Terminal core values associated with adolescent problem behaviors.
Goff, Brent G. ; Goddard, H. Wallace
INTRODUCTION
According to several theories, delinquency is related, directly or
indirectly, to terminal core values (such as security and sense of
belonging). It has been suggested that problem behavior is the result of
deviant self-image, which arises from the values adopted as
"guiding principles" (Grube, Weir, Getzlaf, & Rokeach,
1984). Delinquency may be an attempt to mimic deviant referents and to
repudiate nondeviant referents (Grube et al., 1984). Further, by
subscribing to the values of deviant peers, adolescents select a
delinquent identity (Simons, Whitbeck, Conger, & Conger, 1991).
The subculture-of-violence thesis (Wolfgang & Ferracuti, 1967)
proposes that some groups are accepting of violence. Members are thought
to assimilate the values of the group, which guides violent behavior.
Values are also thought to mediate the relationship between
sociodemographic variables and violent behavior (Felson, Liska, South,
& McNulty, 1994).
Some believe that the development of a delinquent lifestyle is a
matter of conscious choice based on values (Kennedy & Baron, 1993).
An individual may choose to engage in a predatory crime after
considering personal values, such as the need for peer approval or,
expressed in values language, being well-respected (Seigal & Senna,
1991). Maintaining honor, which is related to being well-respected, also
seems to be strongly related to violence (Wolfgang & Ferracuti,
1967). Violence may thus be viewed as a "prestige-conferring
behavior" (Kennedy & Baron, 1993, p. 91). Further, delinquency
is likely to be related to such values as toughness, excitement, and
risk taking (Miller, 1958). In addition, Hurrelmann and Engel (1992)
claim that delinquency is a symptom of adolescents' orientation
toward success (sense of accomplishment) and status (being
well-respected). Consequently, problem behavior may be a rational choice
for individuals whose goals are frustrated (Felson et al., 1994). This
is consistent with the position that delinquency results when success
and achievement are desired but unattainable through socially acceptable
means (Cloward & Olin, 1960).
Strain theory proposes that strain is produced when there is a gap
between security needs and the means to fulfill those needs (Bernard,
1984). Security and delinquency are linked: "people are driven to
do things they do not want to do" (Bernard, 1984, p. 357). Revised
strain theory suggests that adolescents focus on multiple goals (desired
end states) and that blockage of those goals may result in delinquency
(Agnew, 1985). Delinquent behavior seems to be an illegitimate means of
fulfilling desired end states (values), which may explain why
delinquency is found across social classes.
One widely used measure of terminal values is Kahle's (1986)
List of Values (LOV), which can be arranged so as to be congruent with
Maslow's hierarchy of needs. A values-hierarchy approach may
explain anomalies in delinquency theory. For example, some adolescents
go for long periods of time without committing delinquent acts (Hirschi,
1969), behavior that is understandable if these adolescents have found
alternative means of fulfilling their desired end states, or values.
This approach may also explain, in part, why most abandon delinquent
behavior in late adolescence (e.g., Greenberg, 1977; Hirschi, 1969), a
time when they more likely have sorted out their values (prioritized)
and found better means of fulfilling them.
Values are related to how people reason about social issues (e.g.,
Tetlock, 1986; Kristiansen & Matheson, 1990), to attitude formation
(Homer & Kahle, 1988), and to behavior (Williams, 1979). Delinquents
are likely to perceive positive consequences from problem behavior, are
less likely to perceive their behavior in terms of moral issues, and
tend to hold beliefs that neutralize the moral consequences of
delinquent behavior (Guerra, 1989). A link between fun and enjoyment
(positive consequence) and delinquency implies a lack of social control:
"people are freed to do what they want" (Bernard, 1984, p.
357).
Some values are thought to be in opposition to one another. For
instance, the values of achievement and hedonism may conflict with that
of empathy (Kamakura & Novak, 1992). Achievement (i.e., sense of
accomplishment, self-fulfillment, being well-respected) and hedonism
(i.e., fun and enjoyment) are more self oriented, while empathy (i.e.,
warm relationships with others) is other oriented. Similarly, a world at
peace and national security tend to be conflicting values. The
value-justification hypothesis states that individuals justify their
attitudes by appealing to particular values (Eiser, 1987). For example,
some people may oppose nuclear weapons on the grounds that they reduce
the chances of peace, while others support nuclear weapons on the
grounds that they improve national security (Kristiansen & Matheson,
1990).
Dominant values influence beliefs and attitudes, which are
reflected in behavior (lifestyle). Each referent group tends to develop
a dominant set of values to which members of the group are favorably
disposed. They construe reality in terms of those salient values and act
accordingly.
Relationship of Values to Adolescent Problem Behavior
Studies have provided some insight into possible links between
values and problem behaviors. Adoption of prosocial values (Braithwaite
& Law, 1985) has been found to inhibit the selection of deviant
peers and to have a negative, indirect effect on delinquent behavior
(Simons et al., 1991). Specifically, prosocial values correlate
negatively with trouble at school, depression, and substance abuse
(Simons, Whitbeck, Conger, & Melby, 1991).
Values may also mediate the relationship between dysfunctional
family interaction and adolescent problem behavior (Allen, Leadbeater,
& Aber, 1990). This appears to be a special type of learned deviance
(Simons, Whitbeck, Conger, & Conger, 1991). Specifically, adolescent
females' lack of identification with adult values is related to
delinquency, drug use, and unprotected sexual activity (Allen,
Leadbeater, & Aber, 1990).
Some researchers have suggested that the values of independence and
excitement are related to having deviant peers, as well as to substance
abuse (Simons, Whitbeck, Conger, & Melby, 1991). Sensation seeking
(excitement) has been linked to delinquency (Farely & Sewell, 1976).
Findings have indicated that the values of excitement and pleasure are
associated with smoking, while the values of sense of accomplishment,
security, and self-respect are associated with nonsmoking (Grube et al.,
1984). An exciting life is a value that is positively related to
alcoholism, while social recognition (being well-respected) is
negatively related (Flores, 1986). Independence and enjoyment of
sensuous experiences are values that have been linked to drug use, while
sympathy and nurturing (empathy) and high aspiration for self
(achievement) are negatively related to drug use (Block, Block, &
Keyes, 1988).
There also may be a connection between terminal values and sexual
activity. Offir, Fisher, Williams, and Fisher (1993) found sexual
activity to be negatively related to security and positively related to
pleasure (fun and enjoyment, excitement). According to Jessor and Jessor
(1975), sexual activity appears to be related to independence and
autonomy, closeness or love (warm relationships with others), and an
attempt to gain peer-group respect (being well-respected). In addition,
sexual activity is related to pleasure, conquest, and increased status
and esteem (being well-respected, sense of accomplishment) for males
(Sorenson, 1973; Weis, 1983).
Kahle (1983) has demonstrated that groups segmented by their most
important terminal core value show significant differences in race,
gender, political preference, religion, occupation, income, and
lifestyle. The purpose of the present study was to examine whether
adolescents grouped by their dominant value (most important terminal
value) differ in the frequency in which they engage in delinquent acts,
substance use, and sexual activity.
METHOD
Subjects
Participants were 685 students in two rural Southern high schools.
Permission to participate in the study was obtained from parents.
Surveys were administered during regular class periods by trained
teachers or counselors. To reduce information transfer, all of the
classes within each school were administered the survey at the same
time. A system of veracity checks was utilized to test for accuracy of
response, reducing the number of usable surveys to 544. The sample was
48% male, 72% African American, and the average age was 16 years.
Forty-six percent of the students lived with both natural parents. Most
parents (81% of the fathers and 80% of the mothers) had at least a high
school education. Most of the parents were employed (84% of the fathers
and 70% of the mothers). The adolescents were sorted according to their
dominant value: sense of belonging (29), excitement (25), warm
relationships with others (81), self-fulfillment (17), being
well-respected (91), fun and enjoyment (52), security (31), self-respect
(142), and sense of accomplishment (76).
Measures
Delinquency was measured using 17 frequency items derived from the
Alabama Teen Assessment Program (ALTAP). For most delinquent acts,
responses ranged from never to four or more times in the last year.
Trouble at school (items such as parents called, expulsion) was measured
for the last six months, and frequency of truancy was measured for the
last month.
Frequency of use of seven substances was measured on a 6-category
scale, with responses ranging from never used to use every day. Age at
first use of tobacco and alcohol was also assessed. In addition,
students were asked how often they drank to get drunk, with 6 possible
responses, ranging from do not drink to often drink to get drunk.
Frequency of drinking was further assessed among nonabstainers on a
10-point scale, anchored by hardly ever drink (1) and drink all the time
(10).
Sexual frequency items were derived from ALTAP. Age at first
intercourse for the sexually active was measured using 12 categories
that ranged from 8 to 18 years. One item measured number of partners,
with 6 possible responses, ranging from none to 11 or more. Sexually
active adolescents were also asked to rate their activity on a 10-point
scale, ranging from not sexually active (1) to very sexually active
(10).
Terminal core values were measured using LOV. Students were asked
to circle, from a list of nine values, the one that was most important
to them.
Analysis
Students were divided into nine groups according to their most
important value. Table 1 shows the gender, race, approximate grade point
average, and mean age for each group. ANOVA and Tukey multiple range
tests were used to assess differences in problem behaviors across the
nine value groups. A value group was deemed to be high or low in
frequency of a particular problem behavior in relation to the other
value groups.
RESULTS
Certain terminal core (dominant) values appear to inhibit or
enhance problem behavior (see Table 2). Shoplifting appears to be
inhibited by sense of belonging, warm relationships with others, being
well-respected, [TABULAR DATA FOR TABLE 1 OMITTED] self-respect, and
sense of accomplishment. It is encouraged by the value of fun and
enjoyment. Petty theft (less than $50) seems to be inhibited by sense of
belonging and encouraged by fun and enjoyment. Theft (more than $50)
seems to be inhibited by being well-respected and self-respect and
encouraged by fun and enjoyment. Breaking and entering is inhibited by
sense of belonging, excitement, warm relationships with others, being
well-respected, self-respect, and sense of accomplishment. It is
encouraged by security. The same pattern appears for violence involving
a weapon. Physical violence is inhibited by sense of belonging, warm
relationships with others, and self-respect, and encouraged by fun and
enjoyment. Use of a weapon for extortion has a pattern similar to that
for breaking and entering and violence with a weapon, except that
excitement and sense of accomplishment do not act as inhibitors.
Physical extortion is inhibited by warm relationships with others, being
well-respected, self-respect, and sense of accomplishment. It is
encouraged by fun and enjoyment and security. Vehicle theft is inhibited
by excitement, warm relationships with others, being well-respected,
self-respect, and sense of accomplishment. It is encouraged by fun and
enjoyment. Being arrested was less frequent for the excitement and
self-respect groups and most frequent for the security group. Vandalism
is inhibited by sense of belonging, warm relationships with others,
self-fulfillment, being well-respected, self-respect, and sense of
accomplishment. It is encouraged by fun and enjoyment. Truancy is
inhibited by being well-respected and self-respect, and encouraged by
fun and enjoyment. Trouble at [TABULAR DATA FOR TABLE 2 OMITTED] school
is inhibited by sense of belonging, being well-respected, self-respect,
and a sense of accomplishment. It is encouraged by fun and enjoyment.
Forced sex, running away, having parents called to school, and
suspension/expulsion from school were not differentiated by dominant
value. Thus, delinquency seems to be encouraged by two dominant values,
fun and enjoyment (a hedonistic value) and security (a survival value or
very primal motive), and inhibited by the seven other values. The most
effective inhibition values, in order of impact, are self-respect and
being well-respected (moral respect values), warm relationships with
others and sense of belonging (sociability values), sense of
accomplishment (a self-actualization value), excitement (a hedonistic
value), and self-fulfillment (a self-actualization value).
The results for substance use provide additional insight. Smoking
tobacco is inhibited by the values of being well-respected,
self-respect, and sense of accomplishment. It is encouraged by fun and
enjoyment. Drinking beer/wine has the same pattern. Drinking hard liquor is inhibited by self-respect and sense of accomplishment, and encouraged
by fun and enjoyment. Use of marijuana is inhibited by being
well-respected and encouraged by fun and enjoyment.
Self-fulfillment, self-respect, security, sense of accomplishment,
and being well-respected delay the onset of smoking. However, fun and
enjoyment is a value that encourages use of tobacco at an early age.
Only self-respect delays the use of alcohol, while self-fulfillment and
fun and enjoyment encourage drinking at an early age. Drunkenness was
less common for the being well-respected, self-respect, and sense of
accomplishment groups, and most frequent for the fun and enjoyment
segment. Frequent drinking has a similar pattern to that for
drunkenness, although warm relationships with others also acts as an
inhibitor. There were no value differences for chewing tobacco, use of
cocaine, and other drug use.
Having fewer sex partners is associated with sense of belonging,
and having more partners with being well-respected. Frequency of
adolescent sexual activity is inhibited by sense of belonging, and
enhanced by warm relationships with others. Age at first intercourse was
not differentiated by dominant value.
DISCUSSION
The two groups most prone to problem behaviors were those with the
dominant values of fun/enjoyment and security. In addition, it appears
that the dominant values related to recreational deviance were different
from the dominant values related to survival deviance. The dominance of
the fun and enjoyment value suggests that adolescents are free to do
what they want (a lack of self-control and social control), and the
dominance of security suggests that adolescents are forced to do things
they do not want to do (strain theory). Dominance of security as a value
may also suggest anxiety and insecurity arising from insensitive
caregiving during childhood. The more violent crimes and those that
directly involve a victim seem to be associated with the dominance of
security. Crimes against property (vandalism) and those that indirectly
involve a victim through property (shoplifting, theft) seem to be
associated with the dominance of fun and enjoyment. Further, substance
abuse (a crime primarily against self) is strongly associated with fun
and enjoyment but not with security.
The empathy values of sense of belonging and warm relationships
with others seem to be important inhibitors of the more violent crimes,
and tend to offset elements relevant to strain theory. In contrast, the
moral respect values (self-respect, being well-respected) and the
self-actualization value of sense of accomplishment appear to have a
more general effect, inhibiting violent crimes, minor delinquent acts,
and substance abuse.
Consistent with previous research, warm relationships with others
and being well-respected encouraged sexual activity. However, sense of
belonging inhibited sexual activity. It is possible that adolescents
with a strong sense of belonging primarily associate it with family or
friends. Warm relationship with others may represent the transition to
dyadic, sexually oriented relationships. This is consistent with
Kamakura and Novak's (1992) observation that warm relationship with
others is often interpreted as having a sexual connotation. The finding
that being well-respected was associated with a greater number of
partners seems consistent with the male perception of sex as a means to
increase status, esteem, and peer-group respect.
The salience of terminal core values is at the heart of a unique
intervention strategy that may be applicable to problem behavior. Value
self-confrontation methods, which were pioneered by Rokeach (1973), seek
to change people's behavior by changing the priority of the values
that underlie that behavior (Schwartz & Inbar-Saban, 1988). Values
self-confrontation has been successfully used to reduce smoking (Conroy,
1979), increase weight loss (Schwartz & Inbar-Saban, 1988), increase
financial contributions to, and membership in, civil rights
organizations (Rokeach, 1973), and enhance pro-environment behavior
(Ball-Rokeach et al., 1984). Values self-confrontation has been used in
classroom settings, via computer feedback and through the mass media
(Grube, Mayton, & Ball-Rokeach, 1994). The theoretical foundation of
value self-confrontation and a description of its implementation are
provided by Grube et al. (1994) and Schwartz and Inbar-Saban (1988).
Further research should be undertaken to determine its effectiveness
with adolescent problem behaviors.
Identifying adolescents by their most important value appears to
have diagnostic merit, as those whose dominant value is fun and
enjoyment or security appear to be prone to problem behavior. The
salience of fun and enjoyment may be altered by such intervention
approaches as values self-confrontation. However, altering the salience
of security may be more problematic, especially if it is a dominating
value because of difficult life circumstances (as posited by strain
theory). It may be necessary to provide adolescents with the means to
fulfill security needs through more legitimate outlets.
Programs that increase the salience of self-respect, being
well-respected (positive peer pressure), and sense of accomplishment
would tend to decrease propensities toward problem behavior. Further,
sense of belonging appears to be an important value for sexual
abstinence. These values appear to be "guiding principles"
that tend to inhibit the formation of a deviant self-image and,
consequently, trajectory.
The fun and enjoyment group consisted predominantly of males (71%),
which was not surprising. However, the security group was predominantly
female (67%), which seems incongruent with this group's high
frequency of violent crimes. In order to better understand the effects
of gender, exploratory t-test analysis was conducted on selected groups.
Gender was the independent variable and the 17 delinquency items were
the dependent variables. Results indicated that males in the fun and
enjoyment group more frequently engaged in shoplifting, physical
assault, and physical extortion, while females more frequently ran away.
For the group that had security as the dominant value, males more
frequently engaged in breaking and entering, physical assault, vehicle
theft, and vandalism, and were more frequently suspended/expelled from
school and arrested. It is interesting to note that there was no gender
difference in crimes involving weapons. Security as a dominant value
seems to be a marker for violent delinquents of both sexes, but an
especially good marker for violent females, who may be difficult to
detect by other means.
An exploratory analysis of the fun and enjoyment group revealed no
gender differences in frequency of substance use. The dominant value of
fun and enjoyment thus appears to be a good marker for both male and
female substance users. Analysis of the sense of belonging group
revealed no gender differences in frequency of sex or number of
partners. Sense of belonging appears to inhibit sexual activity for both
males and females. Examination of the being well-respected group
revealed no gender differences in the number of sexual partners. For
this sample, adolescents of both sexes who valued being well-respected
tended to have more partners, implying that this behavior increases
respect and enhances social status.
Additional research is needed to fully understand the relationships
between values and various problem behaviors. Future studies should
incorporate a more comprehensive set of deviant activities and values
(e.g., Schwartz & Bilsky, 1990). Further, the effectiveness of
values self-confrontation should be examined for various problem
behaviors and across different samples, such as incarcerated youth.
REFERENCES
Agnew, R. (1985). A revised strain theory of delinquency. Social
Forces, 64, 151-167.
Allen, J. P., Leadbeater, B. J., & Aber, J. L. (1990). The
relationship of adolescents' expectations and values to
delinquency, hard drug use, and unprotected sexual intercourse.
Development and Psychopathology, 2, 85-98.
Ball-Rokeach, S. J., Rokeach, M., & Grube, J. W. (1984). The
great American values test: Influencing behavior and belief through
television. New York: Free Press.
Bernard, T. J. (1984). Control criticisms of strain theories: An
assessment of theoretical and empirical adequacy. Journal of Research in
Crime and Delinquency, 2(4), 353-372.
Block, J., Block, J. H., & Keys, S. (1988). Longitudinally
foretelling drug usage in adolescence: Early childhood personality and
environmental precursors. Child Development, 59, 336-355.
Braithwaite, J., & Law, H. (1985). Structure of human values:
Testing the adequacy of the Rokeach Value Survey. Journal of Personality
and Social Psychology, 49, 250-263.
Cloward, R. A., & Olin, L. (1960). Delinquency and opportunity:
A theory of delinquent gangs. New York: Free Press.
Conroy, W. J. (1979). Human values, smoking behavior, and public
health programs. In M. Rokeach (Ed.), Understanding human values:
Individual and societal (pp. 199-209). New York: Free Press.
Eisler, J. R. (1987). The expression of attitude. New York:
Springer-Verlag.
Farely, F. H., & Sewell, T. (1976). Test of an arousal theory
of delinquency. Criminal Justice and Behavior, 3, 315-320.
Felson, R. B., Liska, A. E., South, S. J., & McNulty, T. L.
(1994). The subculture of violence and delinquency: Individual vs.
school context effects. Social Forces, 73, 155-173.
Flores, P. J. (1986). Alcoholism treatment and the relationship of
Native American cultural values to recovery. International Journal of
Addictions, 20, 1707-1726.
Greenberg, D. (1977). Delinquency and the age structure of society.
Contemporary Crisis, 1, 66-86.
Grube, J. W., Mayton, D. M., II, & Ball-Rokeach, S. J. (1994).
Inducing change in values, attitudes, and behaviors: Belief systems
theory and the method of value self-confrontation. Journal of Social
Issues, 50(4), 153-173.
Grube, J. W., Weir, I. L., Getzlaf, S., & Rokeach, M. (1984).
Own value system, value images, and cigarette smoking. Personality and
Social Psychology Bulletin, 10, 306-313.
Guerra, N. G. (1989). Consequential thinking and self-reported
delinquency in high school youth. Criminal Justice and Behavior, 16(4),
440-454.
Hirschi, T. (1969). Causes of delinquency. Berkeley, CA: University
of California Press.
Homer, P. M., & Kahle, L. R. (1988). A structural equation test
of the value-attitude-behavior hierarchy. Journal of Personality and
Social Psychology, 54(4), 638-646.
Hurrelmann, K., & Engel, U. (1992). Delinquency as a symptom of
adolescents' orientation toward status and success. Journal of
Youth and Adolescence, 21(1), 119-138.
Jessor, S., & Jessor, R. (1975). Transition from virginity to
nonvirginity among youth: A social-psychological study over time.
Developmental Psychology, 11, 473-484.
Kahle, L. R. (Ed.). (1983). Social values and social change:
Adaptation to life in America. New York: Praeger.
Kahle, L. R. (1986). The nine nations of North America and the
value basis of geographic segmentation. Journal of Marketing, 50, 37-47.
Kamakura, W. A., & Novak, T. P. (1992). Value-system
segmentation: Exploring the meaning of LOV. Journal of Consumer
Research, 19(2), 119-132.
Kennedy, L. W., & Baron, S. W. (1993). Routine activities and a
subculture of violence: A study of violence on the street. Journal of
Research in Crime and Delinquency, 30(1), 88-112.
Koch, P. B. (1988). The relationship of first intercourse to later
sexual functioning concerns of adolescents. Journal of Adolescent
Research, 3(3-4), 345-362.
Kristiansen, C. M., & Matheson, K. (1990). Value conflict,
value justification, and attitudes toward nuclear weapons. The Journal
of Social Psychology, 130(5), 665-676.
Kristiansen, C. M., & Zanna, M. P. (1994). The rhetorical use
of values to justify social and intergroup attitudes. Journal of Social
Issues, 50, 47-65.
Miller, W. B. (1958). Lower class culture as a generating milieu of
gang delinquency. Journal of Social Issues, 14, 5-19.
Offir, J. T., Fisher, J. D., Williams, S. S., & Fisher, W. A.
(1993). Reasons for inconsistent AIDS-preventive behavior among gay men.
Journal of Sex Research, 30(1), 62-69.
Rokeach, M. (1973). The nature of human values. New York: Free
Press.
Schwartz, S. H. (1994). Are there universal aspects in the
structure and content of human values? Journal of Social Issues, 50,
19-45.
Schwartz, S. H., & Bilsky, W. (1990). Toward a theory of the
universal content and structure of values: Extensions and cross-cultural
replications. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 58(5),
878-891.
Schwartz, S. H., & Inbar-Saban, N. (1988). Value
self-confrontation as a method to aid in weight loss. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 54(3), 396-404.
Seigal, L. J., & Senna, J. J. (1991). Juvenile delinquency (4th
ed.). St. Paul: West Publishing.
Simons, R. L., Whitbeck, L. B., Conger, R. D., & Conger, K. J.
(1991). Parenting factors, social skills, and value commitments as
precursors to school failure, involvement with deviant peers, and
delinquent behavior. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 20, 645-664.
Simons, R. L., Whitlock, L. B., Conger, R. D., & Melby, J. N.
(1991). The effect of social skills, values, peers, and depression on
adolescent substance use. Journal of Early Adolescence, 11(4), 466-481.
Sorenson, R. (1973). The Sorenson Report: Adolescent sexuality in
contemporary America. New York: World Publishing.
Tetlock, P. E. (1986). A value pluralism model of ideological
reasoning. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 50, 819-827.
Weis, D. (1983). Affective reactions of women to their initial
experience of coitus. Journal of Sex Research, 19, 202-237.
Williams, R. M., Jr. (1979). Change and stability in values and
value systems: A sociological perspective. In M. Rokeach (Ed.),
Understanding human values: Individual and societal (pp. 15-46). New
York: Free Press.
Wolfgang, M. E., & Ferracuti, F. (1967). The subculture of
violence. New York: Tavistock.