The prevalence of depression in a high school population.
Connelly, Brian ; Johnston, David ; Brown, Ian D.R. 等
An increasing number of articles, books, and conferences attest to
the growing attention being paid to depression in children and
adolescents. While few authors question its existence (Chartier &
Ranieri, 1984), there is a general lack of empirical data on affective
disorders in nonclinical adolescent populations (Teri, 1982;
Puig-Antich, 1985; Faulstich et al., 1986; Sullivan & Engin, 1986).
Studies in the area of adolescent depression have involved different
populations, methodologies, and instruments, resulting in widely
discrepant results (Hodgman, 1985; see Table 1). Consequently, a
confusing picture emerges of the mental health of North American youth.
In spite of the difficulties posed by empirical research, prevalence
studies are essential for professionals in this field. Clinically we are
likely to respond differently if we see depression in adolescence as
ubiquitous as opposed to a phenomenon that is rare or does not exist at
all during that stage of development. Prevalence studies provide an idea
of the proportion of adolescents who experience depressive symptomatology at a given time.
TABULAR DATA OMITTED
Another clinical consideration is that, as professionals, we are
expected to represent fairly and accurately the people with whom we
work. Whether this occurs through the presentation of papers, published
articles, or simply in conversation, it is incumbent upon us to reflect
the "current state" of adolescent mental health.
Prevalence studies provide important data for further explorations.
The accumulation of knowledge in this area facilitates understanding of
this group over time, and allows comparison with other groups. In
addition, data obtained from these investigations can play a part in
determining types and amounts of clinical services. In short, the
allocation of (scarce) clinical resources can be affected by the
prevalence rates of mental illnesses afflicting adolescents.
METHOD
Participants
The present study was undertaken in the metropolitan Toronto area.
Parents and students of three representative secondary schools were
informed that, on a specified day, a mental health questionnaire would
be distributed for completion by the students. Both parents and students
had the opportunity to decline participation. The completed
questionnaires were then collected and tabulated by a research team not
connected with the schools involved.
Each school offered a combination of general and advanced academic
courses, as well as business and technical subjects. Data were obtained
from 2,909 students. An additional 430 students were absent from school
on the day data were collected, and another 272 chose not to
participate.
Of the 2,909 students who completed the form, 141 did not indicate
their gender and 70 were aged 20 or older. Data from these students were
not included in the overall analysis. Thus, the final number of students
in this study was 2,698, of which 49% were male and 51% were female.
Instrument
Kovacs (1985) points out that it is important to quantify clinical
phenomena such as depression in order to understand them from a
scientific perspective. However, one reason for the lack of empirical
information on adolescent depression may be that there is no widely
accepted instrument specifically designed for evaluation of this
population, although there is for children (Children's Depression
Inventory; Kovacs, 1985) and adults (Beck Depression Inventory; Beck
& Beamesderfer, 1974). Nevertheless, Table 1 demonstrates that the
Beck Depression Inventory (BDI) has been used successfully with an
adolescent population. It has also been used effectively as a screening
instrument with both adults and adolescents.
The BDI is a 21-item self-report measure assessing four relevant
aspects of depression: cognition, behavior, affect, and somatic concerns. Each item consists of four statements reflecting increasing
depressive symptomatology. Statements are ranked from 0 to 3, with 0
being the least serious and 3 representing the most serious. This
results in a total score ranging from 0 to 63. In terms of readability,
Teri (1982) classified the BDI as requiring a fifth-grade reading level,
making it readily comprehensible to the average high school student.
RESULTS
The results of a three-way analysis of variance (Age X Gender X
School) are shown in Table 2. There was no significant difference in
depression scores among the three schools. However, there was a
significant main effect of age (F = 3.91, p |is less than~ .001) and
gender (F = 38.31, p |is less than~ .001). There was one interaction
effect, School X Gender (F = 5.51, p |is less than~ .004).
TABULAR DATA OMITTED
TABULAR DATA OMITTED
TABULAR DATA OMITTED
Further analysis was conducted to obtain descriptive statistics.
Table 3 shows the mean scores on the BDI by age and gender.
Table 4 presents the distribution of students scoring in the four
categories, ranging from none to severe symptomatology. The cutoff
points for these groups, taken from Kendall et al. (1987), were: none
(0-9), mild (10-19), moderate (20-29), and severe (30-63).
Table 5 shows the percentage of subjects by age and gender with
scores in the collapsed categories of either none to mild depression or
moderate to severe depression. The categories used for this breakdown
were based on Beck's cutoff points of 19 or under constituting none
to mild and 20 and above indicating moderate to severe depression
(Kendall et al., 1987).
Tables 3 and 5 indicate that, with greater age, there was an increase
in depression scores for both males and females. Table 3 provides this
information in terms of mean BDI scores, while Table 5 shows that, with
an increase in age, there was a gradual shift in frequencies away from
the none to mild category and toward the moderate to severe category.
However, this trend was not monotonic. Rather, there was a peak for both
female and males at age 16, followed by a decrease and then another rise
at age 19.
The data also reveal that for males there was (a) a somewhat steady
increase in the frequency of scores in the mildly depressed range as
TABULAR DATA OMITTED age increases, (b) a slight increase in the
frequency of scores in the moderate range, and (c) virtually no change
in the frequency of scores in the severe range. For females, there was
virtually no age trend in the frequency of scores in the mildly
depressed range, but clear age-related increases for both moderate and
severe depression.
While there was a significant gender difference, it is important to
note that the overall mean scores for both males (6.93) and females
(9.58) were below the cutoff point for mild depression. In fact only 81
students from the entire population (2,698) scored in the severely
depressed range, and 182 students scored in the moderately depressed
range.
DISCUSSION
With reference to other studies, there are clear similarities and
differences. The overall mean score on the BDI for this population was
8.29. This is lower than the mean in three studies and higher than in
one other, using the same instrument. It approximates more closely the
8.45 average obtained by Ehrenberg et al. (1990) and the 8.47 average of
Teri (1982) than the 10.30 of Baron and Perron (1986) or the 6.23 of
Kaplan et al. (1980). The results of the present investigation lend
substantial confirmation to the moderate mean scores of those other
studies, especially when the large sample size is taken into
consideration.
The gender difference, although similar to that of Baron and Perron
(1986), stands in contrast to the findings of Teri (1982) and Sullivan
and Engin (1986), who found no gender differences in their studies using
the BDI and the BDI Short Form, respectively. We believe that our
finding should be interpreted with caution because of the possibility
that it reflects response bias rather than any real difference in the
rate of depression.
While the results show an increase in measured depression over the
age range sampled, there was an interesting and unexpected finding,
namely the peak in the depression scores at age 16. When gathering
demographic data, one is not surprised by occasional deviations from
smooth or monotonic functions. There are several reasons, however, for
arguing that this peak in the data may not be spurious. First, the peak
itself was fairly pronounced. Second, the sample size for this study was
large. Third, the effect was consistent for both males and females.
While this finding needs to be confirmed through further study, one
might speculate about its meaning. Around age 16, adolescents may
experience more stress and anxiety as a result of the increasing demands
of school life, or as a reaction to social adjustment to peers and
dating or to tensions arising from emerging independence from the
family. Whatever the causes, this is a time when some adolescents need
to be approached with heightened sensitivity.
Finally, substantial differences are apparent in the percentage of
students scoring in the moderate to severe range. Other studies, using a
variety of instruments, have reported findings such as: (a) 33% of
seventh- and eighth-grade students fell into the moderate to severe
range (Albert & Beck, 1975); (b) over 30% of students reported
moderate to severe levels of depressive symptomatology (Sullivan &
Engin, 1986); (c) 48.5% of students evidenced depressive symptoms
(Kashani et al., 1987); (d) nearly one half of 14- and 15-year-olds
reported "appreciable misery" or depression (Rutter et al.,
1976); (e) 32% of adolescents fell into the moderate or severely
depressed categories (Teri, 1982); and (f) 35% of adolescents reported
symptoms sufficient for "high depressed" labels and an
additional 38% for "medium depressed," for a total of 73%
classified as medium or highly depressed (Paton & Kandel, 1978).
The findings of this investigation are more similar to those of Adams
(1986), who reported only 18.1% of the adolescents scoring above 16 on
the BDI, and the results of Kaplan et al. (1980), who found only 60% of
80 students scoring over 16 on the BDI. Clearly our finding of only 7%
of males and 12% of females scoring in the moderate to severe range (20
+ on the BDI) lends considerable credence to a more positive view of
adolescent mental health.
This study was not without its limitations. The population consisted
of students present on a specific day in three schools in a large urban
center. Students not enrolled at this level of secondary school,
adolescents no longer attending school, students absent on the day the
study was completed, and those who refused to participate were excluded.
It may be argued that, in this excluded group, there may be a subgroup
at greater risk for depression. That is, depressed adolescents may be
more likely to drop out or have school attendance problems.
The setting for this study, and others like it, also may have
affected the results. It is known that administering self-report
measures in nonclinical populations can result in inflated scores (Baron
& Perron, 1986). One may question whether this inflation would be
more or less likely to occur when questionnaires are completed
confidentially in large groups in classroom settings.
One final comment relates to the usefulness of the BDI with an
adolescent population. As mentioned earlier, this instrument was
selected due to the absence of an "industry standard" for this
age group. Strengths of the BDI are the clarity of its language and ease
of administration. In terms of the psychometric properties of this
instrument, our findings were consistent with other reports (Kendall et
al., 1987) which have confirmed a positive skew in the distribution of
scores.
SUMMARY
Dramatic and extreme rhetoric punctuates many articles and
discussions involving adolescents. Two problems that arise as a
consequence are: (a) all adolescents are viewed as experiencing
psychological distress, and (b) adolescents who need help are not taken
seriously because their behavior and feelings are considered part of a
normal phase of adolescence. This investigation, with its substantial
sample size, lends support to the view of Offer and Sabshin (1984), who
suggest that most adolescents pass through this time period with little
psychological disruption. The relatively low rates of moderate and
severe depression in this population indicate that the large majority of
teenagers do not experience difficulty in this area. The corollary is
that those who evidence symptomatology need to be identified and helped.
REFERENCES
Adams, R. M. (1986). Adolescent depression study. Unpublished
manuscript, Phi Delta Kappa (Dallas Chapter).
Albert, N., & Beck, A. T. (1975). Incidence of depression in
early adolescence: A preliminary study. Journal of Youth and
Adolescence, 4 (4), 301-307.
Baron, P., & Perron, L. M. (1986). Sex differences in the Beck
Depression Inventory scores of adolescents. Journal of Youth and
Adolescence, 15 (2), 165-171.
Beck, A. T., & Beamesderfer, A. (1974). Assessment of depression:
The Depression Inventory. In P. Pichet (Ed.), Psychological measurements
in psychopharmacology: Vol. 7 (pp. 151-169). Paris: Karger, Basel.
Chartier, G. M., & Ranieri, D. J. (1984). Adolescent depression:
Concepts, treatments, prevention. In P. Karoly & J. J. Steffen
(Eds.), Advances in child behavioral analysis and therapy: Vol. 3 (pp.
153-193). Lexington, MA: D. C. Heath and Co.
Ehrenberg, M. F., Cox, D. N., & Koopman, R. F. (1990). The
prevalence of depression in high school students. Adolescence, 25(100),
905-912.
Faulstich, M. E., Carey, M. P., Ruggiero, C., Enyart, P., &
Gresham, F. (1986). Assessment of depression in children and
adolescents: An evaluation of the Center for Epidemiological Studies
Depression Scale for Children (CES-DC). American Journal of Psychiatry,
143(8), 1024-1027.
Hodgman, C. H. (1985). Recent findings in adolescent depression and
suicide. Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics, 6(3), 162-170.
Kaplan, S. L., Nussbaum, M., Skomorowsky, P., Shenker, I. R., &
Ramsey, P. (1980). Health habits and depression in adolescence. Journal
of Youth and Adolescence, 9(4), 299-304.
Kashani, J. H., Carlsen, G. A., Beck, N. C., Hoeper, E. W., Corcoran,
C. M., McAllister, J. A., Fallahi, C., Rosenberg, T. K., & Reid, J.
C. (1987). Depressive symptoms and depressed mood among a community
sample of adolescents. American Journal of Psychiatry, 144(7), 931-934.
Kendall, P. C., Hollon, S. D., Beck, A. T., Hammen, C. L., &
Ingram, R.E. (1987). Issues and recommendations regarding use of the
Beck Depression Inventory. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 11(3),
289-299.
Kovacs, M. (1985). The Children's Depression Inventory (Revised
July 1985). Unpublished manuscript.
Offer, D., & Sabshin, M. (1984). Adolescence: Empirical
perspectives. In D. Offer & M. Sabshin (Eds.), Normality and the
life cycle (pp. 76-107). New York: Basic Books.
Paton, S. M., & Kandel, D. B. (1978). Psychological factors and
adolescent illicit drug use: Ethnicity and sex differences. Adolescence,
13(50), 187-200.
Puig-Antich, J. (1985). Affective disorders. In H. I. Kaplan & B.
J. Sadock (Eds.), Comprehensive textbook of psychiatry: Vol. 2 (4th
ed.). Baltimore: Williams and Wilkins.
Rutter, M., Graham, P., Chadwick, P. F. D., & Yule, W. (1976).
Adolescent turmoil: Fact or fiction? Journal of Child Psychology and
Psychiatry, 17, 35-56.
Sullivan, W. O., & Engin, A. W. (1986). Adolescent depression:
Its prevalence in high school students. Journal of School Psychology,
24, 103-109.
Teri, L. (1982). The use of the Beck Depression Inventory with
adolescents. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 10, 277-284.
Brian Connelly, Ph.D., David Johnston, M.S.W., Ian D. R. Brown,
Ph.D., and Steve Mackay, M.Sc., Scarborough Board of Education.
Edward G. Blackstock, Ph.D., Peel Board of Education.