Effects on conformity and nonconformity to gender-role expectations for dress: teachers versus students.
Workman, Jane E. ; Johnson, Kim K.P.
Many schools have regulations that prohibit students from wearing
certain articles of dress. Dress is defined by Roach and Musa (1980) as
the total arrangement of all outwardly detectible modifications of the
body itself and all material objects added to it. Thus, an earring would
properly be classified as an item of dress. Some dress regulations are
applied uniformly to both male and female students as in the case of
regulations that prohibit students from wearing shorts ("Pupil
protest . . .", 1988). Others are applied to either males or
females. For example, one school required boys to wear their hair at a
"conventional length" while girls were not allowed to wear
extremely tight or short skirts (Hambleton, Roach, & Ehle, 1972).
The basis of some dress codes appears to be tied to traditional
gender-role expectations for dress ("Students' attire . .
.", 1979). Because regulations are not always applied uniformly, as
in the case of male students but not female students being prohibited
from wearing earrings, they sometimes evoke controversy (Allis, 1989;
Mullen, 1985; Stafford, 1987).
The controversy over young men wearing earrings is reminiscent of the
1960s controversy over men wearing long hair. Long hair on men was
viewed as a challenge to the status quo and was assumed to reflect a
lack of self-discipline (Simpson, 1973). When males first began to wear
their hair long, many people reacted negatively because the appearance
did not conform to traditional masculine role expectations (David &
Brannon, 1976). To some people, long hair and femininity seemed
synonymous (Plumb, 1967). Perhaps regulations concerning young males
wearing earrings reflect a similar phenomenon. Therefore, the purpose of
this study was to examine the effects of conformity and nonconformity to
masculine gender-role expectations for dress, specifically the effects
of wearing and not wearing an earring, on evaluations of a male student
by teachers and students.
Theoretical Framework
It is impossible to look at an object or person without identifying
it as something. Instead of treating all objects or persons as
different, individuals group them into cognitive categories based on
their similarities (Rosch, 1973). Included in these categories are
expectations for appearance and behavior. Two fundamental bases for
categorization are gender and age (Horn & Gurel, 1981). For example,
when a person is categorized and a gender label applied, such as
masculine or feminine, others may behave toward that person on the basis
of the expectations they hold for individuals so labeled. The cluster of
expectations for behavior of individuals in a particular category is
called a role (Chafetz, 1978).
Beliefs exist in every society concerning the roles that are
appropriate for each gender (Rosencrantz, Vogel, Bee, Broverman, &
Broverman, 1968). According to Basow (1980), gender-role stereotypes
demarcate the behaviors and appearance to which each gender is expected
to conform. Thus, males are expected to have a different appearance from
females. Expectations for appearance can influence social interaction
because they serve as standards by which to conform, against which to
rebel, or with which to evaluate others. When males conform or do not
conform to these expectations, it may affect how others perceive them.
Role theory would predict that conformity to gender-role expectations
is rewarding, both to those who hold the expectations and to those who
conform because it facilitates social interaction (Stryker &
Stathem, 1985). However, conforming to gender-role expectations can be
complicated by contradictory expectations originating from different
reference groups. For example, expectations for appearance of an
adolescent male from peers may include adopting a traditionally feminine
item (e.g., an earring), while expectations from teachers may include
maintaining a traditionally masculine appearance. In a school setting,
wearing an item of dress that conforms to peer expectations may enhance
social acceptance and opportunities for social interaction. However,
nonconformity may result in negative consequences from school
authorities (Allis, 1989; Mullen, 1985; Stafford, 1987;
"Students' attire . . .", 1979). For example, when
students violate dress codes, they may be sent home to change and, in
extreme cases, expelled from school (Allis, 1989; "Students'
attire . . .", 1979).
Additionally, there is a prevailing assumption that individuals are
better adjusted if they conform to gender-role expectations (Bem, 1976).
Males displaying behavior associated with a masculine gender role have
been judged as more well-adjusted, more likeable, and more physically
attractive as compared to males displaying behavior consistent with a
feminine gender role (Shinar, 1978). Because mental health and social
acceptability may be evaluated solely by an individual's
conformance to gender-role expectations, in many situations it may be
socially rewarding to conform (Basow, 1980).
Consequences for Social Interaction
A major social interaction arena affected by gender-role expectations
is the school--where teachers' evaluations are significant.
According to Adams (1978), students' personal characteristics have
associated stereotypic expectations. Research has demonstrated that a
variety of student characteristics can influence teachers'
evaluations and expectations. Although the effect of dress on
teachers' evaluations has not been investigated, previous research
has shown that students' physical attractiveness, gender, conduct,
and social class, among other factors, affect evaluations by teachers
(Adams, 1978; Adams & LaVoie, 1974).
Several investigations have shown that teachers expect physically
attractive students, in comparison to unattractive students, to be more
academically successful and socially responsive (Adams & Cohen,
1976; Clifford & Walster, 1973; Dion, 1972; Rich, 1975). Adams and
Cohen (1976) found that teachers interacted more frequently and
positively with attractive children. Clifford and Walster (1973) found
that attractive students were perceived by teachers to possess a higher
IQ, greater educational potential, and more interested parents. Dion
(1972) found that attractive children were less likely to receive
punishment for incorrect responses in a learning task. Rich (1975) found
that attractive children were rated more favorably on personal and
academic development, intelligence, and competence.
Nakdimen (1984) contended that when a mental status report included
the notation that a patient looked attractive, it was an evaluation that
the patient's appearance conformed to traditional gender-role
expectations. It might be expected, then, that conformity to gender-role
expectations for dress would result in more favorable ratings of
attractiveness. Further, since it has been demonstrated that attractive
children receive more favorable ratings on factors such as intelligence,
educational potential, social responsiveness, personal development, and
competence, conformity to gender-role expectations for dress might
result in favorable ratings of these same factors.
Gender is also a characteristic that has been shown to affect
teacher-student interactions and expectations. Brophy and Good (1970)
found that boys received significantly more behavior criticism than did
girls. Sadker and Sadker (1973) concluded that elementary school boys
received more active attention from their teachers in the form of
disapproval and general interaction, than did girls. Adams and Lavoie
(1974) found that boys were rated lower than girls on attitudes toward
school, work habits, and personal attitudes. Rich (1975) found that
unattractive girls were blamed less frequently for a misbehavior and
received more lenient recommendations for punishment than did
unattractive boys. Rosen (1977) asked subjects to rate four individuals
whose actual gender was discrepant with the apparent gender assumed from
dress or physical appearance. After an initial rating, the subjects were
informed of the biological gender of these four individuals and were
asked to rate them a second time. The individuals were identical between
two viewings, except that a gender label was changed. Gender-consistent
stereotypes were invoked and social judgments were affected as the
gender label changed the viewer's perception. Results from these
studies suggest that males who do not conform to gender-role
expectations for dress may be presenting an appearance that stimulates
attention, criticism, and disapproval from teachers.
Conduct has been shown to bias teacher assessments, and boys have
generally been rated somewhat more negatively than girls (Lippitt &
Gold, 1959; Sears & Feldman, 1966). LaVoie and Adams (1974) found
that good-conduct students, regardless of gender and level of
attractiveness, were rated higher on ability measures and leadership
potential and were predicted to pursue more post high school education
and to obtain higher status vocations than did poor-conduct children.
Adams and LaVoie (1974) found that good-conduct students were rated
significantly higher by teachers than poor-conduct students on parental
interest and involvement, peer relations, personal and school attitudes,
and work habits. Nonconformity with dress codes (and not coincidentally,
nonconformity with traditional gender-role expectations) may be viewed
by school authorities as a conduct problem, and students who do not
conform are subjected to disciplinary actions.
Another significant reference group for adolescents is peers.
Researchers have concluded that in a school setting, dress that conforms
to peer expectations may enhance social acceptability and opportunities
for social interaction with peers (Allen & Eicher, 1973; Creekmore,
1980; Littrell & Eicher, 1973; Morganosky & Creekmore, 1981).
Allen and Eicher (1973) investigated friendship choice and choice for
peer group membership as a function of violation of dress norms.
Respondents were presented with stories about three hypothetical
adolescents, each of whom violated the dress norms accepted in the
school where the study was conducted. In every instance, a higher
percentage of respondents rejected the adolescents as "group"
members than as friends, an indication that some respondents could
accept the adolescent as a personal friend, but did not believe the
adolescent would fit into the "group."
Creekmore (1980) assessed the personal attractiveness of 228 high
school students (males = 119; females = 109) and the attractiveness of
their clothing. Generally, attractive students wore attractive clothing
and were aware of and conformed to the dress worn by most students.
Attractive students were more likely to be accepted by their peers, to
be selected for important roles in the high school situation, and to be
more active in school activities than students assessed as less
attractive.
Research by Morganosky and Creekmore (1981) suggested that attractive
clothing may actually be more important to male than to female
adolescent leaders. They suggested that perhaps gender-role expectations
for females, whether leaders or not, include an awareness of appearance
styles. Conversely, gender-role expectations for males do not include
clothing awareness and thus males may be more likely to be able to
distinguish themselves through such an awareness.
Littrell and Eicher (1973) studied opinions about clothing as a
variable in the movement of adolescents from social isolation to social
acceptance. These researchers found that clothing opinions
differentiated individuals who had become accepted in peer friendship
groups from those not accepted. When adolescents had opinions concerning
clothing and appearance similar to those of their reference groups, they
were more likely to become members of their groups than when they had
unlike opinions.
Male students may tend to conform more to clothing norms of peers
than do females (Hambleton, Roach, & Ehle, 1972). According to
Warden and Colquett (1982), adolescent males perceive social acceptance
to be related to conformity in dress. Warden and Colquett found that 64%
of their sample of 260 adolescent males reported that friends were the
most frequent source of information about clothing they might want to
buy.
These findings suggest that conformity and nonconformity to
gender-role expectations for masculine dress may affect the following
student and teacher evaluations: academic ability, post high school
training, vocation, leadership potential, interpersonal skills,
attractiveness, conduct, parental attitudes, and personal traits.
Therefore, the present study was undertaken to evaluate the effects of
conformity and nonconformity to masculine gender-role expectations for
dress on teachers' and students' evaluations.
METHOD
Subjects
Junior high school students served as one group of subjects for the
study. There were 51 students (22 male, 27 female, and two for which
data on gender were missing) with an average age of 12.80 (range = 11 to
15). Students who volunteered to participate in this study represented
Grades 6 (n = 9), 7 (n = 22), 8 (n = 19), and 9 (n = 1). The other group
of subjects was 52 teachers (14 male, 37 female, and 1 missing data)
whose average age was 37.41 (range = 22 to 60). Teachers indicated they
had taught for an average of 12 years (range = 1 to 30). Thirty-two
teachers had a bachelor's degree and 20 had a master's degree.
Experimental materials
Two head and shoulders photographs were taken of a young male
student; in one he was wearing an earring in his left ear and in the
other he was not. The earring was a faux diamond stud about 1/4" in
diameter which utilized a magnetic back to give the appearance that the
ear was pierced. The photographs were taken under identical conditions
by a professional photographer. Hair style, facial expression, and other
aspects of appearance were kept constant.
Personal traits were measured by means of a 20-item questionnaire.
Items included were each of the following and their negation:
intelligent, honest, happy, kind, independent, well-adjusted, hard
working, competent, respectful, healthy, likeable, active, obedient,
persistent, attractive, cheerful, masculine, popular, creative,
feminine. Items included in the scale were adapted from Rich (1975) and
Stephan and Langlois (1984). Each item was presented in a 7-point Likert
type format with end points of the word (e.g., intelligent) and its
negation (e.g., not intelligent).
Educational performance was measured by items adapted from Adams and
LaVoie (1975). These included: IQ (1 = below 85 to 6 = over 145); grade
point average (1 = 1.5-2.0 to 5 = 3.6-4.0); percentile rank in class (1
= below 25th to 4 = above 75th); highest educational level (1 = finish
high school to 5 = doctorate); and future vocation (1 = laborer to 5 =
professional).
Parental attitudes were measured by items adapted from Adams and
LaVoie (1975). These included parents' attitude toward school (7 =
strong interest to 1 = strong indifference); involvement in school (7 =
very active to 1 = not very active); interest in student's school
performance (5 = interested to 1 = not interested); permissiveness (5 =
not permissive to 1 = permissive); discipline practices (5 = reasoning
to 1 = physical punishment); and social class (1 = lower lower to 9 =
upper upper).
A behavior vignette was presented which described an incident in
which a boy is tripped and falls down a flight of stairs (Rich, 1975).
Subjects were asked to indicate, using a 7-point Likert-type format, how
positive they were that the boy pictured (Michael) tripped the other
student, how undesirable the act was, and the extent and intensity of
punishment they would recommend.
Estimates of how well the student would interact with others were
measured by responses to eleven questions adapted from Roth and Isenberg
(1983) and Adams and LaVoie (1975). Each question was accompanied by a
7-point scale with end points of 7 = agree and 1 = disagree. Subjects
were asked to indicate their degree of agreement with each of the
following statements:
1. The student would do well in school.
2. The student would mind the teacher in school.
3. The student would do what the group wants even if he didn't
want to.
4. The student would be elected as a class officer.
5. The student would respond to other people.
6. The student would not cause parents problems.
7. The student would like to be with other people.
8. The student has a strong attachment to his mother.
9. The student would get along with others.
10. The student would understand another person's point of view.
11. The student would be disruptive in class.
Design
The study was an experiment with two variations of dress (earring, no
earring) and two groups of subjects (students, teachers). Data were
collected in large group settings, specifically, in physical education
classes for students and at faculty meetings for teachers. Each subject
viewed one of the two photographs of the male student either wearing or
not wearing an earring, and responded to the dependent measures. Data
were analyzed using Cronbach's alpha, factor analyses, analyses of
variance, and Student-Newman-Keuls test.
RESULTS
Personal Traits
A factor analysis of the 20-item personal traits questionnaire
resulted in 6 factors. Each was subjected to a reliability test
(Cronbach's alpha), and those factors with an acceptable
reliability (alpha greater than .65) were subjected to further analysis.
These criteria were based on Touliatos and Compton's (1988)
discussion of reliability. Four factors met this requirement. The first
consisted of five items: obedient, respectful, hardworking, intelligent,
independent and was called "Conduct" (Cronbach's alpha =
.78). The second factor consisted of four items: happy, cheerful,
healthy, likeable and was called "Disposition"
(Cronbach's alpha = .75). The third factor consisted of three
items: attractive, popular, masculine and was called "Social
acceptability" (Cronbach's alpha = .76). The fourth factor
consisted of three items: honest, competent, kind and was called
"Trustworthy" (Cronbach's alpha = .75).
Analysis of variance on the first factor, Conduct, revealed a
significant 2-way interaction between subject and dress, F(1, 91) =
5.58, p |is less than~ .02. Students, but not teachers, rated the
student higher on the items constituting Conduct when he was wearing an
earring than when he was not.
Analysis of variance on the third factor, Social acceptability,
resulted in a significant main effect for subject, F(1, 97) = 6.68, p
|is less than~ .01 and a significant 2-way interaction between subject
and dress, F(1, 97) = 5.76, p |is less than~ .03. The main effect was a
result of teachers rating the student as significantly more socially
acceptable (M = 16.13) than students (M = 14.06). Student-Newman-Keuls
test revealed that students rated the student as least socially
acceptable when he was not wearing an earring and as significantly less
socially acceptable than all of the other three groups (p |is less than~
.05).
Table 1
Mean Scores on Factor "Conduct" Dress by Subject Interaction
Dress
Subjects Earring No Earring
Students 23.75(*) 20.26(*)
(n = 51)
Teachers 22.11 23.04
(n = 52)
Note. Means were significantly different from each other, p |is less than~
.05.
Analysis of variance on the second factor, Disposition, and the
fourth factor, Trustworthy, revealed no significant differences based on
the independent variables.
There was a significant main effect for subjects, F(1, 92) = 7.46, p
|is less than~ .008 and a significant 2-way interaction between subjects
and dress, F(1, 92) = 16.06, p |is less than~ .03, on ratings of
attractiveness. The main effect was a result of teachers rating the
student higher on attractiveness (M = 5.34) than did students (M =
4.31). Student-Newman-Keuls test revealed that students rated the
student as least attractive when he was not wearing an earring and as
significantly less attractive than all of the other three groups.
Table 2
Mean Scores on Factor "Social Acceptability" Depicting Dress by Subject
Interaction
Dress
Subjects Earring No earring
Students 15.42 12.96(*)
(n = 51)
Teachers 15.54 16.76
(n = 52)
Note. Mean was significantly different from all other means, p |is less than~
.05.
Educational Performance
There was no significant difference based on the independent
variables on the dependent variables of intelligence, GPA, percentile
rank in class, highest educational level he would attain, or his future
vocation.
Parental Attitudes
Analysis of variance on items measuring parental attitudes resulted
in no significant differences in predictions of the parents'
attitudes toward school, parental involvement in school, parental
permissiveness, or parental discipline practices. Both teachers and
students agreed that this student's parents were average in
permissiveness (M = 2.83; range = 1-5), that they favored reasoning (M =
3.73; range = 1-5) as a method of discipline, that their attitude toward
school showed moderate interest (M = 5.04; range = 1-7), and that their
involvement in school was moderate (M = 4.27; range = 1-7). There was a
significant main effect for dress, F(1, 98) = 3.90, p |is less than~ .05
for parental interest in student's performance. When the student
wore an earring, parental interest in the student's performance was
rated lower (M = 3.59) than when he did not wear an earring (M = 4.02).
Table 3
Mean Scores on "Attractiveness" by Dress by Subject
Dress
Subjects Earrings No Earring
Students 4.92 3.72(*)
(n = 51)
Teachers 5.13 5.57
(n = 52)
Note. Mean was significantly different from all other means, p |is less than~
.05.
Analysis of variance of ratings of social class revealed a main
effect for dress, F(1, 93) = 5.46, p |is less than~ .03 and a
significant 2-way interaction, F(1, 92) = 3.82, p |is less than~ .05,
between subject and dress. The main effect was a result of subjects
assigning the student wearing an earring (M = 5.63) a lower social class
than when not wearing an earring (M = 6.56). Student-Newman-Keuls test
revealed that students, but not teachers, assigned a lower social class
to the student when he was wearing an earring than when he was not
wearing an earring.
Behavior Vignette
Analysis of variance on how positive subjects were that Michael
tripped the other boy revealed a main effect for subjects, F(1, 93) =
4.42, p |is less than~ .04). The main effect revealed students were more
positive that the student tripped the other boy (M = 5.48) than were
teachers (M = 4.94). There was a significant main effect, F(1, 93) =
16.79, p |is less than~ .001 for subject on how undesirable the act was,
with teachers rating the act as significantly more undesirable (M =
2.11) than did students (M = 3.54). There was a significant main effect
for subject, F(1, 93) = 24.99, p |is less than~ .001, for the
appropriate intensity of punishment, with teachers recommending stronger
punishment (M = 2.55) than did students (M = 4.02). There was no
significant difference based on the independent variables on how sure
subjects were that the student should be punished.
Table 4
Mean Scores on "Social class" by Dress by Subject
Dress
Subjects Earring No Earring
Students 5.36(*) 7.04(*)
(n = 51)
Teachers 5.96 6.08
(n = 52)
Note. Means were significantly different from each other, p |is less than~
.05.
Interpersonal Skills
A factor analysis of the 11-item scale designed to measure
interpersonal skills resulted in three factors. Each of these was
subjected to a reliability test (Cronbach's alpha), and those
factors with an acceptable reliability (alpha greater than .65) were
subjected to further analysis. One factor reached an acceptable level of
reliability. It consisted of 4 items: "understand another's
view," not be disruptive in class," "mind the
teacher," "get along with others" and was called
"Interpersonal skills" (Cronbach's alpha = .73). Ratings
on this factor were not influenced by the independent variables
manipulated.
DISCUSSION
It is notable that masculine grouped together with attractive and
popular into one factor, labeled social acceptability, and that this
factor was significantly affected by conformity to gender-role
expectations for dress. This lends support to Basow's (1980)
argument that social acceptability is judged by one's peers by the
extent to which one conforms to gender-role expectations. According to
role theory (Stryker & Stathem, 1985), the favorable evaluation
received from peers when wearing an earring could be interpreted as
evidence that he was conforming to peer expectations for a masculine
appearance. Students also rated him as having better conduct (i.e., as
more obedient, respectful, hard-working, intelligent, and independent)
than did teachers. Because the student was conforming to peer
expectations for dress, he was rated as more socially acceptable, and
therefore it follows that more positive evaluations of other social
traits would be likely to occur.
Results do not support Nakdimen's (1984) contention that
judgments of attractiveness are tied to conformity to traditional
gender-role expectations since peers found the nontraditional appearance
to be more attractive. Further, teachers did not differ in their
evaluations of attractiveness based on conformity or nonconformity to
gender-role expectations for appearance. In addition, wearing or not
wearing an earring did not result in differences in responses by
teachers to evaluations of social acceptability, conduct, disposition,
trustworthiness, measures of educational performance, or interpersonal
skills. Teachers were not told that the student was violating a dress
cede when they were asked to make these evaluations. Teachers'
tolerance of nonconformity to gender-role expectations for dress can be
explained as a reflection of their maturity and teacher training in
adolescent psychology, and perhaps their tolerance for whatever dress
code the school specifies.
Adams and LaVoie (1974) speculated that teachers attributed a
student's problem behavior to inadequate socialization by the
parents. Thus teachers may have believed that the nonconformity
reflected child-rearing practices more than the student's
characteristics. Both teachers and students agreed that this
student's parents were average in permissiveness, that they favored
reasoning as a method of discipline, that their attitude toward school
showed moderate interest, and that their involvement in school was
moderate; in others words, they were average parents with perhaps
laissez-faire child-rearing attitudes. Perceptions of parental interest
in the student's school performance was affected by his conformity
or nonconformity to gender-role expectations for dress. This was true
for both teachers and peers. It might be speculated that parents who
practice noninterference may tolerate nonconformity to expected
appearance in their children and thus may be perceived as less
interested in their children's school performance.
With regard to the behavior vignette, the significant differences
were all based on differing views between teachers and students. This
would be as expected due to differences in their roles and status.
Teachers have responsibility for maintaining an atmosphere conducive to
learning and are likely to judge infringements of rules more harshly
than do students. Students evaluated the student as having good conduct
and may have been inclined to give him the benefit of the doubt.
Student evaluation of social class was affected by the presence or
absence of the earring. Social class was judged to be significantly
lower when an earring was worn. This finding may reflect the historical
association of upper-class status with traditionality in clothing and
appearance (Horn & Gurel, 1981). Judgments of social class might
affect interaction between students because those of the same social
class tend to interact socially.
IMPLICATIONS
Research by Allen and Eicher (1973), Creekmore (1980), Littrell and
Eicher (1973), and Morganosky and Creekmore (1981) suggests that
students who are accepted by their peers would find school to be a
positive social environment. Results of the present research suggest
that conforming to peer expectations for dress may contribute to peer
acceptance. The rewarding aspects of peer acceptance and a positive
social environment might contribute to a tendency to participate in
school activities, to attend school regularly, and to stay in school
until graduation. Perhaps the regulations concerning the wearing of
earrings by males are much ado about nothing, similar to the regulations
concerning long hair in the 1970s. As the practice of wearing earrings
by males increases, it may well be perceived as a masculine gender-role
cue.
REFERENCES
Adams, G. (1978). Racial membership and physical attractiveness
effects on preschool teachers' expectations. Child Study Journal,
8(1), 29-41.
Adams, G., & Cohen, A. (1976). An examination of cumulative
folder information used by teachers in making differential judgments of
children's abilities. The Alberta Journal of Educational Research,
22(3), 216-225.
Adams, G., & LaVoie, J. (1974). The effect of student's sex,
conduct, and facial attractiveness on teacher expectancy. Education,
95(1), 76-83.
Adams, G., & LaVoie, J. (1975). Parental expectations of
educational and personal-social performance and childrearing patterns as
a function of attractiveness, sex, and conduct of the child. Child Study
Journal, 5(3), 125-142.
Allen, C, & Eicher, J. (1973). Adolescent girls' acceptance
and rejection based on appearance. Adolescence, 8(29), 125-138.
Allis, T. (1989, October 23). Controversy: Arguing that sloppy duds
make sloppy minds, schools fight to revive stricter dress codes. People,
32(17), 73-74, 76.
Basow, S. (1980). Sex-role stereotypes: Traditions and alternatives.
Monterey, CA: Brooks/Cole.
Bem, S. (1976). Probing the promise of androgyny. In A. Kaplan, &
J. Bean (Eds.), Beyond sex-role stereotypes: Readings toward a
psychology of androgyny. Boston: Little Brown, pp. 47-62.
Brophy, J., & Good, T. (1970). Teachers' communication of
differential expectations for children's classroom performance:
Some behavioral data. Journal of Educational Psychology, 61, 364-374.
Chafetz, J. (1978). Masculine/feminine or human? (2nd ed.). Itasca,
IL: Peacock.
Clifford, M., & Walster, E. (1973). The effect of physical
attractiveness on teacher expectations. Sociology of Education, 46,
248-258.
Creekmore, A. (1980). Clothing and personal attractiveness of
adolescents related to conformity, to clothing mode, peer acceptance,
and leadership potential. Home Economics Research Journal, 8(3),
203-215.
David, D., & Brannon, R. (1976). The forty-nine percent majority:
The male sex role. Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley.
Dion, K. (1972). Physical attractiveness and evaluations of
children's transgressions. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 24, 207-213.
Hambleton, K., Roach, M., & Ehle, K. (1972). Teenage appearance:
Conformity, preferences, and self-concepts. Journal of Home Economics,
64(2), 29-33.
Horn, M., & Gurel, L. (1981). The second skin. Boston: Houghton
Mifflin.
LaVoie, J., & Adams, G. (1974). Teacher expectancy and its
relation to physical and interpersonal characteristics of the child. The
Alberta Journal of Educational Research, 20(2), 122-132.
Lippitt, R., & Gold, M. (1959). Classroom social structures as a
mental health problem. Journal of Social Issues, 15, 40-49.
Littrell, M., & Eicher, J. (1973), Clothing opinions and the
social acceptance process among adolescents. Adolescence, 8(30),
197-212.
Morganosky, M., & Creekmore, A. (1981). Clothing influence in
adolescent leadership roles. Home Economics Research Journal, 9,
356-362.
Mullen, K. (1985, August 15). Committee working to standardize AISD dress codes. Dallas Times Herald, p. D-13.
Nakdimen, K. (1984, April). The physiognomic basis of sexual
stereotyping. American Journal of Psychiatry, 141(4), 499-503.
Plumb, J. (1967, Winter). A return to manliness. Horizon, 9(1),
100-101.
Pupil protest ploy puts males in miniskirts. (1988, May 16). Denton
Record-Chronicle, p. 12B.
Rich, J. (1975). Effects of children's physical attractiveness
on teachers' evaluations. Journal of Educational Psychology, 67(5),
599-609.
Roach, M., & Musa, K. (1980). New perspectives on the history of
Western dress. New York: NutriGuides, Inc.
Rosch, E. (1973). On the internal structure of perceptual and
semantic categories. In T. M. Moore (Ed.), Cognitive development and the
acquisition of language. New York: Academic Press, 111-114.
Rosen, A. (1977). Gender stereotypes, ascribed gender, and social
perception. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 45, 851-860.
Rosencranz, P., Vogel, S., Bee, H. Broverman, I., & Broverman, D.
(1968). Sex-role stereotypes and self-concepts in college students.
Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 32, 287-295.
Roth, K., & Isenberg, N. (1983). Effects of children's
heights on teacher's attributions of competence. The Journal of
Genetic Psychology, 143, 45-50.
Sadker, M., & Sadker, D. (1973). Sexual discrimination in the
elementary school. National Elementary Principal, 52, 41-45.
Sears, P., & Feldman, D. (1966). Teacher interactions. The
National Elementary Principal, 46, 30-36.
Shinar, E. (1978). Person perception as a function of occupation and
sex. Sex Roles, 4, 679-693.
Simpson, T. (1973, November). Real men, short hair. Intellectual
Digest, 3, 76, 78.
Stafford, L. (1987, September 2). Measuring up. Denton
Record-Chronicle, p. 2D.
Stephan, C., & Langlois, J. (1984). Baby beautiful: Adult
attributions of infant competence as a function of infant
attractiveness. Child Development, 55, 576-585.
Stryker, S., & Stathem, A. (1985). Symbolic interaction and role
theory. In G. Lindzey, & E. Aronson (Eds.), Handbook of social
psychology (3rd ed.). New York: Random House, 311-378.
Students' attire again HEW target. (1979, December 3). U.S. News
and World Report, 87, p. 14.
Touliatos, J., & Compton, N. (1988). Research methods in human
ecology/home economics. Ames: Iowa State University Press.
Warden, J., & Colquett, J. (1982, Spring). Clothing selection by
adolescent boys. Journal of Home Economics, 37-40.