Status,
Mental Health Status, Family Relations, Peer Relations, Educational
Status, Vocational Status, Social Skills, Leisure and Recreation, and
Aggressive Behavior/Delinquency.
The Interaction Rating Scale (Field, 1980) was utilized to measure
mothers' sensitivity to infant cues. Feeding interactions were
coded by research assistants who were blind to the mothers' group
status. Kappa coefficients for interobserver reliability averaged .82,
and the alpha coefficient for internal consistency was .83.
The mothers provided a sample of first morning urine, and the
infants' urine was collected on the second day. Urine samples were
frozen and sent to Duke University to be assayed for catecholamines
(norepinephrine, epinephrine, dopamine), 5-HIAA (serotonin metabolite),
and cortisol (see Kuhn, Schanberg, Field, Symanski, Zimmerman, Scafidi
& Roberts, 1991). Catecholamine and 5-HIAA assays were conducted by
high-pressure liquid chromatography with electrochemical detection,
cortisol was determined by radioimmunoassay, and creatinine was assayed
calorimetrically (Kuhn et al., 1991).
In other studies, "flat" affect (limited number and
variability of facial expressions) and lower activity levels in infants
of symptomatic mothers have been noted (Abrams, Field, Scafidi, &
Prodromidis, 1995; Field, 1992). Because these infant behaviors might
contribute to disturbed interactions independent of the mothers'
depressive behavior, the Brazelton Neonatal Behavior Assessment Scale
was administered and sleep/wake behavior assessments were conducted. For
the sleep-observation sessions (45 minutes), a research assistant
continuously coded the infant's sleep/wake states and behaviors
(using a laptop computer), yielding the percentage of time that the
different sleep states and behaviors occurred (see Field, Schanberg,
Scafidi, Bauer, VegaLahr, Garcia, Nystrom, & Kuhn, 1986). An
adaptation of Thoman's sleep state criteria was used to define
categories (Thoman, 1975). Prior to coding, the research assistant was
trained to .90 reliability.
The Brazelton Neonatal Behavior Assessment Scale was administered
midway between feedings and following the sleep observations (Brazelton,
1973). This scale consists of 20 neurological reflex items and 27
behavior items constituting 7 factors: habituation, orientation, motor
behavior, range of state, regulation of state, autonomic stability, and
abnormal reflexes (Lester, Hoffman, & Brazelton, 1985). At all
assessment periods, infant growth (weight, length, and head
circumference) was measured, because failure to thrive has been noted in
some samples.
Assessments at Three and Six Months
The same measures were administered at these two periods. The order
of the assessments was as follows: the maternal interview and EEG
recordings occurred concurrently, next was the mother-infant
face-to-face interaction, followed by saliva samples and growth
measures.
The maternal assessment consisted of the same scales used at the
neonatal period, including the BDI, the DISC, and the Maternal Stress
Interview, repeated at 3 and 6 months. In addition, the mothers'
perceptions of their infants' vulnerability were assessed using the
Vulnerable Child Scale (VCS; Perrin, West, & Culley, 1989). The VCS,
which is based upon Forsyth and Caney's Child Vulnerability Scale
(1985), consists of 16 items centering on concern about the child's
health (adapted here to be appropriate for infants). Examples include:
"This infant seems to be more sickly than other infants" and
"This infant seems to have as much energy as other infants of the
same age." Each item is rated on a four-point Likert scale, with
lower scores being optimal. The original scale was standardized on 320
mothers of 3 1/2 year old children, and both acceptable reliability and
concurrent validity were reported. Perrin et al. (1989) also reported
excellent test-retest reliability (r = .96), as well as acceptable
internal reliability (Cronbach's alpha = .75). VCS scores range
from 16 to 64, with higher scores in the present study indicating the
perception of greater vulnerability.
Just prior to the mother-infant interaction session, EEG was
recorded for mothers and infants. For the infant EEG, the infant was
placed on the mother's lap and bubbles were blown to maintain the
infant's attention. For the maternal EEG, the mother simply
reclined with eyes closed for three minutes.
EEG was recorded using a stretchable lycra cap that was positioned
on the subject's head using anatomical landmarks (Bloom &
Anneveldt, 1982; Field, Fox, Pickens, Nawrocki, & Soutullo, 1995).
Electrode gel was injected into the electrodes at the following sites:
F3, F4, P3, P4, and Cz (used as the reference), and impedances were
brought below 5,000 ohms. Additional electrodes were placed at the outer
canthus of each eye to obtain the subject's EOG (electrooculogram),
which was used to facilitate artifact scoring.
The signal was passed through a Grass Model 12 Neurodata
Acquisition System with amplifiers set as follows: low-frequency filter,
1 Hz; high-frequency filter, 100 Hz; amplification, 20,000. The line
frequency filter was on for all channels. The output from the amplifiers
was directed to a Dell 325D PC fitted with an Analog Devices RTI-815 A/D
board. The signal was sampled at a rate of 512 Hz and streamed to hard
disk using data acquisition software (Snapstream v. 3.21, HEM Data
Corp.).
EEG data were analyzed using an EEG analysis software package (EEG
Analysis System v. 5.3, James M. Long, 1987-1990). The first step of
this process involved the manual elimination of data that were unusable
due to artifact (eye movements, muscle activity, or technical
difficulties). The remaining artifact-free data were then spectrally
analyzed using discrete Fourier transforms to yield power data for
specific frequency bands. The average number of seconds of artifact-free
data available was 222.1 and 198.5 for the mothers with depressive
symptoms and without symptoms, respectively, and 120.1 and 125.9 for
their respective infants. The infant EEG data were analyzed from 1 to 12
Hz in 1 Hz bins. For mothers, the alpha band analyzed was 8 to 12 Hz.
Frontal alpha laterality ratios (FALR) were computed by dividing the
difference between right and left frontal alpha powers by the sum of
these powers. A score of zero represents hemispheric symmetry, a
negative score represents greater relative right frontal activ ation,
and a positive score represents greater relative left frontal
activation. Data analyses were also conducted on the natural log power
data for both hemispheres in the frontal and parietal regions.
The mothers and infants were then videotaped in a 3-minute
face-to-face interaction. The mother was simply asked to "pretend
you are playing with your infant at home" (Field, 1980). The infant
was placed in an infant seat on a table approximately fifteen inches
from the face of the mother, who was seated at the table. Two video
cameras and a split-screen generator enabled simultaneous monitoring of
the mother's face and torso and the infant's upper body. In
addition, heart rate was simultaneously recorded (Field, Pickens, Fox,
Nawrocki, & Gonzalez, 1995). EKG electrodes were placed on the right
scapula, the lower left lateral costal margin, and the lower left
vertebral region just above the waistline of the infants and mothers.
Heart rate was then converted to vagal tone using a vagal tone monitor
designed by Porges (1985).
Immediately prior to and 20 minutes following the interaction
sessions, saliva samples were taken from the mothers and infants by
placing a dental swab (dipped in lemonade crystals) along the gumline to
be subsequently assayed for cortisol levels. This permitted a
determination of the differential stressful effects of these
interactions on the depressed and nondepressed mother-infant dyads
(Field, Healy, Goldstein, Perry, Bendell, Schanberg, Zimmerman, &
Kuhn, 1988). Urine cortisol, the serotonin metabolite 5-HIAA, and
catecholamines (norepinephrine, epinephrine, dopamine) were assaysed
from first morning urine samples brought to the lab by the mothers
(although 24-hour urine samples would have been more reliable, the pilot
study indicated compliance problems). These assays were included because
mothers with depressive symptoms and their infants were found to differ
from control groups on these measures in earlier studies (Field, 1995).
The videotapes were subsequently coded according to three levels of
engagement: negative, neutral, and positive (Field, Healy, &
LeBlanc, 1989). The coding was completed on a laptop computer whose
software yielded a data matrix showing the percentage of time that the
mothers and infants were in these states, as well as the percentage of
time that they shared these states. The videotapes were also rated using
the Interaction Rating Scale (Field, 1980), which includes the following
behavior: (a) infant's state, physical activity, head orientation,
gaze behavior, facial expressions, vocalizations, and fussiness; and (b)
mother's state, physical activity, head orientation, gaze behavior,
facial expressions, contingent responsivity, and game playing. These are
scored on a 3-point Likert scale and then averaged to obtain a summary
rating for both mother and infant. Field (1980) reported interrater
reliabilities ranging from .81 to .96 (mean = .88). Interrater
reliabilities were determined on the basis of replicati on coding of
one-third of the videotapes and calculated using Kappa coefficients,
correcting for chance agreement. Heart rate was analyzed off-line using
a vagal tone monitor, such that the average heart rate and vagal tone
could be estimated for each interaction situation.
RESULTS
Depressed and nondepressed groups were compared using multivariate
analysis of variance. Where MANOVAs were significant, ANOVAs were
conducted and post hoc comparisons were made using Bonferroni t tests.
In addition, because of attrition across the 6-month period, analyses
were conducted to determine whether the retained and attrition samples
differed.
First, however, analyses were conducted on maternal background
variables and infant gender to determine whether these needed to be
entered as covariates in the MANOVAs. As can be seen in Table 1, the
depressed and nondepressed groups did not differ on the demographic
variables, including mother's age, education, socioeconomic status,
and ethnic distribution, nor did they differ on background stress. As
expected, the depressed group scored significantly higher on the Beck
Depression Inventory, had a significantly greater incidence of dysthymia
as measured by the DISC, and had higher risk scores on the POSIT.
Neonatal Variables
A MANOVA performed on the birth measures for the infants, including
gestational age, birth weight, birth length, head circumference,
ponderal index, 5-minute Apgar scores, and obstetric complications, was
not significant (see Table 2). However, the infants in the depressed
group had significantly more postnatal complications. A MANOVA performed
on the Neonatal Behavior Assessment Scale scores was significant, and
post hoc ANOVAs revealed that the infants of mothers with depressive
symptoms had significantly lower habituation and orientation scores and
higher depression scores (see Table 3). MANOVAs performed on the
sleep/wake behaviors and Interaction Rating Scale scores were also
significant, and post hoc ANOVAs indicated that the infants of depressed
mothers experienced significantly less frequent active sleep, more
frequent indeterminate sleep, less drowsy behavior, and more
jerks/startles (see Table 4). In addition, both the depressed mothers
and their infants received significantly lower scores in terms of
interaction behaviors.
MANOVAs performed on the mothers' and infants'
biochemical data were significant. Univariate ANOVAs revealed that
norepinephrine, epinephrine, dopamine, and serotonin (5-HIAA) levels
were significantly higher in mothers with depressive symptoms (see Table
5). ANOVAs for the infants' biochemical data revealed that the
norepinephrine, epinephrine, and dopamine levels of the infants of
mothers with depressive symptoms were significantly higher.
Three-Month and Six-Month Variables
MANOVAs conducted on the maternal interview variables at both the
3- and 6-months periods were significant. Post hoc ANOVAs indicated that
the mothers in the depressed group continued to have higher Beck
Depression Inventory scores and a greater incidence of dysthymia.
Further, at 3 months, they considered their infants more vulnerable (see
Table 6). On the mother-infant interaction variables, the depressed
mothers and their infants not only received inferior Interaction Rating
Scale scores at both time periods, but also showed less frequent
positive and/or more frequent negative behaviors during their
interactions.
A MANOVA performed on the infant measures was significant, and
univariate ANOVAs revealed that the vagal tone of the infants of
depressed mothers was significantly lower at both 3 and 6 months. Growth
measures also differentiated the infants of depressed mothers, including
significantly shorter length and smaller head circumference at 3 months
and significantly lower weight and smaller head circumference, as well
as more pediatric complications, at 6 months.
Regression Analysis
Correlation and regression analyses were then conducted on the data
of the depressed group to determine which maternal variables predicted
continued depressive symptoms at 6 months. A correlation analysis of all
maternal variables from the neonatal and 3-month periods and Beck
Depression Inventory scores at 6 months yielded significant correlations
only between BDI score at 6 months and a few 3-month variables,
including greater right frontal EEG, less positive interaction behavior,
and low vagal tone, as well as elevated norepinephrine, serotonin, and
cortisol levels at the neonatal stage. These potential
"predictor" variables were entered into a stepwise regression
analysis with 6-month BDI scores (continuing depression) as the outcome
measure and earlier BDI scores (neonatal and 3 month) as covariates. The
results of the multiple regression revealed that the predictor variables
entered the equation in the following order: (1) right frontal EEC accounted for 31% of the variance (p [less than] .001); (2) el evated
serotonin (5HIAA) accounted for an additional 13% of the variance (p
[less than] .01); and (3) elevated cortisol, less positive interaction
behavior, elevated norepinephrine, and low vagal tone added 7% to the
variance (p [less than] .05). Together, these explained 51% of the
variance in continuing depressive symptoms (6-month BDI scores greater
than 12). A discriminant function analysis revealed that 73% of the
cases were correctly classified by this group of variables: 71%
classified as having depressive symptoms had depressive symptoms, and
75% of the mothers without symptoms were correctly classified as not
having symptoms (overall p [less than].01).
STUDY 2
A review of the literature revealed that few studies have
investigated interventions with depressed mothers. Typically, these
interventions involved brief interaction coaching or mood induction
(Malphurs et al., 1996; O'Hara et al., 1990; Pickens & Field,
1995). These are not likely to have effects that are as long term as
would be the case with a more comprehensive intervention, particularly
for low-income adolescent mothers.
In Study 2, a second sample was recruited during the mothers'
hospital stay after childbirth. Mothers with and without depressive
symptoms were assessed over the first three months. At three months,
mothers who were at risk for continuing depressive symptoms were
identified using the predictor variables from Study 1. They were then
randomly assigned to a comprehensive intervention program or to a
control group. At 6 and 12 months, these groups were compared with each
other and with the nondepressed group.
METHOD
Sample
A sample similar to that of Study 1 was recruited during the
neonatal period (n = 160 mothers with depressive symptoms and n = 100
mothers without symptoms). The same neonatal and 3-month assessments
were conducted (see Study 1), and dyads whose values were above the
median (or below, depending on the negative direction) on the predictor
variables (derived from the regression analysis in Study 1) were
randomly assigned to an intervention group or to a control group. The
demographics of the two groups are shown in Table 7. As can be seen by
comparing Tables 1 and 7, the demographics of the Study 1 and Study 2
samples did not differ.
Assessments
The neonatal and 3-month assessments included the same measures
elaborated in Study 1. Longer-term outcomes of the intervention were of
interest; thus, 12-month assessments were also conducted. Further,
infant development was evaluated at 12 months using the Bayley Scales of
Infant Development (Bayley, 1969) and the Early Social Communication
Scales (Seibert & Hogan, 1982).
The Bayley Scales of Infant Development include a Mental scale and
a Motor scale. These standardized tests of infant development measure a
variety of sensorimotor and temperament functions. The Early Social
Communication Scales were developed to measure interactive behaviors of
older infants. The tester presents a series of toys and engages the
child in interactive games. Interactive behaviors are categorized by
developmental level, from simple to symbolic, and by communicative
function, from self-attention to attention to someone else.
Intervention
Brief interventions, such as interaction coaching, have been tried
with depressed mothers (Maphurs, Larrain, Field, Pickens,
Pelaez-Nogueras, Yando, & Bendell, 1996; Pickens & Field, 1995).
However, comprehensive programs, such as the one used in this study,
have rarely been tried with depressed mother-infant dyads.
It was thought that an educational/vocational experience might
attenuate the mothers' depression. Further, substitute caregiving
would both give the mothers the opportunity to pursue educational or
vocational interests and reduce the infants' exposure to their
mothers' depression. Thus, a 3-month social/educational/vocational
rehab program was arranged, and free day care in a model infant nursery
in a local public vocational high school was provided. Although this
kind of intervention had not been tried with depressed mothers, a
similar intensive intervention program was successfully used with
nondepressed teenage mothers (Field, Widmayer, Ignatoff, & Stronger,
1982). Mood induction activities were also part of the program,
including music mood induction, relaxation therapy, massage therapy, and
infant massage. In addition, interaction coaching was provided to help
the mothers and infants improve their interactions.
Vocational high school. The mothers attended a vocational high
school in the morning and participated in social and vocational rehab
activities and aerobics in the afternoon, while their infants received
all-day care. In addition, the mothers spent approximately one hour per
day in the nursery, helping to take care of their infants. The mothers
also received relaxation therapy, massage therapy, and music mood
induction, and their infants received massage therapy. The mothers and
infants together were given interaction coaching. Attendance averaged
80% across mothers, with very low variability.
Mood induction. The mood induction intervention targeted two
commonly noted parenting problems of depressed mothers: depressed mood
state and insensitivity to their infants' emotional cues. Depressed
mood state is a problem because the infants do not receive feedback for
their behaviors (contingent responsivity), and the depressed behavior
also serves as a model for the infants' own behavior (Field, 1986).
The mother's insensitivity to her infant's emotional cues
probably derives from her being preoccupied (Ingram & Smith, 1984).
The interventions designed here focused on altering the mothers'
mood state, thereby increasing sensitivity to their infants' cues.
Music mood induction was reported to be effective with 100% of
subjects in a review of mood induction techniques (Clark, 1983). In
contrast, mood induction cards were effective with only 68%, and greater
effects were reported for despondency and happiness using the music mood
induction. In the intervention implemented here, the mothers listened to
a half hour of different rock music selections for four weeks (two
sessions per week).
Relaxation therapy is another effective technique for reducing
depression (Agras, 1983). In one study, relaxation therapy was as
effective as psychotherapy and pharmacotherapy in reducing
depression-related anxiety (McClean & Hakistian, 1979). In another
study, only the relaxation therapy group continued to show reduced
anxiety levels (Reynolds & Coats, 1986). Relaxation therapy has also
been noted to slow heart rate, alter EEG, and diminish cortisol
production (Cooper, Jaffe, Lamprey, Botha, Shives, Baker, & Seftel,
1985; Platania-Solazzo, Field, Blank, Seligman, Kuhn, Schanberg, &
Saab, 1992). Here, progressive muscle relaxation and visual imagery were
used. Tapes were prepared by a relaxation therapist; gentle Far Eastern
music was combined with descriptions of positive visual images and
instructions on progressive muscle relaxation. These sessions were held
two times per week for four weeks.
Massage therapy has been found to reduce depression and anxiety, as
well as levels of stress hormones (norepinephrine and cortisol), in
hospitalized, depressed adolescents (Field, Morrow, Valdeon, Larson,
Kuhn, & Schanberg, 1992). In addition, their sleep patterns and
clinical condition improved. In the intervention implemented in the
present study, the mothers were given a 20-minute Swedish massage two
times per week for four weeks.
Infant massage has become increasingly popular because it not only
relaxes the infant and alleviates difficult problems for parents, such
as colic and disturbed infant sleep, but also because it enhances
parental sensitivity to infant cues and the parent-child relationship
(Auckett, 1981; Field, Grizzle, Scafidi, Abrams, & Richardson, 1996;
Schneider, 1982). For the 15-minute massage sessions, the infant was
placed in a prone position for 5 minutes of stroking, followed by 5
minutes in a supine position for cycling of the limbs (flexion and
extension), then 5 minutes in the prone position during which time the
infant was stroked again (Field et al., 1986). The infants received
massage two times per week for four weeks.
Interaction coaching. Field (1977) found that a technique called
maternal imitation of infant behavior contributed to mothers becoming
more sensitive to their infants' cues of being underaroused or
overaroused. The infants, in turn, became more attentive and responsive
than they typically were during spontaneous interactions. In another
attention-getting technique, mothers engaged in more game playing and
their infants' affect improved (Clark & Seifer, 1983; Pickens
& Field, 1995). Thus, for the interaction coaching in this study,
the mothers were given instructions on imitation (they were asked to
imitate their infants' behaviors) and attention getting (they were
asked to keep their infants' attention). The intent was to help
them be both more sensitive to their infants' cues (in the case of
imitating their infants) and more active (in the case of getting their
infants' attention).
RESULTS
Neonatal and Three Months
MANOVAs and ANOVAs comparing the depressed and nondepressed groups
on the birth measures and neonatal assessments (Table 8) and 3-month
variables (Table 9) led to results similar to those of Study 1, with the
depressed group having inferior scores. At 3 months, mothers in the
depressed group who had low interaction scores (below the median),
greater right frontal EEG, and low vagal tone (below the median) at 3
months and elevated norepinephrine, serotonin, and cortisol levels
(above the median) at the neonatal stage were randomly assigned to an
intervention group or a control group. These groups were then compared
with each other and with the nondepressed group at 6 and 12 months.
Six Months
Maternal depression scores. An ANOVA and post hoc Bonferroni t test
indicated that, following the 3-month-long intervention (from 3 to 6
months), the intervention group had a lower mean BDI score than did the
control group. However, the score was still higher than that for the
nondepressed group (see Table 10). Analyses revealed a similar pattern
for the incidence of DISC dysthymia. No significant differences across
groups were noted for background stress.
Interaction ratings. MANOVAs on the interaction ratings and
percentage of time positive, neutral, and negative behaviors occurred
were significant, as were most of the ANOVAs. Post hoc Bonferroni t
tests indicated that the mothers and infants in the control group had
lower interaction ratings and engaged in more frequent negative and less
frequent positive behaviors than did the mothers and infants in the
intervention group, who had scores that were comparable to those of the
nondepressed group.
Infant measures. A MANOVA on the infant measures at 6-month outcome
was significant. Individual ANOVAs and post hoc Bonferroni t tests
indicated that the vagal tone of the intervention group approximated
that of the nondepressed group and was significantly higher than that of
the control group. Infant weight for the intervention group was higher
than that for the control group, but lower than that for the
nondepressed group. In terms of pediatric complications, the score for
the intervention group was also more optimal than that for the control
group and not significantly different from the score for the
nondepressed group.
Biochemical variables. A MANOVA on the mothers' biochemical
variables at 6 months was significant. ANOVAs and post hoc Bonferroni t
tests indicated that the intervention group had significantly lower
epinephrine, dopamine, serotonin, and cortisol levels than did the
control group, approximating the levels of the nondepressed group (see
Table 11). This suggests that their biochemistry had normalized.
The MANOVA on the infants' biochemical variables was also
significant. ANOVAs and post hoc Bonferroni t tests indicated that the
intervention group's norepinephrine, epinephrine, dopamine, and
cortisol levels were higher than those of the control group. However,
the biochemical levels of the intervention group approximated those of
the control group, again suggesting normalization.
Twelve Months
Maternal depression. An ANOVA and post hoc Bonferroni t tests
revealed that the intervention group continued to experience
significantly greater depressive symptoms (i.e., higher BDI score) than
did the nondepressed group (see Table 12). However, the BDI score for
the intervention group was significantly lower than that of the control
group (see Table 12).
Infant development. A MANOVA on the infant development scores was
significant. Subsequent ANOVAs and post hoc Bonferroni t tests indicated
that the intervention group scored higher on the Bayley Mental and Motor
scales than did the control group, with scores approximating those of
the nondepressed group. In addition, on the Early Social Communication
Scales, the intervention group had higher Responding and Initiating
scores than did the control group, and a higher Responding score than
even the nondepressed group.
Physical measures. A MANOVA on the infants' physical measures
was significant. Post hoc ANOVAs and Bonferroni t tests revealed that
the intervention group had greater length and fewer pediatric
complications than did the control group, with values approximating
those of the nondepressed group.
DISCUSSION
Most of the mothers with depressive symptoms in Study 1 continued
to have symptoms over the first 6 months (about 13% went into
remission). Continued symptoms over the first 6 months has been
associated with developmental and growth delays for infants starting at
around one year (Field, 1992). This highlights the importance of
identifying variables that predict which mothers will experience
continuing symptoms. In addition to identifying predictor variables,
Study 1 documented a "dysregulation" profile, starting as
early as the neonatal period, for infants of symptomatic mothers. The
data from Study 2 showed the effectiveness of an intervention program
for these adolescent mothers and their infants.
The depressed mothers' negative interactional behaviors, as
well as their continuing symptoms, across the first 6 months were
consistent with findings from earlier research (see Field, 1992, 1995,
for reviews of studies conducted by Cohn et al., 1990; Murray, 1992; and
O'Hara, Rehm, & Campbell, 1983). In addition, several
physiological and biochemical differences were noted; for example, the
symptomatic mothers had elevated levels of catecholamines
(norepinephrine and epinephrine), which often accompany depression,
along with elevated dopamine and serotonin metabolites (5-HIAA). Also
seen in adults with symptoms of chronic depression, 90% of the
symptomatic mothers had greater right frontal EEG activation (see
Henriques & Davidson, 1990). The regression equation revealed that
these variables (right frontal EEG, lower vagal tone, and less positive
interaction behavior at 3 months, and elevated norepinephrine,
serotonin, and cortisol at the neonatal period) were responsible for 51%
of the variance in the mothe rs' continuing symptoms at 6 months.
Greater right frontal EEG alone accounted for 31% of the variance, with
elevated serotonin adding another 13%. The discriminant function
analysis indicated that the mothers' continuing depressive symptoms
were predicted by these variables with 73% accuracy. These variables
were then used to identify mothers who need early intervention.
The dysregulation profile of the infants of symptomatic mothers
might also be useful for targeting intervention dyads. The infants had
inferior Brazelton scores, not unlike those reported in earlier studies
(Abrams et al., 1995; Zuckerman & Beardslee, 1987). Specifically,
their inferior habituation and orientation scores suggested high
stimulus thresholds and less ability to attend to stimulation, and their
Lester "depression" scores were consistent with their
frequently noted flat affect and low activity levels. Their immature
sleep patterns (e.g., greater indeterminate sleep and more
jerks/startles) were also consistent with physiological dysregulation,
as were their elevated norepinephrine, epinephrine, and dopamine levels
(which mirrored their mothers' elevated levels). The absence of
cortisol elevations among both the mothers and infants was unexpected,
because elevated levels have been noted in previous samples (Field,
1995). However, cortisol may "reequilibrize" with continuing
depression, and more se vere depression ratings may be associated with
lower cortisol levels, as if cortisol conforms to an inverted U
function. At 3 months, greater right frontal EEG activation and lower
vagal tone suggested both autonomic and central nervous system
involvement.
The less positive interaction behaviors were not surprising,
because they have been reported by many investigators, although not
typically as early as the neonatal period, as found here. While infant
growth delays by one year have been reported in a previous sample
(Field, 1992), the delays noted in the present study were seen as early
as 3 months and involved head circumference and length. At 6 months,
growth delays were noted for head circumference and weight.
Such growth delays highlight the need for early intervention.
Because the intervention in this study was multifaceted (the mothers
participated in a social/educational/vocational rehab program and
several mood-induction interventions, with day care provided for their
infants), it is not possible to determine which components were most
important, or whether all were necessary for successful outcomes.
Interestingly, despite the intervention and its several positive
outcomes, participating mothers continued to have elevated Beck
Depression Inventory scores (although they were not as high as those for
the mothers in the control group). Nonetheless, mother and infant
interaction behaviors became more positive and, for both, several
physiological and biochemical measures normalized (i.e., became more
like the nondepressed group). Normalization entailed a decrease in
epinephrine, dopamine, and serotonin for the mothers and, for the
infants, an increase in norepinephrine, epinephrine, dopamine, and
cortisol. Although the meaning of the increased levels for the infants
is unclear, given the lack of norms for neurotransmitters in infants,
the fact that their levels approximated those of the infants of
nondepressed mothers is encouraging. In addition, the increase was
comparable to that noted in the catecholamine levels of preterm infants
who received early intervention (Kuhn et al., 1991).
By 12 months (6 months after the end of the 3-month-long
intervention), the BDI score of the mothers in the intervention group
remained higher than that of the mothers in the nondepressed group, but
their infants were not showing the growth or developmental delays noted
in an earlier study (Field, 1992). Their infants, compared with those in
the control group, had fewer pediatric complications, greater length,
and superior mental, motor, and social communication scores, with values
approximating those of the nondepressed group. This suggests that the
positive effects of the intervention were persisting for the infants.
Further, despite the fact that the BDI score remained higher than normal
for the mothers in the intervention group, their behavior had improved
and their biochemistry suggested less stress and depression.
Assessing these mothers during pregnancy and studying the buffering
effects of fathers may help disentangle prenatal and genetic factors. In
addition, further studies are needed on the dysregulation noted in the
infants. Finally, because the intervention was so comprehensive, it is
not clear which components are critical to achieve these effects.
Nonetheless, the data suggest that, at the very least, adolescent
mothers who are not receiving treatment and who continue to experience
depressive symptoms can be identified, and those mothers and their
infants can be offered a fairly cost-effective intervention that
significantly attenuates the infants' delays in growth and
development.
The authors thank the infants and parents who participated in this
study and the research assistants who helped with data collection. This
research was supported by an NIMH Research Scientist Award (#MH00331)
and an NIMH Research Grant (#MH46586) to Tiffany Field.
Tiffany Field, Margarita Prodromidis, and Julie Malphurs, Touch
Research Institutes, University of Miami School of Medicine and Nova
Southeastern University.
Jeffrey Pickens, James Madison University.
Nathan Fox, University of Maryland.
Debra Bendell, Fremont Kaiser Permanente.
Regina Yando, Harvard Medical School.
Saul Schanberg and Cynthia Kuhn, Duke University Medical School.
Reprint requests to Tiffany Field, Touch Research Institutes,
University of Miami School of Medicine, Department of Pediatrics
(D-820), P.O. Box 016820, Miami, Florida 33101.
REFERENCES
Abrams, S. M., Field, T., Scafidi, F., & Prodromidis, M.
(1995). Newborns of depressed mothers. Infant Mental Health Journal, 16,
231-237.
Agras, W. S. (1983). Medical uses of relaxation training. Paper
presented at Grand Rounds, University of Wisconsin, Department of
Psychiatry, Madison, WI.
Auckett, A. D. (1981). Baby massage. New York: Newmarket Press.
Bayley, N. (1969). Bayley Scales of Infant Development. New York: The
Psychological Corporation.
Beardslee, W. R., Bemporad, J., Keller, M. B., & Kierman, G. L.
(1983). Children of parents with major affective disorder: A review.
American Journal of Psychiatiy, 140, 825-832.
Beck, A. T. (1976). Cognitive therapy and emotional disorders. New
York: International Universities Press.
Beck, A. T., Ward, C. H., Mendelson, M., Mock, J. E., &
Erbaugh, J. (1961). An inventory for measuring depression. Archives of
General Psychiatry, 4, 561-571.
Bloom, J. L., & Anneveldt, M. (1982). An electrode cap tested.
Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology, 54, 591-594.
Brazelton, T. B. (1973). Neonatal Behavioral Assessment Scale.
London: Spastics International Medical Publications.
Campbell, S. B., Cohn, J. F., & Meyers, T. (1995). Depression
in first-time mothers: Mother-infant interaction and depression
chronicity. Developmental Psychology, 31, 349-357.
Clark, D. M. (1983). On the induction of depressed mood in the
laboratory: Evaluation and comparison of the Velten and musical
procedures. Advances in Behavior Research and Therapy, 5, 27-49.
Clark, G. N., & Seifer, R. (1983). Facilitating mother-infant
communication: A treatment model for high-risk and developmentally
delayed infants. Infant Mental Health Journal, 4, 67-82.
Cohen, J. (1990). Things I have learned (so far). American
Psychologist, 45, 1304-1312.
Cohn, J. F., Campbell, S. B., Matias, R., & Hopkins, J. (1990).
Face-to-face interactions of postpartum depressed and nondepressed
mother-infant pairs at two months. Developmental Psychology, 26, 15-23.
Cooper, R., Jaffe, B. I., Lamprey, J. M., Botha, L. Shives, R.,
Baker, S., & Seftel, H. C. (1985). Hormonal and biochemical
responses to TM. Postgraduate Medical Journal, 61, 301-304.
Costello, E. J., Edelbrock, C. S., & Costello, A. J. (1985).
Validity of the NIMH Diagnostic Interview Schedule for Children: A
comparison between psychiatric and pediatric referrals. Journal of
Abnormal Child Psychology, 13, 579-595.
Field, T. (1977). Effects of early separation, interactive deficits
and experimental manipulations on infant-mother face-to-face
interaction. Child Development, 48, 763-771.
Field, T. (1980). Interactions of preterm and term infants with
their lower-and middle-class teenage and adult mothers. In T. Field, S.
Goldberg, D. Stern, & A. Sostek (Eds.), High-risk infants and
children: Adult and peer interactions. New York: Academic Press.
Field, T. (1984). Early interactions between infants and their
postpartum depressed mothers. Infant Behavior and Development, 7,
517-522.
Field, T. (1986). Interventions for premature infants. Journal of
Pediatrics, 109, 183-191.
Field T. (1992). Infants of depressed mothers. Development and
Psychopathology, 4, 49-66.
Field, T. (1995). Infants of depressed mothers. Infant Behavior and
Development, 18, 1-13.
Field, T., Fox. N., Pickens, J., Nawrocki, T., & Soutullo, D.
(1995). Right frontal EEG activation in 3- to 6-month-old infants of
"depressed" mothers. Developmental Psychology, 31, 358-363.
Field, T., Grizzle, N., Scafidi, F., Abrams, S., & Richardson,
S. (1996). Massage therapy for infants of depressed mothers. Infant
Behavior and Development, 19, 109-114.
Field, T., Healy, B., Goldstein, S., & Guthertz, M. (1990).
Behavior state matching and synchrony in mother-infant interactions of
nondepressed versus depressed dyads. Developmental Psychology, 26, 7-14.
Field, T., Healy, B., Goldstein, S., Perry, S., Bendell, D.,
Schanberg, S., Zimmerman, E. A., & Kuhn, C. (1988). Infants of
depressed mothers show "depressed" behavior even with
nondepressed adults. Child Development, 59, 1569-1579.
Field, T., Healy, B., & LeBlanc, W. (1989). Sharing and
synchrony of behavior states and heart rate in "nondepressed"
versus "depressed" mother-infant interactions. Infant Behavior
and Development, 12, 357-376.
Field, T., Morrow, C., Valdeon, C., Larson, S., Kuhn, C., &
Schanberg, S. (1992). Massage therapy reduces anxiety in child and
adolescent psychiatric patients. Journal of the American Academy of
Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 31(1), 125-131.
Field, T., Pickens, J., Fox, N., Nawrocki, T., & Gonzalez, J.
(1995). Vagal tone in infants of depressed mothers. Development and
Psychopathology, 7, 227-231.
Field, T., Schanberg, S. M., Scafidi, F., Bauer, C. R., Vega-Lahr,
N., Garcia, R., Nystrom, J., & Kuhn, C. M. (1986).
Tactile/kinesthetic stimulation effects on preterm neonates. Pediatrics,
77, 654-658.
Field, T., Widmayer, S., Ignatoff, E., & Stronger, S. (1982).
Developmental effects of an intervention for preterm infants of teenage
mothers. Infant Mental Health Journal, 3, 11-18.
Forsyth, B., & Caney, P. (1985). Long-term implications of
problems of feeding and behavior in early infancy: A 3 1/2-year
follow-up. Paper presented at a meeting of the Ambulatory Pediatric
Association, Washington, DC.
Henriques, J. B., & Davidson, R. J. (1990). Regional brain
electrical asymmetries discriminate between previously depressed and
healthy control subjects. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 99, 22-31.
Ingram, R. E., & Smith, T. W. (1984). Depression and internal
versus external focus of attention. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 8,
139-152.
Kuhn, C., Schanberg, S., Field, T., Symanski, R., Zimmerman, E.,
Scafidi, F., & Roberts, J. (1991). Tactile/kinesthetic stimulation
effects on sympathetic and adrenocortical function in preterm infants.
Journal of Pediatrics, 119, 434-440.
Leaf, P. J., Bruce, M. L., Tischler, G. L., Freeman, D. H.,
Weisman, M. M., & Myers, J. K. (1988). Factors affecting the
utilization of specialty and general medical mental health services.
Medical Care, 26, 9-26.
Lester, B. M., Hoffman, J., & Brazelton, T. B. (1985). The
rhythmic structure of mother-infant interaction in term and preterm
infants. Child Development, 56, 15-27.
Littman, D., & Parmelee, A. (1978). Medical correlates of
infant development. Pediatrics, 61, 470-482.
Lyons-Ruth, K., Zoll, D., Connell, D., & Grunebaum, H. (1986).
The depressed mother and her one-year-old infant: Environment,
interaction, attachment, and infant development. In E. Tronick & T.
Field (Eds.), Maternal depression and infant disturbance. San Francisco:
Jossey-Bass.
Malphurs, J., Larrain, C. M., Field, T., Pickens, J.,
Pelaez-Nogueras, M., Yando, R., & Bendell, D. (1996). Altering
withdrawn and intrusive interaction behaviors of depressed mothers.
Infant Mental Health Journal, 17, 152-160.
McClean, P. D., & Hakistian, R. (1979). Clinical depression:
Comparative efficacy of outpatient treatments. Journal of Clinical and
Consulting Psychology, 47, 818-836.
Murray, L. (1992). The impact of postnatal depression on infant
development. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 33, 543-561.
National Institute on Drug Abuse. (1987). The adolescent
assessment/referral system. Available from the U.S. Department of Health
and Human Services, Alcohol, Drug Abuse and Mental Health
Administration.
O'Hara, M., Rehm, L. P., & Campbell, S. B. (1983).
Postpartum depression: The role of social network and life stress
variables. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 171, 336-341.
O'Hara, M. W., Zekoski, E. M., Philipps, L. H., & Wright,
E. J. (1990). A controlled, prospective study of postpartum mood
disorders: Comparison of childbearing and non-childbearing women.
Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 99, 3-15.
Orvaschel, H. (1983). Maternal depression and child dysfunction:
Children at risk. In B. Lahey & A. Kazdin (Eds.), Advances in
clinical child psychology. New York: Plenum.
Perrin, E., West, P., & Culley, B. (1989). Is my child normal
yet? Correlates of vulnerability. Pediatrics, 83(3), 355-363.
Pickens, J., & Field, T. (1995). Facial expressions and vagal
tone in infants of depressed and nondepressed mothers. Early Development
and Parenting, 4, 83-89.
Platania-Solazzo, A., Field, T., Blank, J., Seligman, F., Kuhn, C.,
Schanberg, S., & Saab, P. (1992). Relaxation therapy reduces anxiety
in child/adolescent psychiatry patients. Acta Paedopsychiatrica, 55,
115-120.
Porges, S. W. (1985). Method and apparatus for evaluating rhythmic
oscillations in aperiodic physiological response systems. United States
Patent No. 4, 510, 944 (April 16, 1985).
Radke-Yarrow, M., Cummings, E. M., Kuczynski, L., & Chapman, M.
(1985). Patterns of attachment in two- and three-year-olds in normal
families and families with parental depression. Child Development, 56,
886-893.
Radloff, L. S. (1977). The CES-D Scale: A self-report depression
scale for research in the general population. Applied Psychological
Measurement, 1, 385-401.
Reynolds, W., & Coats, K. (1986). A comparison of
cognitive-behavioral therapy and relaxation training for treatment of
depression in adolescents. Journal of Clinical and Consulting
Psychology, 54, 653-660.
Sameroff, A. J., & Seifer, R. (1983). Familial risk and child
competence. Child Development, 54, 1254-1268.
Schneider, V. (1982). Infant massage. Toronto: Bantam Books.
Seibert, J., & Hogan, A. (1982). Procedures manual for the
Early Social Communication Scales. Unpublished manuscript, University of
Miami.
Seifer, R. (1995). Perils and pitfalls of high-risk research.
Developmental Psychology, 31, 420-424.
Thoman, E. B. (1975). Early development of sleeping behaviors in
infants. In N. T. Ellis (Ed.), Behavior and development in infancy:
Human and animal studies. New York: John Wiley and Sons.
Zuckerman, B. S., & Beardslee, W. R. (1987). Maternal
depression: A concern for pediatricians. Pediatrics, 79, 110-117.