STRESS-BUFFERING FACTORS RELATED TO ADOLESCENT COPING: A PATH ANALYSIS.
Printz, Brian L. ; Shermis, Mark D. ; Webb, Patrick M. 等
ABSTRACT
To uncover those factors that buffer the impact of stressful
negative experiences on adolescent adjustment, a theoretical model of
adolescent stress and coping, with social support and social problem
solving proposed as moderators, was investigated using path analysis.
The study was conducted with 122 ninth and tenth-grade nonreferred high
school students. Using the LISREL statistical package (Joreskog &
Sorbom, 1986), it was found that a recursive loop leading from stress
outcomes back to negative stressors did not allow for a successful
solution to the model. However, the effects of stressful events on
adjustment were mediated by coping resources, which included a
combination of problem-solving abilities and social support. Overall,
the findings replicated previous investigations that have demonstrated
direct relationships among stressful life events, social support,
problem solving, and adolescent adjustment. While a successful fit to
the theoretical model was not attained, it was concluded that a refined
model may provide a more acceptable solution.
INTRODUCTION
Unhealthy adaptation to stress can take many forms, such as school
maladjustment. For example, stressors at home and school may lead to
reduced attention span and to diminished motivation to succeed
academically (Pryor-Brown & Cowen, 1989). Some students develop
socially maladaptive coping patterns, including verbal and physical
aggression toward others, defiance of authority, acting out, and
juvenile delinquency (Compas, Howell, Phares, Williams, & Giunta,
1989). Anxiety (Swearingen & Cohen, 1985a, 1985b), depression, and
suicidal
ideation (Cohen-Sandier, Berman, & King, 1982) are other
reactions to stress. Moreover, some youths experience
psychophysiological symptoms in response to chronic or severe levels of
stress (Walker & Greene, 1987).
The availability of coping resources varies considerably among
students. During times of high stress, these resources, whether social
or individual, may be insufficient. When coping resources, such as
problem-solving skills, are inadequate, stressful situations may give
rise to unhealthy outcomes (Spirito, Overholser, & Vinnick, 1995;
Wills, Vaccaro, & Benson, 1995).
Research has sought to explicate factors associated with
adolescents' stress-related coping. Models have been constructed to
identify the key variables in the stress-adjustment relationship (see
Lazarus & Folkman, 1984; Petersen & Spiga, 1982; Rice, Herman,
& Petersen, 1993; Shermis & Coleman, 1990). However, such models
have rarely been examined using rigorous statistical analyses that allow
for modification of the particular model to provide for a goodness of
fit of key variables based upon the findings. Moreover, there continues
to be little understanding as to how the results of such investigations
can be applied to interventions in the area of adolescent health. The
intent of the present investigation was to specify and test a model of
stress-related coping among high school students, examine the
model's key components, and utilize this information to provide
guidelines for school-based interventions to address adolescent health
issues.
Shermis and Coleman Model
While adult models of stress and coping processes have been
postulated (e.g., Lazarus & Folkman, 1984), there is a paucity of
models for adolescents. Shermis and Coleman (1990) have offered a
cognitive-behavioral model of adolescent stress and coping (see Figure
1). It has five major components: environmental stressors, environmental
moderators, personal factors, stress outcomes, and behavioral outcomes.
Environmental stressors include daily hassles (e.g., getting
involved in an argument, experiencing bad weather, having plans change
unexpectedly) and major life events (e.g., parental divorce, death of a
friend or relative, serious illness or injury), with differential
effects (Compas, 1987b). Environmental moderators include support from
family members, peers, and school personnel. These individuals may offer
advice, teach skills, provide material aid, help the adolescent overcome
emotional distress, and share responsibilities (Compas, 1987a). Shermis
and Coleman (1990) suggest that it is the adolescent's perception
of support that actually determines the extent to which the effects of
stress are moderated.
Personal factors--cognition being prominent--also may impact
affective and behavioral outcomes. Shermis and Coleman (1990) have
identified self-talk as one form of cognitive coping. An earlier
conceptualization (Chandler, 1985) included age, cognitive appraisal
(e.g., perceptions of threat or loss, perceptions of control),
self-esteem, and problem-solving skills as personal moderating factors.
Stress outcomes may include physical and psychological symptoms.
Chandler (1985) noted the outcomes of stress in the areas of school
functioning and social relations.
Finally, behavioral outcomes, which are linked to stress outcomes,
can be thought of as secondary responses to stress. Shermis and Coleman
(1990) have listed drug abuse, delinquency, pregnancy, and dropping out
of school as maladaptive behaviorial responses to stress.
Stressful Life Events
The emerging consensus within the literature is that the experience
of stressful events during childhood greatly increases an
individual's vulnerability to behavioral and psychological
maladjustment, in addition to physical illness. Pearson correlation
coefficients have been found to range from .10 to .68, with the majority
of studies reporting coefficients between .20 and .30 (see Compas,
1987b). In general, major life events were predictive of daily hassles,
which in turn were associated with adjustment.
Typically, life stress models conceptualize mediating factors as
being either personal or environmental. For example, investigations of
personal factors have examined demographic variables (Dohrenwend,
Krasnoff, Askenasy, & Dohrenwend, 1978), temperament (Kagan, 1983),
attachment and separation during infancy (Ainsworth, 1979), social
problem solving (Mullins, Siegal, & Hodges, 1985), and Type A and B
behavior patterns (Dweck & Wortman, 1982). Investigations of
environmental factors have focused on social support as a resource for
coping (Barrera, 1981; Compas, Wagner, Slavin, & Vannatta, 1986;
Pryor-Brown & Cowen, 1989; Walker & Greene, 1987). The present
study targeted those factors that can be influenced via school-based
interventions (i.e., social support and problem solving). Moderating
variables that are relatively permanent or immune to treatment (i.e.,
temperament, personality type, and attachment during infancy) were not
assessed.
Social Support
Perceived social support is the cognitive appraisal of the presence
and quality of interpersonal ties (Barrera, 1986). For school-aged
children, these sources of support primarily include family members,
school personnel, and peers.
Overall, research suggests that perceived social support and
psychological and physical symptoms are negatively correlated (Billings
& Moos, 1981). For example, results of three path analytic studies
(Dean & Ensel, 1982; Lin & Dean, 1984; Lin & Ensel, 1984)
indicate that perceived social support is negatively correlated with
depressive symptoms.
The results have been mixed concerning the stress-buffering role of
social support. Several investigations with child and adolescent samples
(Compas et al., 1986; Gad & Johnson, 1980; Rowlison & Felner,
1988) did not note any significant interactions among life events,
social support, and symptom levels. In contrast, Walker and Greene
(1987) found that as negative life events increased, adolescent males
with low peer support reported significantly more symptoms than did
males with high peer support. Additionally, they noted that at low
levels of life stress, peer support was not significantly associated
with males' symptomatology. However, peer support appeared more
necessary for the well-being of adolescent females, regardless of the
level of life stress. Family support was negatively associated with
males' and females' reports of symptomatology.
The study of social support has produced findings consistent with
several different models of stress, support, and adjustment. While
critics (Barrera, 1986; Compas, 1987a; Heller & Swindle, 1983) do
not rule out the possibility of the stress-buffering position, they
indicate that conceptual and methodological deficiencies hinder
definitive conclusions.
Social Problem Solving
Problem solving has been proposed as a moderating variable in
models of stress and coping (e.g., Nezu, 1987). Studies by Spivack,
Platt, Shure, and their associates have investigated deficits in the
ability to generate multiple ways of solving a problem. Means-ends
performance among impulsive adolescents at a residential treatment
center (Spivack & Levine, 1963), adolescent psychiatric patients
(Platt, Spivack, Altman, Altman, & Peizer, 1974), and emotionally
disturbed children (Larcen, Spivack, & Shure, 1972; Shure &
Spivack, 1972) was found to be markedly lower than that of control
groups.
Investigations of adult populations (Heppner & Anderson, 1985;
Heppner & Petersen, 1982; Heppner, Hibel, Neal, Weinstein, &
Rabinowitz, 1982; Heppner, Reeder, & Larson, 1983; Nezu, 1985, 1986)
have demonstrated the mediating role of self-perceived problem-solving
ability. Individuals who rated themselves as ineffective at solving
problems were more likely to exhibit depressive symptomatology (Nezu,
1986), reported more problems (Heppner et al., 1982) and general
symptoms of maladjustment (Heppner & Anderson, 1985), and had more
irrational beliefs and lower self-concepts (Heppner et al., 1983).
Examinations of adolescent self-appraised problem-solving abilities are
needed.
In short, the ultimate objective is to identify the relationships
among key components of adolescent coping. Thus, the purpose of this
study was to determine how well the empirical data from an adolescent
sample fit the conceptual model presented by Shermis and Coleman (1990).
METHOD
Subjects
One hundred twenty-two high school students, 65 males (53%) and 57
females (47%), served as subjects. They were between 14 and 19 years old
(M = 15.25, SD = 0.98). Thirty-nine were freshmen, 79 were sophomores, 3
were juniors, and 1 was a senior. These students were not receiving
special education services.
Sixty-seven students lived with their biological mother and father,
23 lived with a biological parent and a stepparent, 28 lived with one
parent, and 4 had adoptive parents or lived with other relatives. The
racial distribution of the students was as follows: Anglo (n = 74, 61%),
Asian (n = 21, 17%), Hispanic (n = 19, 16%), African-American (n = 6,
5%), and other racial groups (n = 2, 2%). The high school from which
they were drawn was located in a lower-middle-class to middle-class
suburb of Houston, Texas.
Procedure
Teachers from six health classes explained the purpose of the study
to their students. Informed consent (parent and child) was obtained for
144 of the 160 eligible students (90%).
To reduce order effects, the five instruments, administered over
two days, were counterbalanced. Twenty-two students did not complete the
assessment phase for various reasons (e.g., absence from class). Those
whose scores indicated that they were experiencing a. high degree of
emotional distress were contacted, along with their parents, and
referral information and counseling opportunities were provided.
Instrumentation
Environmental stressors. The high school version of the Adolescent
Perceived Events Scale (APES; Compas, Davis, Forsythe, & Wagner,
1987) includes separate scales for daily (APES-D) and major (APESM)
events, and for positive and negative events. Compas et al. (1987)
reported high test-retest reliability over a two-week period. Several
studies examining relationships between negative life events and
psychological and behavioral symptoms have employed the APES. For
example, using weighted negative events and the Hopkins Symptom
Checklist (Derogatis, Lipman, Rickels, Uhlenhuth, & Covi, 1974),
Wagner, Compas, and Howell (1988) reported correlations ranging from .23
to .32 for major events, and from .54 to .68 for daily events.
Social support. The Perceived Social Support self-report scales
(PSS; Procidano & Heller, 1983) are designed to assess perceptions
of the adequacy of support from family (PSS-Fa) and friends (PSS-Fr).
Procidano and Heller reported high internal consistency (alpha
coefficients of .88 for the PSS-Fa and .90 for the PSS-Fr).
Social problem solving. The self-report Social Problem-Solving
Inventory (SPSI; D'Zurilla & Nezu, 1988) measures social
problem-solving ability and consists of 10 subscales comprising 70
items. Two composite scales, the Problem Orientation Scale (POS) and the
Problem Solving Skills Scale (PSSS), were of interest here.
D'Zurilla and Nezu (1988) reported test-retest coefficients of .83
for the POS and .88 for the PSSS. Internal consistency was also high
(alpha coefficients of .89 and .81, respectively). Concurrent validity was assessed by comparing the SPSI with the Problem Solving Inventory
(PSI; Heppner & Petersen, 1982) and the Means-Ends Problem-Solving
Procedure (MEPS; Platt & Spivack, 1975). Correlations between the
PSI and the P05 and PSSS were - .62 and - .69, respectively.
Correlations between the MEPS and the POS and PSSS were .61 and .65,
respectively.
Psychological adjustment. The Stress Response Scale (SRS; Chandler,
1983) is a 40-item behavior rating instrument that was designed to
assess the emotional adjustment reactions of children. Teachers or
parents are required to rate children's behaviors on a six-point
scale. The SRS is derived from Chandler's (1983) stress response
model and is supported by factor analyses (Chandler, 1982). The
five-factor solution that forms the basis for the subscales--Impulsive
(acting out), Passive-Aggressive, Impulsive (overactive), Repressed, and
Dependent--accounted for 62% of the variance in a clinically referred
sample (Chandler, 1982), 64% of the variance in a nonreferred sample
(Chandler & Lundahl, 1983), and 65% of the variance with a randomly
selected group (n = 962) of children (Shermis & Chandler, 1985).
The Youth Self-Report (YSR; Achenbach & Edelbrock, 1987)
consists of 112 items taken from the Child Behavior Checklist (CBCL;
Achenback & Edelbrock, 1983, 1986) and reworded in the first person.
Designed for youth aged 11-18, the YSR is a relatively stable measure of
self-reported social competence and behavior problems. Achenbach and
Edelbrock (1987), examining a sample of youths referred to a community
mental health facility, reported good test-retest reliability (.69) for
total behavior problem scores over a 6-month period. They also found a
high degree of consistency between CBCL ratings (by mothers and
clinicians) and YSR ratings at the time of intake and at 6-month
follow-up.
Data Analysis
The hypotheses were tested using techniques appropriate for latent
variable models, including covariance structure analysis (CSA) and
factor analysis. CSA allows for testing of bidirectional relationships
between variables, as well as relationships involving recursive loops.
In addition, CSA and path analysis permit statements concerning the
direction of causal flow between variables ("causal" refers to
the degree of impact one variable, or a set of variables, has on another
variable). Thus, they are a step beyond the predictive power of
traditional correlational and regression analyses. Factor analysis is
commonly used to confirm the relationships among the structural and
measured variables, or as an exploratory tool to assess problems within
the observed model.
CSA entails the construction of diagrams that specify causal flow
among the variables within a model, and then solving for these
relationships (Loehlin, 1992). A path diagram, initially constructed
based on existing knowledge or assumptions, depicts relationships among
latent (theoretical) and/or observed variables. Each of the
relationships in a path model can be expressed with a linear structural
equation. There will be numerous structural equations to account for a
given model. The CSA process involves solving for the unknown
coefficients (which are indicative of the strength of the causal
relationships) in each of the equations simultaneously, thus reducing
the amount of error (Aaronson, Frey, & Boyd, 1988). The goal is to
seek the most parsimonious explanation for the data. Through the process
of simultaneous iteration, computer programs such as LISREL6 (Joreskog
& Sorbom, 1986) and SIMPLIS (Joreskog & Sorbom, 1987) use matrix
algebra to solve for the unknown path coefficients and to test the
degree of variance, or "fit," between the implied
(theoretical) and the observed models.
LISREL6, the computer program used in the present investigation, is
based on the LISREL model, which provides for the inclusion of both
latent and observed variables. Observed, or measured, variables are
thought to be indicators for latent variables. A latent variable is best
estimated when it serves as a composite for several observed variables,
provided that the observed variables
are valid and reliable indicators of the theoretical construct.
The LISREL model requires the construction of a structural model
and a measurement model. The structural model consists of the
relationships among the theoretical constructs, or latent variables. For
this study, the structural model was composed of four latent variables,
as illustrated in Figure 2: negative stressors, perceived social
support, problem solving, and stress outcomes. In contrast, the
measurement model, as shown in Figure 3, identified the observed
variables (i.e., instruments or indices) used to measure the latent
variables in the structural model. Five pencil-and-paper instruments,
including several subscales, were used in the measurement model: APES
(APES-D, APES-M), PSS (PSS-Fr, PSS-Fa), SPSI (POS, PSSS), SRS, and the
YSR. With the exception of the SRS, which involved teacher reports, all
were youth self-report measures.
RESULTS
An initial correlation matrix was generated that included each of
the variables present in the model. Although a covariance matrix could
have been generated for the input variables, the correlation matrix was
substituted to facilitate interpretation of the results. Because
correlations are standardized variables, they allow for more
straightforward comparisons across variables.
The initial model proposed that all of the latent variables be
placed on the Y-side (right) of the model. This allowed for a feedback
loop from stress outcomes back to environmental stressors. Further,
paths between each of the mediator variables (social problem solving,
social support) and stress outcome variables were identified. Personal
factors were excluded due to insufficient sample size.
Specification of the initial model failed to produce a significant
match between the data and the model. A [x.sup.2] value of 58.22 with 9
degrees of freedom (p [less than] .001) and a goodness of fit (GFI)
index of .901 was obtained. Moreover, some of the residuals proved to be
negative. Further, the SRS did not perform as expected; instead, it
performed in the reverse direction of the YSR. In addition, when
personal factors were included in the model, the problems were
exacerbated. These findings indicated that adjustments to the model were
necessary.
Respecification of the Model
In order to identify the sources of error in the original model,
several methods suggested by Joreskog and Sorbom (1986) were employed:
(1) consideration of alternative plausible models, (2) factor analysis,
and (3) elimination of constructed variables. First, however, problems
encountered in attempting to fit the original model necessitated the
exploration of a more parsimonious relationship among the data. The
initial step in reducing the complexity of the model involved removing
the reciprocal path leading from stress outcomes back to negative
stressors. In addition, the negative stressor variable and measures were
placed on the X-side of the model, leaving the mediator and outcome
variables on the Y-side. Due to its poor performance, the SRS was
eliminated from the analysis, which left only the YSR as a measure of
stress outcomes. Since it could not be assumed that the YSR was a
perfect measure of the stress outcome variable, the YSR's
reliability was used as a best estimate of the path from the latent v
ariable to YSR. Furthermore, based on LISREL's analysis of where
the structural model was most problematic, a direct path from negative
stressors to stress outcomes was included. Once these changes were
implemented, several variations of the model were explored.
Alternative plausible models. The strength of each of the mediator
values was tested. In succession, single paths leading from the
mediating variables to the stress outcome variable were removed to
determine their individual contribution to the model. Each of the trials
remained consistent with the underlying theoretical framework of the
original model. The models produced identical [X.sup.2] values (39.54,
df = 6, p[less than].001) and GFI values (.912), while adjusted GFIs
ranged from .591 to .650, depending on df in the particular model. These
findings suggested that each of the trials solved for the model equally
well, though none of the exploratory models provided an acceptable fit
for the data. To further assess problems with the model and to construct
a better model, a confirmatory factor analysis was undertaken.
Factor analysis. To assess the possibility that a poorly
constructed or implemented measurement model was responsible for the
poor fit with the data, a confirmatory factor analysis was conducted.
This involved allowing all of the variables between the latent variables
in the original model (excluding SRS and personal characteristics) to be
correlated. No change was detected, [X.sup.2](6) = 39.54, p = .001,
suggesting that the poor fit may have been due to the measurement model.
Once the original model was rejected, the nature of the
investigation became exploratory. An exploratory factor analysis was
conducted in which the SRS was removed due to its poor performance,
while the YSR was included since it was the only indicator for the
stress outcomes variable. Using orthogonal rotation, two underlying
factors were uncovered. As would be expected, the APES-M and APES-D
loaded on one factor, identified as "negative stressors."
Contrary to original projections, though in keeping with a more
parsimonious conceptualization, the PSS-Fa, PSS-Fr, POS, and PSSS loaded
on the second factor, identified as "coping resources."
Elimination of constructed variables. Indicators produced by the
initial test, together with the results of the exploratory factor
analysis, suggested that significant problems were inherent in the
model. Due to these problems, the LISREL program was unable to generate
t values and standard error estimates on the path coefficients. The
significance and strength of the paths in the model could not be
assessed without these indices. Further, the program located a major
problem with the path from the PSS-Fr variable to the perceived support
latent variable.
Due to the problematic nature of the PSS-Fr variable, it was
important to determine whether eliminating perceived support's
direct impact on stress outcomes would enhance the model. However,
models that allowed both problem solving and perceived support to have a
direct impact on stress outcomes demonstrated a significantly better fit
of the data, [X.sup.2]diff(1) = 5.33, p [less than].05.
Based on the results of the factor analysis and trials with various
models, a more parsimonious model was explored, involving three latent
variables: negative stressors, coping resources, and stress outcomes.
Only one mediating latent variable, consisting of the PSS-Fr, PSS-Fa,
POS, and PSSS, was used in the final model (see Figure 4). Further, a
path between the residuals of the PSS-Fr and the PSSS was freed, since a
correlation between the two variables may have accounted for some of the
variance in the model. The correlation matrix for the respecified model
is shown in Table 1.
Assessment of the Goodness of Fit of the Respecified Model
Minimization programs typically produce several indicators of the
goodness of fit of a model (Joreskog & Sorbom, 1987). The LISREL6
program generates the chi-square statistic, multiple correlation
coefficients, GFI and adjusted GFI, root mean square residual (RMR), as
well as a Q-plot of data, which assess the efficacy of a model. Perhaps
most commonly reported is the chi-square statistic; a close
approximation of the model to the data is indicated when the ratio of
the chisquare to the degrees of freedom is small. Path analysis of the
final model produced a relatively large chi-square (32.86, df = 11,p
[less than] .001), suggesting that the respecifled model (three latent
variables) did not fit the data, although the improvement over the
original model was significant, [X.sup.2]diff(2) 25.36, p [less than]
.00 1.
Squared multiple correlation coefficients indicate that a small
amount of the variance was accounted for by the mediating variable
([R.sup.2] = .204), while stress outcomes accounted for a significant
amount of the variance ([R.sup.2] = .873). Thus, the model does not
account for all of the variance; factors outside of the model appear to
be impacting the dependent variables.
The GFI, a measure of the amount of variance between the implied
and the observed variables (Joreskog & Sorbom, 1987), for this model
was .934 (.831 when adjusted for degrees of freedom). These two indices
are relatively independent of the sample size and are affected minimally
by departures from normality (Joreskog & Sorbom, 1987).
The root mean square residual (RMR), a measure of the average size
of the estimated residuals, for the final model was .096. The RMR
statistic is most valuable for comparing the effectiveness of separate
models (Joreskog & Sorbom, 1987). Among the models tested, the final
model had the lowest RMR.
DISCUSSION
Similar to the earlier work of Chandler and Shermis (1990), the
present study failed to support the notion that maladjustment
exacerbates the amount of negative life stress experienced by high
school students--the model's recursive loop was found to be
ineffective. However, findings on the role of negative life events were
consistent with other research indicating that an accumulation of
stressful experiences increases vulnerability to maladjustment (Barrera,
1981; Compas et al., 1989; Compas & Phares, 1986; Gad & Johnson,
1980; Sandier, 1980). Most studies have reported correlations between
stress and adjustment ranging from .20 to .30 (see Compas, 1987b). This
study found a similar correlation for major events (r = .33, p [less
than] .05), but a higher correlation for daily events (r = .46, p [less
than] .001).
The latter finding suggests that adolescent adjustment is
influenced less by discrete events than by chronic stressors (Compas,
1987b). While demonstrating some predictive power in the present study,
major events proved to be a poor indicator of student distress in
several previous investigations (e.g., Wagner, Compas, & Howell,
1988). Here, this variable was causally related to daily events, which,
in turn, impacted adolescent adjustment. Moreover, other researchers
(Compas et al., 1989; Compas, Wagner, Slavin, & Vannatta, 1986) have
argued that the reciprocal relationship between stress and
symptomatology excludes major life stressors.
While a successful fit between the respecified model and the data
was not attained, a trend in the findings supported previous research on
the stress-adjustment relationship and mediating factors (Peterson &
Spiga, 1982; Sherniis & Coleman, 1990). High school students'
perceptions of available coping resources, including social support and
problem-solving abilities, significantly buffered the impact of
stressors on symptomatology. When examined separately, students'
problem-solving abilities and perceptions of social support from friends
and family were not powerful enough to reduce the effects of negative
stress in their lives.
In the attempts to find a solution for the structural model, a
major source of the variance between the implied and observed models was
attributed to the path between a measurement variable, PSS-Fr (peer
support), and its predetermined latent variable, social support. PSS-Fr
was found to correlate significantly (r = .39, p [less than] .005) with
PSSS (problem-solving skills). In addition, the degree of association
between PSS-Fr and POS (problem orientation), an indicator of cognitive
appraisal rather than social skill, was nonsignificant (r = .13). These
results suggest that support from friends and support from family, as
coping resources, serve unequivocal functions.
Soliciting support from peers requires social competencies (the
ability to build and maintain friendships). The difference in the
correlations between peer support and the two problem-solving
measurement variables supports the contention that PSS-Fr scores are
indicative of social skills. Similarly, Procidano and Heller (1983)
found that the PSS-Fr was more highly associated with measures of social
competence than was the PSS-Fa. Taken together, these findings suggest
that general orientation to solving problems and perceptions of family
functioning better predict adolescent adjustment problems than do social
skills.
Support from friends did not have a significant impact on distress.
This may indicate that even during adolescence, the support received
from family members is more vital for healthy functioning than is the
quality of affective bonds with friends. Alternatively, it may suggest
that peers serve as a secondary coping resource; their help is solicited
when the family proves nonsupportive.
Like the two support variables, problem orientation and
problem-solving skills may play very different roles. Students'
problem orientation was more highly associated with measures of stress
and adjustment, while problem-solving skills were more closely
associated with perceived support from friends. Thus, cognitive
appraisal of effectiveness at resolving problems appeared to have a
greater impact than did actual skill (see Nezu, 1987).
Overall, these data for a nonreferred sample of ninth- and
tenth-grade students provide some indication that coping resource
variables are impacted by stressful events, and consequently impact
self-rated adjustment. While scores suggest that students had adequate
problem-solving skills, it appears that those with a negative
orientation to problems were less successful at coping with stress. More
specifically, cognitive and affective factors related to problem
orientation may have impacted the ability of some of the students to
employ effective coping strategies. Although the stress-buffering
hypothesis was not supported with the combined social problem solving
construct, it is highly probable that problem orientation, along with
other cognitive factors, plays a moderating role. Further research into
the nature of the cognitive factors that hinder effective coping is
needed.
A limitation of this study was its reliance on self-report measures
to assess the latent variables. In addition, methodological problems
associated with the use of the SRS, a teacher-rated measure of student
adjustment, severely limited the strength of the stress outcomes
variable (large classrooms meant teachers lacked familiarity with
students). Since the SRS did not provide discriminatory power, the YSR
served as the lone index of adjustment. Future investigations should
incorporate reports from others, observational data, and clinical
interviews. Furthermore, assessments of other hypothesized moderators
(e.g., school-based social support, cognitions, and temperament), as
well as inclusion of several outcomes of stress (e.g., academic
performance and additional forms of symptomatology), would be valuable.
CONCLUSIONS
The primary source of stress for adolescents appears to be chronic
interpersonal and nonsocial problems. When faced with a seemingly
overwhelming accumulation of micro-stressors (i.e., daily hassles), they
may feel less capable of solving their problems, despite having the
necessary skills. Instead, they often resort to avoidance, shift causal
attributions to factors beyond their control, or adopt irrational
beliefs. Therefore, interventions that focus on improving
adolescents' orientation to problems--helping them effectively
resolve daily stressors--are recommended for reducing symptomatology
(see D'Zurilla, 1988).
The assessment of stressors, problem solving, and social support
offers valuable insight into adolescents' perceptions of their
environment and ability to cope. Their problem orientation provides an
indicator of coping efficacy, while appraisal of problem-solving skills
suggests areas in need of improvement. Finally, indices of family
support reveal the availability of important coping resources.
The assessment of life events, problem orientation, problem-solving
skills, and social support should prove especially helpful to mental
health professionals working at the high school level. Students
exhibiting symptoms of maladjustment can be offered group counseling
based on their specific needs and situation; for example, those
requiring social skills training or cognitive restructuring, those who
are experiencing family conflict versus peer relationship problems, and
those who would benefit from general stress management techniques (e.g.,
relaxation training). Further examination of the respecified model of
adolescent coping should be conducted, with appropriate linkages to
school-based interventions.
This article is based on the first author's doctoral
dissertation, completed at the University of Texas at Austin.
Mark D. Shermis, Ph.D., Director, Testing Center, and Associate
Professor, Department of Psychology, Indiana University-Purdue
University Indianapolis.
Patrick M. Webb, Ph.D., Testing Center and Department of
Psychology, Indiana University-Purdue University Indianapolis.
Reprint requests to Brian Printz, Ph.D., Director of Special
Education, Pikes Peak B.O.C.E.S., 4825 Lorna Place, Colorado Springs,
Colorado 80915.
REFERENCES
Aaronson, L. S., Frey, M. A., & Boyd, C. J. (1988). Structural
equation models and nursing research (part 2). Nursing Research, 37(5),
315-318.
Achenbach, T. M., & Edelbrock, C. (1983). Manual for the Child
Behavior Checklist and Revised Child Behavior Profile. Burlington, VT:
Department of Psychiatry, University of Vermont.
Achenbach, T. M., & Edelbrock, C. (1986). Manual for the
teacher version of the Child Behavior Checklist and Profile. Burlington
VT: Author.
Achenbach, T. M., & Edelbrock, C. (1987). Manual for the Youth
Self-Report. Burlington, VT: Department of Psychiatry, University of
Vermont.
Ainsworth, M. D. S. (1979). Infant-mother attachment. American
Psychologist, 34, 932-937.
Barrera, M., Jr. (1981). Social support's role in the
adjustment of pregnant adolescents: Assessment issues and findings. In
B. H. Gottleib (Ed.), Social networks and social support in community
mental health. Beverly Hills, CA: Sage.
Barrera, M., Jr. (1986). Distinctions between social support
concepts, measures, and models. American Journal of Community
Psychology, 14, 413-445.
Billings, A. G., & Moos, R. H. (1981). The role of coping
responses and social resources in attenuating the stress of life events.
Journal of Behavioral Medicine, 4, 139-157.
Chandler, L. A. (1982). The Stress Response Scale: A preliminary
report. Paper presented at the convention of American Psychological
Association, Washington, DC.
Chandler, L. A. (1983). The Stress Response Scale: An instrument
for use in assessing emotional adjustment reactions. School Psychology
Review, 12, 260-265.
Chandler, L. A. (1985). Assessing stress in children. New York:
Praeger.
Chandler, L. A., & Lundahl, W. T. (1983). Empirical
classification of emotional adjustment reactions. American Journal of
Orthopsychiatry, 53, 460-467.
Chandler, L. A., & Shermis, M. D. (1990). A paradigm for the
study of childhood stress. In J. H. Humphrey (Ed.), Human stress:
Current selected research (Vol. 4). New York: AMS Press.
Cohen, L. H., Burt, C. E., & Bjork, J. P. (1987). Effects of
life events experienced by young adolescents and their parents.
Developmental Psychology, 23, 583-592.
Cohen-Sandler, R., Berman, A. L., & King, R. A. (1982). Life
stress and symptomatology: Determinants of suicidal behavior in
children. Journal of the American Academy of Child Psychiatry, 21,
178-186.
Compas, B. E. (1987a). Coping with stress during childhood and
adolescence. Psychological Bulletin, 101, 393-403.
Compas, B. E. (1987b). Stress and life events during childhood and
adolescence. Clinical Psychology Review, 7, 275-302.
Compas, B. E., Davis, G. E., Forsythe, C. J., & Wagner, B. M.
(1987). Assessment of major and daily life events during adolescence:
The Adolescent Perceived Events Scale. Journal of Consulting and
Clinical Psychology, 55, 534-541.
Compas, B. E., Howell, D. C., Phares, V., Williams, R. A., &
Giunta, C. T. (1989). Risk factors for emotional/behavioral problems in
young adolescents: A prospective analysis of adolescent and parental
stress and symptoms. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 57,
732-740.
Compas, B. E., & Phares, V. (1986, August). Child and parental
stress and symptomatology: An integrative analysis. Paper presented at
the annual meeting of the American Psychological Association,
Washington, DC.
Compas, B. E., Wagner, B. M., Slavin, L. A., & Vannatta,
K.(1986). A prospective study of life events, social support, and
psychological symptomatology during the transition from high school to
college. American Journal of Community Psychology, 14, 241-257.
Dean, A., & Ensel, W. M. (1982). Modelling social support, life
events, competence, and depression in the context of age and sex.
Journal of Community Psychology, 10, 392-408.
Derogatis, L. R., Lipman, R. S., Rickels, K., Uhlenhuth, E. H.,
& Covi, L. (1974). The Hopkins Symptom Checklist.
Pharmacopsychiatry, 7,79-110.
Dobrenwend, B. S., Krasnoff, L., Askenasy, A. R., & Dohrenwend,
B. P. (1978). Exemplification of a method for scaling life events: The
PERI Life Events Scale. Journal of Health and Social Behavior, 19,
225-239.
Dweck, C. S., & Wortman, C. B. (1982). Learned helplessness,
anxiety, and achievement motivation: Neglected parallels in cognitive,
affective, and coping responses. In H. W. Krohne & L. Laux (Eds.),
Achievement, stress, and anxiety (pp. 93-125). Washington, DC:
Hemisphere.
D'Zurilla, T. J. (1988). Problem-solving therapies. In K. S.
Dobson (Ed.), Handbook of cognitive-behavioral therapies. New York:
Guilford Press.
D'Zurilla, T. J., & Nezu, A. (1982). Social problem
solving in adults. In P. C. Kendall (Ed.), Advances in
cognitive-behavioral research and therapy (Vol. 1, pp. 201-274). New
York: Academic Press.
D'Zurilla, T. J., & Nezu, A. M. (1988). Development and
preliminary evaluation of the Social Problem-Solving Inventory (SPSI).
Manuscript submitted for publication.
Gad, M. T., & Johnson, J. H. (1980). Correlates of adolescent
life stress as related to race, SES, and levels of perceived social
support. Journal of Clinical Child Psychology, 9, 13-16.
Gersten, J. C., Languer, T. S., Eisenberg, J. G., & Orzek, L.
(1974). Child behaviors and life events: Undesirable change or change
per se? In B. S. Dobrenwend & B. P. Dohrenwend (Eds.), Stressful
life events: Their nature and effects. New York: Wiley.
Heller, K, & Swindle, R. W. (1983). The effects of social
support: Prevention and treatment implications. In R. D. Felner, L. A.
Jason, J. N. Moritsugn, & S. S. Farber (Eds.), Preventive
psychology: Theory, research and practice (pp. 87-103). New York:
Pergamon Press.
Heppner, P. P., & Anderson, W. P. (1985). The relationship
between problem solving self-appraisal and psychological adjustment.
Cognitive Therapy and Research, 9, 415-427.
Heppner, P. P., Baumgardner, A., & Jackson, J. (1985).
Problem-solving self-appraisal, depression, and attributional style: Are
they related? Cognitive Therapy and Research, 9, 105-113.
Heppner, P. P., Hibel, J. H., Neal, G. W., Weinstein, C. L., &
Rabinowitz, F. E. (1982). Personal problem solving: A descriptive study
of individual differences. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 29,
580-590.
Heppner, P. P., Kampa, M., & Brunning, L. (1984, August).
Problem-solving appraisal as a mediator of stress. Paper presented at
the annual meeting of the American Psychological Association, Toronto.
Heppner, P. P., & Petersen, C. H. (1982). The development and
implications of a personal problem solving inventory. Journal of
Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 29, 66-75.
Heppner, P. P., Reeder, B. L., & Larson, L. M. (1983).
Cognitive variables associated with personal problem-solving appraisal:
Implications for counseling. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 30,
537-545.
Joreskog, K. G., & Sorbom, D. (1986). LISREL6: Computer
program. Moorseville, IN: Scientific Software, Inc.
Joreskog, K. G., & Sorbom, D. (1987). SIMPLIS: Users guide.
Moorseville, IN: Scientific Software, Inc.
Kagan, J. (1983). Stress and coping in early development. In N.
Garmezy & M. Rutter (Eds.), Stress, coping, and development in
children. New York: McGraw-Hill.
Larcen, S., Spivack, G., & Shure, M. B. (1972, April).
Problem-solving thinking and adjustment among dependent-neglected
preadolescents. Paper presented at the meeting of the Eastern
Psychological Association, Boston.
Lazarus, R. S., & Folkman, S. (1984). Stress, appraisal, and
coping. New York: Springer.
Lin, N., & Dean, A. (1984). Social support and depression: A
panel study. Social Psychiatry, 19, 83-91.
Lin, N., & Ensel, W. M. (1984). Depression-mobility and its
social etiology: The role of life events and social support. Journal of
Health and Social Behavior, 25, 176-188.
Loehlin, J. C. (1992). Latent variable models: An introduction to
factor, path, and structural analysis (2nd ed.). New Jersey: Lawrence
Erlbaum Associates.
Mullins, L. L., Siegal, L. J., & Hodges, K. (1985). Cognitive
problem-solving and life event correlates of depressive symptoms in
children. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 13, 305-314.
Nezu, A. M. (1985). Differences in psychological distress between
effective and ineffective problem solvers. Journal of Counseling
Psychology, 32, 135-138.
Nezu, A. M. (1986). Cognitive appraisal of problem-solving
effectiveness: Relation to depression and depressive symptoms. Journal
of Clinical Psychology, 42, 42-48.
Nezu, A. M. (1987). A problem-solving formulation of depression: A
literature review and proposal of a pluralistic model. Clinical
Psychology Review, 7, 121-144.
Nezu, A. M., & Ronan, G. F. (1985). Life stress, current
problems, problem solving, and depressive symptoms: An integrative
model. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 53, 693-697.
Pearlin, L. I., Menaghan, E. G., Lieberman, M. A., & Mullan, J.
T. (1981). The stress process. Journal of Health and Social Behavior,
22, 337-356.
Perkins, D. V. (1982). The assessment of stress using life events
scales. In L. Goldberger & S. Breznitz (Eds.), Handbook of stress:
Theoretical and clinical aspects. New York: MacMillan.
Petersen, A. C., & Spiga, R. (1982). Adolescence and stress. In
L. Goldberger & S. Breznitz (Eds.), Handbook of stress: Theoretical
and clinical aspects. New York: MacMillan.
Platt, J.J., & Spivack, G. (1972). Problem solving thinking of
psychiatric patients. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 39,
148-151.
Platt, J.J., & Spivack, G. (1973). Studies in problem-solving
thinking of psychiatric patients: Patient-control differences and
factorial structure of problem-solving thinking. Proceedings of the 81st
Annual Convention of the American Psychological Association, 8, 461-462.
Platt, J.J., & Spivack, G. (1975). Manual for the Means-Ends
Problem-Solving Procedure (MEPS). Philadelphia: Hahnemann Community
Mental Health/Mental Retardation Center.
Platt, J. J., Spivack, G., Altman, N., Altman, D., & Peizer, S.
B. (1974). Adolescent problem solving thinking. Journal of Consulting
and Clinical Psychology, 42, 787-793.
Procidano, M. E., & Heller, K. (1983). Measures of perceived
support from friends and family: Three validation studies. American
Journal of Community Psychology, 11, 1-24.
Pryor-Brown, L., & Cowen, E. L. (1989). Stressful life events,
support, and children's school adjustment. Journal of Clinical
Child Psychology, 18, 214-220.
Rice, K. G., Herman, M. A., & Petersen, A. C. (1993). Coping
with challenge in adolescence: A conceptual model and psycho-educational
intervention. Journal of Adolescence, 16, 235-251.
Rowlison, R. T., & Felner, R. D. (1988). Major life events,
hassles, and adaptation in adolescence: Confounding in the
conceptualization and measurement of life stress and adjustment
revisited. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 55, 432-444.
Sandler, R. N. (1980). Social support resources, stress and
maladjustment of poor children. American Journal of Community
Psychology, 8, 41-52.
Sandler, I. N., & Block, M. (1979). Life stress and
maladaptation of children. American Journal of Community Psychology, 7,
425-439.
Shermis, M. D., & Chandler, L. A. (1985). A confirmatory factor
analysis of the Stress Response Scale. Unpublished manuscript,
University of Pittsburgh.
Shermis, M. D., & Coleman, M. (1990). Stress and coping: A
cognitive-behavioral model. Unpublished manuscript.
Shermis, M. D., Rudin, D., & Chandler, L.A. (1990). An
extension of the norms for the Stress Response Scale for Children. Paper
presented at the annual meeting of the American Educational Research
Association, Boston.
Shure, M. B., & Spivack, G. (1972). Means-ends thinking,
adjustment and social class among elementary school aged children.
Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 38, 348-353.
Spirito, A., Overholser, J., & Vinnick, L. (1995). Adolescent
suicide attempters in general hospitals: Psychological evaluation and
disposition planning. In J. L. Wallander, L. J. Siegel et al. (Eds.),
Adolescent health problems: Behavioral perspectives, advances in
pediatric psychology (pp. 97-116). New York: Guilford Press.
Spivack, G., & Levine, M. (1963). Self-regulation in acting-out
and normal adolescents (Report M-4531). Washington, DC: National
Institute of Mental Health.
Swearingen, E. M., & Cohen, L. H. (1985a). Measurement of
adolescent's life events: The Junior High Life Experiences Survey.
American Journal of Community Psychology, 13, 69-85.
Swearingen, E. M., & Cohen, L. H. (1985b). Life events and
psychological distress: A propsective study of young adolescents.
Developmental Psychology, 21, 1045-1054.
Wagner, B. M., Compas, B. E., & Howell, D. C. (1988). Daily and
major life events: A test of an integrative model of psychosocial
stress. American Journal of Community Psychology, 16, 189-205.
Walker, L. S., & Greene, J. W. (1987). Negative life events,
psychosocial resources, and psychophysiological symptoms in adolescents.
Journal of Clinical Child Psychology, 16, 29-36.
Wills, T. A., Vaccaro, D., & Benson, G. (1995). Coping and
competence in adolescent alcohol and drug use. In J. L. Wallander, L. J.
Siegel et al. (Eds.), Adolescent health problems: Behavioral
perspectives, advances in pediatric psychology (pp. 160-178). New York:
Guilford Press.
Correlations of the Variables Involved in Model Verification
Variables SRS YSR APES-D APES-M PSS-Fr PSS-Fa
SRS 1.000
YSR .139 1.000
APES-D -.078 .457 [****] 1.000
APES-M -.050 .332 [*] .389 [****] 1.000
PSS-Fr -.130 -.131 .188 .200 1.000
PSS-Fa .032 -.496 [****] -.279 -.134 .245 1.000
POS -.063 -.450 [****] -.272 -.192 .133 .370 [***]
PSSS -.229 -.119 .009 .127 .387 [***] .262
Variables POS PSSS
SRS
YSR
APES-D
APES-M
PSS-Fr
PSS-Fa
POS 1.000
PSSS .351 [*] 1.00
(*.)p [less than] .05
(**.)p [less than] .01
(***.)p [less than] .005
(****.)p [less than] .001