Pubertal status, interaction with significant others, and self-esteem of adolescent girls.
Lackovic-Grgin, Katica ; Dekovic, Maja ; Opacic, Goran 等
Over the last decade an increased interest in the psychology of
self-concept has led to numerous studies which examined self-esteem as
an evaluative component of the self-concept. A review of recent studies
which examined self-esteem during adolescence leads to three
conclusions. First, the results of these studies are not systematized.
Several variables found to be related to self-esteem differ with regard
to their conceptual level: locus of control, physical appearance,
relationship with parents and peers, and change of residence or school
(Hirsch & Rapkin, 1987; Houston, 1984; Kulas, 1982; Marron &
Kayson, 1984). Second, very few studies examined the relative
contribution of these variables to the prediction of self-esteem (Buri,
Kirchner, & Walsh, 1987; Wade, Thompson, Tashakkori, & Valente,
1989). Given that the correlates of self-esteem tend to be interrelated,
examination of their independent effects seems to be the next necessary
step. Third, the studies that examined stability of self-esteem during
adolescence produced inconsistent results. This could be due to
differences in design and in the measures used to assess self-esteem.
Cross-sectional studies showed changes in self-esteem and decrease of
self-respect during middle adolescence (Rosenberg, 1979). On the other
hand, findings from the studies that used a longitudinal design
indicated relative stability or even a slight increase in self-esteem
during adolescence (Nottelman, 1987; Savin-Williams & Demo, 1984).
Mostly, changes of self-esteem during adolescence was seen as the
consequences of physical maturation or radical changes in the way of
living. The interactionist theory of self proposes, however, that the
interactions with significant others play a crucial role in the
development of self-esteem. This assumption has been tested and
confirmed in our research (Lackovic-Grgin, 1987; 1988; Lackovic-Grgin
& Dekovic, 1990). It seems therefore reasonable to expect that
changes in self-esteem could be related to changes in adolescents'
interactions with significant others.
From the psychological studies come evidence that the quality of
interaction between children and their parents is related to the
descriptive and evaluative aspects of self-concept (Burns, 1982; Papini
& Sebby, 1987). During adolescence the quality of interactions with
peers also becomes important (Lackovic-Grgin, 1988). Positive aspects of
interactions such as intimacy, acceptance, and nurturance are related to
higher self-esteem.
The biosocial studies pointed out that physical maturation affects
the quality of interactions between adolescents and significant others,
though these changes appear to be more subtle than is suggested by the
traditional conception of adolescence. Contrary to the traditional view
of adolescence as a period of stress and turmoil, it appears that the
increase in conflicts, larger distance, and less intimacy within the
parent-adolescent relationship characterized only the period at the apex
of pubertal maturation. These changes are especially evident in girls
(Steinberg, 1988; Steinberg & Hill, 1978).
With regard to the relationship between physical maturation and
self-esteem, no clear findings have been reported. Ulman (cited in
McGrory, 1990) found that girls who have been menstruating for two years
were more depressed than those who have not started to menstruate. Early
pubertal development has also been associated with lower self-esteem
(Wade et al., 1989). On the other hand, Simmons, Blyth, and McKinney
(1983) found no effects of onset and presence of menses on girls'
global self-image in early adolescence. Similarly, Brack, Donald, &
Ingersoll (1988) found no variation in self-esteem across Tanner's
five stages of physical development.
Based on these findings it seems unlikely that physical maturation
would have a direct effect on self-esteem of adolescent girls. It is
more logical to assume that the effects are indirect, mediated by the
changes in the adolescents' interactions with significant others.
The purpose of this study was to test these hypotheses.
From the previous cross-sectional studies that examined the effects
of physical maturation on different aspects of adolescent behavior and
development, it is difficult to conclude whether the effects found are
due to the pubertal status, chronological age, or their interaction.
Mostly, the design used involved selection of a wide age-range sample,
after which the pubertal status was assessed and its effects on several
dependent variables examined. In order to avoid such confounding between
age and pubertal maturation, the sample of this study consisted of girls
of the same chronological age.
Another problem involves the operationalization of pubertal
maturation. The most frequently used criterion for girls is the
occurrence of the first menstrual period. This criterion, aside from
being reliable and valid, also makes it possible to assess the amount of
time of physical maturation, a dimension that is especially important
because menarche is more than just a biological change. It is an
important life event which requires adjustment by the girl herself and
her environment. Thus the comparison of girls on this dimension seems
more appropriate than comparison based on timing of menarche relative to
the population. Relatively early or late maturation probably has some
effect on the relationship with peers, but the degree of physical
maturation seems to be more important to family members. For these
reasons the pubertal status in this study was operationalized in terms
of the number of months from the occurrence of the first menstrual
period to the time of investigation.
METHOD
Subjects
The sample for the study consisted of 178 girls from the eighth grade
of primary school in Zadar, Croatia (mean age, 13 years and 9 months).
Measures
The first part of the questionnaire included demographic variables,
date of the first menstrual period, and a question regarding academic
achievement (whether school performance was lower, the same, or higher
as compared with the previous school year).
Self-esteem. Subjects completed the shortened version of the
Coopersmith Self-Esteem Inventory (SEI) (Coopersmith, 1967), consisting
of 25 items which have been checked in a previous study for psychometric properties (Bezinovic & Lackovic-Grgin, 1990). The subjects were
asked to indicate for each item whether it is "like me" or
"not like me." A measure of self-esteem was derived by summing
the number of affirmative answers. The internal consistency, as
determined by Chronbach's alpha, was .88.
Interactions with significant others. The questionnaire which was
developed in our previous research (Lackovic-Grgin & Opacic 1989;
Opacic & Kos, 1987) consisted of three sets of items (mother,
father, and friend) measuring the following dimensions of interaction:
intimacy, control, and punitiveness (interaction with parents), and
intimacy and control (interaction with friends). Each of these
one-dimensional Likert-format scales consisted of eight items. An
example of the items measuring intimacy is: "I can talk to my
mother (father) about personal things"; an example of the items
measuring control: "My mother (father) tells me what I should or
should not do"; an example of the punitiveness items: "I
sometimes get undeserved physical punishment from my mother
(father)." Chronbach's alphas for intimacy, control, and
punitiveness in relationship with mother were .80, .64, and .75,
respectively. In the same order, the alphas for fathers were .91, .68,
and .75. Finally, the alphas for intimacy and control in the
relationship with friend were .60 and .58, respectively.
For each subject eight scale scores (three for each parent and two
for friend) were computed as unweighted averages of the items included
in the respective scales.
RESULTS
In order to determine if there is a relationship between pubertal
status (length of physical maturation) and self-esteem, the sample was
divided into four groups. The first consisted of girls who had not yet
menstruated (n = 27), the second were girls whose first menstrual period
had occurred within the past six months (n = 44), the third were girls
whose time of physical maturation was between 7 and 12 months (n = 78),
and the fourth group consisted of girls who have been menstruating 13 or
more months (n = 29). Though the sample was homogeneous with regard to
age, in order to check for possible age effects, two groups were formed:
girls above the mean age (167 months) and those below the mean age.
The 2 (age) x 4 (length of physical maturation) analysis of variance
revealed significant effect of physical maturation on self-esteem, F(3,
175) = 10.16, p [is less than] .05. Neither significant effects of age
nor significant interactions were found. As shown in Table 1 the girls
whose first menstrual period occurred more than a year ago had the
lowest self-esteem.
The next question concerned the relationship between girls'
self-esteem and pubertal status, and interaction variables with
significant others (their parents and friends). Highly significant
correlations TABULAR DATA OMITTED were found between self-esteem and
interaction with parents, but not with interaction with friends. All
correlations were in the predicted direction: higher self-esteem was
associated with more intimacy and a lower level of control and
punitiveness in the relationship with both mothers and fathers. Table 2
also shows the correlations between these variables and the time of
physical maturation. The time of physical maturation appears to be
associated with interactional variables; an especially high correlation
was found between maturation and experienced control by mothers.
Table 2
Means and Standard Deviations of Selected Variables and Their Correlations
with Self-esteem and Pubertal Maturation
Variable M SD SE PM
Age 167.79 5.57 .14 .15
School performance 1.81 .83 -.07 .03
Intimacy - mother 23.62 6.83 -.41(**) -.29(*)
Punitiveness - mother 10.24 5.91 .50(**) .37(**)
Control - mother 8.79 4.48 .46(**) .72(**)
Intimacy - father 16.54 7.62 -.32(*) -.24
Punitiveness - father 9.08 5.89 .48(**) .17
Control - father 8.53 4.79 .37(**) .38(**)
Intimacy - friend 10.01 2.86 -.12 .14
Control - friend 5.44 3.60 .02 .14
Self-esteem 9.03 5.33 -
Pubertal maturation 7.61 5.00 .31(*)
Note. SE = self-esteem; PM = length of pubertal maturation in months.
* p [is less than] .05. ** p [is less than] .01.
These findings suggest that the relationship between physical
maturation and self-esteem could be mediated by interactional variables.
In order to test this hypothesis, a stepwise regression analysis was
conducted with self-esteem as the criterion variable. In the first step,
all independent variables were entered. In each of the following steps
the predictor variables were eliminated from the regression until the
deletion of a variable in the next step yielded a significant decrement in the explained variance. Only the variables that remained in the
equation were considered as predictor variables.
In the first step, the girls' age, school performance, pubertal
maturation, and eight interaction variables explained 43% of variance in
their self-esteem. When viewed with other predictor variables, the
effect of pubertal maturation on self-esteem was not significant ([Beta]
= -.02; N.S.).
The elimination of variables: age, school performance, pubertal
maturation, intimacy with father, father's control, and intimacy
and control in relationships with friend, did not significantly decrease
the accuracy of the prediction. The variables that remained as
significant predictors in the last (eighth) regressed step were:
intimacy, control, and punitiveness in relationship with the mother and
punitiveness of the father. These variables explained 41% of the
variance in self-esteem. These results suggest that the effects of
pubertal maturation or self-esteem is indirect, mediated by the quality
of the relationship with significant others (especially the mother).
DISCUSSION
The findings that pubertal maturation affects adolescent girls'
self-esteem and that girls who have just started to menstruate have the
highest self-esteem point out the importance of menarche. Rather than a
gradual biological change which characterizes physical development
during adolescence, menarche represents a dramatic singular event. It
signals not only reproductive capability, but a status change in roles
from girl to young woman. This could be emphasized by the social
environment (peers and parents) when they tend to view the menarche as a
sign of general, rather than only biological maturation.
Table 3
Summary of Regression Analyses with Self-esteem as Criterion
Step/predictor Beta [R.sup.2] F
First step
Age .11
School performance .00
Intimacy - mother -.23
Punitiveness - mother .14
Control - mother .22
Intimacy- father -.07
Punitiveness - father .34
Control - father -.09
Intimacy - friend -.04
Control- friend -.03
Pubertal maturation -.02
Total set $ .43 11.56(*)
Last step (backward)
Intimacy - mother -.25
Punitiveness - mother .18
Control - mother .17
Punitiveness - father .29
Total set .41 29.85(*)
* p [is less than] .001.
Biological changes and the responses of others affect adolescent
girls' perception of self; physical aspects of self and body image
become especially important for the girls' self-esteem in this
period (Bezinovic & Lackovic-Grgin, 1990; McGrory, 1990).
Satisfaction with the fact that the expected event has occurred and that
one's body is functioning properly, as well as the symbolic change
in status to a grown-up, may explain this increase in self-esteem.
The girls who had been menstruating longest (more than a year) had
the lowest self-esteem. It is possible that after this period of
experience with menstruation, they adjust to physical changes and
incorporate menstruation into their perception of self.
Physical changes during adolescence have an impact not only on
girls' self-esteem but on their interpersonal relationships
(Steinberg, 1988; Steinberg & Hill, 1978). Previous studies have
shown an increase in conflict at home, possibly because parents tend to
become more restrictive and controlling (Hill, Holembeck, Marlow, Green,
& Lynch, 1985; Papini & Sebby, 1987). The present results are
consistent with these findings. Parental control seems to increase as
girls mature physically. As a function of time of physical maturation,
there is less intimacy and more punitiveness in the relationship with
mothers.
Though pubertal maturation was associated with both self-esteem and
interaction variables, the results of regression analysis showed that
pubertal maturation has only an indirect effect on self-esteem. The best
predictors of self-esteem of adolescent girls appear to be the quality
of their relationship with mother and punitiveness of their fathers.
Similar results were found by Buri, Kirchner, and Walsh (1987) and by
Coopersmith (1967); higher self-esteem is positively related to intimacy
and nurturance in relationship with parents, and negatively related to
parental punitiveness and control. This supports the view that the
quality of interactions of the same-sex parent may be especially
important for the development of self-esteem during early and middle
adolescence (Wade et al., 1989). Nonsignificant relationships between
self-esteem and the quality of interaction with friends for this age
group is not surprizing. It is not until late adolescence that increased
intimacy in relationships with peers leads to a stronger impact of
friends on adolescents' self-esteem (Lackovic-Grgin & Dekovic,
1990).
Finally, this study examined only one of a number of possible
mediators in the relationship between puberal maturation and self-esteem
of adolescent girls: the quality of their interactions with significant
others. Future research should focus, in addition to interaction
variables, on adolescents' personal variables such as formal
thinking, attitudes toward their body and its development, and coping
strategies.
REFERENCES
Bezinovic, P., & Lackovic-Grgin, K. (1990). Percepcija vlastite
kompetentnosti, tjelesnog izgleda i samopostovanja kod 10-godisnje djece
[Perceived competence, physical appearance and self-esteem of
10-year-old children]. Primjenjena Psihologija, 11, 71-75.
Brack, C. J., Donald, P., & Ingersoll, G. (1988). Pubertal
maturation and adolescent self-esteem. Journal of Adolescent Health
Care, 9, 280-285.
Buri, J. R., Kirchner, P. A., & Walsh, J. M. (1987). Familial
correlates of self-esteem in young American adults. Journal of Social
Psychology, 127, 583-588.
Burns, R. (1982). Self-concept development and education. London:
Holt, Rinehart & Winston.
Coopersmith, S. (1967). The antecedents of self-esteem. San
Francisco: Freeman.
Hill, J. P., Holembeck, G. N., Marlow, L., Green, T. M., & Lynch,
M. E. (1985). Menarcheal status and parent-child relations in families
of seventh-grade girls. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 14, 301-316.
Hirsch, B. J., & Rapkin, B. D. (1987). The transition to junior
high school: A longitudinal study of self-esteem, psychological
symptomatology, school life, and social support. Child Development, 58,
1235-1243.
Houston, L. N. (1984). Self-esteem, locus of control, and
conservatism. Psychological Reports, 55, 851-854.
Kulas, H. (1982). Level of self-evaluation and social position in the
peer group. Psychologia Wychowawcza, 25, 280-292.
Lackovic-Grgin, K. (1987). Spolno sazrijevanje i neki aspekti
socijalnog ponasanja adolescentica [Sexual maturation and some aspects
of social behavior of adolescent girls]. Radovi Filozofskog Fakulteta u
Zadru, 28, 95-105.
Lackovic-Grgin, K. (1988). Problemi adolescenata iz gradskih sredina
[Adolescents' problems in urban setting]. Zadar: Narodni list.
Lackovic-Grgin, K., & Dekovic, M. (1990). The contribution of
significant others to adolescents' self-esteem. Adolescence, 25,
839-946.
Lackovic-Grgin, K., & Opacic, G. (1989). Interakcije s majkom i
zadovoljstvo obiteljskim zivotom mladih iz obitelji s ocem i bez oca
[Adolescent's interaction with mother and satisfaction with family
life in families with and without father]. Radovi Filozofskog Fakulteta
u Zadru, 28, 155-163.
Marron, J. A., & Kayson, W. A. (1984). Effects of living status,
gender, and year in college on college students' self-esteem and
life change experiences. Psychological Reports, 55,
McGrory, A. (1990). Menarche: Responses of early adolescent females.
Adolescence, 25, 265-270.
Nottelman, E. D. (1987). Competence and self-esteem during transition
from childhood to adolescence. Developmental Psychology, 23, 441-450.
Opacic, G., & Kos, G. (1987) Pokusaj identificiranja faktorske
strukture nekih aspekata porodicne interakcije [Identification of factor
structure of some aspects of family interaction.] Dani Psihologie
87-Zbornik Radova, 5, 115-126.
Papini, D. R., & Sebby, R. A. (1987). Adolescent pubertal status
and affective family relationships: A multivariate assessment. Journal
of Youth and Adolescence, 16, 1-15.
Rosenberg, M. J. (1979). Conceiving the self New York: Basic Books.
Savin-Williams, R. C., & Demo, D. H. (1984). Developmental change
and stability in adolescent self-concept. Developmental Psychology, 20,
1100-1110.
Simmons, R. G., Blyth, D. A., & McKinney, K. L. (1983). The
social and psychological effects of puberty on white females. In J.
Brooks-Gun, & A. Petersen (Eds.), Girls at puberty: Biological and
psychosocial perspective (pp. 229-272). New York: Plenum Press.
Steinberg, L. (1988). Reciprocal relation between parent-child
distance and pubertal maturation. Developmental Psychology, 24, 122-128.
Steinberg, L. D., & Hill, J. P. (1978). Patterns of family
interaction as a function of age, the onset of puberty, and formal
thinking. Developmental Psychology, 14, 683-684.
Wade, T. J., Thompson, V., Tashakkori, A., & Valente, E. (1989).
A longitudinal analysis of sex by race differences in predictors of
adolescent self-esteem. Personality and Individual Differences, 10,
717-729.