Occupational identity development, school performance, and social support in adolescence: findings of a Dutch study.
Meeus, Wim
INTRODUCTION
According to Erikson (1968), the most important developmental task in
adolescence is the formation of an identity. Marcia's (1966)
identity status model is held to be the major elaboration of
Erikson's views on identity formation in adolescence (Cote &
Levine, 1988). Identity, Marcia suggests, is an ego structure--an
internal, self-constructed and dynamic organization of aspirations,
skills, beliefs, and individual history.
Following Erikson, Marcia looks upon adolescence as the period in
which youngsters experience an identity crisis, which they solve by
making choices regarding their future in a number of life domains.
Crisis and commitment are the core variables in Marcia's identity
status model. These variables make it possible to distribute adolescents
over four identity statuses. Identity diffusion indicates that the
adolescent has made no commitment as yet regarding a specific
developmental task and may or may not have experienced a crisis in that
domain. Foreclosure holds that the adolescent has made a commitment
without having experienced a crisis. In Moratorium, the adolescent is in
a state of crisis and has made no commitment or at best an unclear one.
Identity achievement signifies that the adolescent has surmounted the
crisis and made a commitment.
Table 1.
Marcia's identity status model
IDENTITY FORECLOSURE MORATORIUM IDENTITY
DIFFUSION
ACHIEVEMENT
CRISIS/
EXPLORATION yes or no no actual yes, past
COMMITMENT no yes unclear yes
Reviews of research using Marcia's paradigm (Marcia, 1980;
Waterman, 1982) indicate that the identity statuses can be divided into
two groups: identity achievement and moratorium are generally associated
with positive characteristics (e.g., high levels of self-esteem,
autonomy, reasoning in terms of moral values), whereas foreclosure and
identity diffusion are associated with negative characteristics (e.g.,
low levels of self-esteem, autonomy, reasoning). While there are
exceptions (cf. Meeus, 1991), overall the division is valid (see Marcia,
1980, and Archer & Waterman, 1988, for a discussion of whether the
various identity status categories for men and women can indeed be
divided into the same two groups).
Marcia's paradigm assumes that identity formation is
domain-specific. That is, adolescents will or may have a distinct
identity status in the areas of school/occupation, politics/ideology,
and intimate relationships. The present study concentrated on the
development of occupational identity, and the factors that influence
this process.
Adolescence, Status Insecurity, School
The transition from school to job is a crucial passage in
adolescence. In general, the lifelong occupational die is cast at an
early age. In view of the fact that occupational position is a major
indicator of social status, one can define adolescence as the period in
which social status is assigned (Kreutz, 1974).
This stamps adolescence as a time of both opportunity and
uncertainty. Side by side are the challenge to move up the social ladder
and the threat of moving down. Young people may aspire to a higher
occupational level than that of their parents (upward mobility); a lower
occupational level implies downward mobility.
As can be adduced from much research on schooling and occupational
careers (Diederen, 1983; Meijers & Wesselingh, 1983; Meijers, 1989),
the educational level attained determines labor-market opportunities.
For this reason, the transition from a lower to a higher educational
level in adolescence can be defined as upward mobility as well, and the
move from a higher to a lower educational level as downward mobility.
Educational level and school performance, then, are very important to
young people. A review article spanning thirty years of research (Nurmi,
1991) indicates that, in planning for the future, adolescents are most
preoccupied with their careers in school and work. Thus, education and
occupation become more important during adolescence while the importance
of leisure time decreases (Nurmi, 1989).
Recent studies confirm Nurmi's conclusions. Diekstra, Garnefski,
De Heus, De Zwart, Van Praag, and Warnaar (1991) show that 86% of all
Dutch students find school important. German research (Hurrelmann, 1990)
indicates that adolescents look upon poor school performance as their
most serious problem. Verhofstadt-Deneve (1991) reports similar findings
with respect to Belgian youngsters. That school is important is
underscored by the fact that poor school performance is related to
psychosomatic complaints--headaches, nervousness, poor concentration
(Hurrelmann, Engel, Holler, & Nordlohne, 1988)--aggressive behavior
(Engel, 1988), and juvenile delinquency (Hirschi, 1972; Junger-Tas,
1985). In brief, poor school performance is conducive to greater status
insecurity and hence to behavioral problems in adolescence.
It therefore seems obvious that a relationship exists between
occupational identity formation and educational achievement. A
successful school career reduces status insecurity because the
adolescent can justifiably anticipate attainment of an attractive job,
while downward occupational mobility is less likely. In terms of
Marcia's model, this means that good school performance should lead
to a "higher" identity status with respect to
occupation--moratorium or identity achievement. Two studies confirm this
hypothesis. Cross and Allen (1970) report that the school performance of
students who have attained the identity achievement status is
significantly better than that of students in the other statuses.
Berzonsky (1985) has demonstrated that the school performance of
students in the identity achievement and moratorium statuses outstrips
that of students in foreclosure. Additional indirect support can be
derived from studies by Waterman (1970, cited by Marcia, 1980), Orlofsky
(1978), and Grotevant and Thorbecke (1982). Waterman has shown that the
attitude toward studying among identity achievement students is better
than that of students in the other statuses; Orlofsky as well as
Grotevant and Thorbecke report that identity achievement and moratorium
students score higher on achievement motivation.
Personal Networks in Adolescence
Another major process during adolescence is that of the "second
separation-individuation" (Blos, 1967). Young people shed parental
influence and increasingly orient themselves to their peers.
The degree of influence that parents and peers have on adolescents
has been investigated for at least thirty years under the heading of
parent-peer conflict (for a review, see Meeus, 1989). These studies have
yielded two important conclusions. First, parental and peer influence
need not necessarily conflict. This result was first established by
Kandel and Lesser (1969) and replicated since (see the review by Conger
& Petersen, 1984). This is also true for Dutch adolescents (Meeus,
1989). Second, there is general confirmation of the hypothesis that
parent-peer influence is situational. This hypothesis was formulated by
Brittain in 1968 and states that in some domains parental influence is
greater, and in others that of peers. For issues relating to the future
(school, occupation), the influence of parents is of greater weight,
while for other issues (e.g., leisure time), peer influence is more
significant. A study among sixteen-year-olds (Meeus, 1989) provided
support for the situational hypothesis. In the area of personal
relationships, the mother's influence was found to be greatest;
with respect to school, the father exerted more influence; and for
leisure time, adolescents conformed to their friends' views.
However, longitudinal follow-up research (Meeus, Raaijmakers, &
Vollebergh, 1991) indicates that this pattern is age-specific. At age
eighteen, the influence of friends is greatest in each of the three
domains, or equal to that of the most influential parent. This indicates
that the situational hypothesis as valid for middle adolescence; in late
adolescence there is an obvious trend wherein the influence of peers
becomes strongest.
In this study, two questions were posed: (1) How does school
performance influence occupational identity formation? It was expected
that the development of occupational identity benefits from good school
performance. Such performance will correlate with a high-level
occupational identity status; that is, either identity achievement or
moratorium. (2) How does social support, by means of personal networks,
influence the formation of occupational identity? Since occupational
career is strongly influenced by educational career, it was expected
that social support provided by personal networks relative to the school
domain would have more impact on occupational identity formation than
would social support via personal networks relative to the domains of
leisure time and intimate relationships. Since the average age of the
sampled adolescents was eighteen, it was further expected, based on
research by Meeus et al. (1991), that the influence on occupational
identity formation of social support provided by friends would outstrip that of social support available from parents.
METHOD
Subjects
Three hundred adolescents, whose average age was just over eighteen
years, participated in the survey. Half of them were at
intermediate-level high schools for occupational or vocational training,
25% attended high schools for advanced occupational training, and 25%
attended high schools that prepared them for university. Gender division
in the sample was fifty-fifty. The adolescents were administered a
questionnaire, which they completed during a 45-minute class period.
Measures
Occupational identity. A Dutch translation of the Dellas Identity
Status Inventory-Occupation (DISI-O) was used to measure occupational
identity. The DISI-O is one of the most promising instruments for
measuring occupational identity (Bosma, 1985). It contains 35 items and
allows ascription to a student of one of the following types of identity
status: identity achievement, moratorium, foreclosure,
diffused-diffused, and diffused-luck. This division is identical to that
of Marcia, except for the subdivision of diffusion. Diffused-diffused
respondents are entirely unconcerned regarding their future occupation,
while diffused-luck respondents do not worry because they place their
trust in luck.
The 35 items are ordered in seven sets of five. Each item in a set
stands for one of the five identity status categories. (Examples of
DISI-O items include: identity achievement--"I have looked into
various kinds of work, made my choice, and am satisfied with my
occupation"; moratorium--"At this time I am still checking on
a number of things to get a clear picture of the occupation I
want"; foreclosure--"I have long known what occupation I want
to take up. That is why I did not have to look at other jobs";
diffused-diffused--"I am not so concerned with a profession yet; I
do not want to choose until the time is ripe";
diffused-luck--"You never know what course your career will take,
so I am not worrying much about occupations.") In every set, the
respondent is asked to check the item that fits him or her best. DISI-O,
then, relies on forced choices. The number of items checked for each
status are added together, and the respondent is assigned to a status
when at least four of the seven possibilities relative to that status
are checked. The DISI-O takes about twenty minutes to complete.
Personal social networks and social support. To delineate the
personal network, a version of the role-relation approach (Fischer,
1982) was used. Respondents indicate the degree to which they experience
social support from a standard set of persons, each of whom assumes a
distinct role. The standard set consisted of father, mother, and
friends. Respondents delineated their personal network three times: for
the domains of school, personal relationships, and leisure time.
School performance. The respondents' assessment of their school
performance was measured using the three-point item proposed by Van der
Linden and Roeders (1983). In Category 1, the respondent considers his
or her school performance generally good; Category 2, school performance
is about average; Category 3, school performance is assessed as
generally poor.
Additional measures. Along with data concerning the usual background
variables (e.g., gender, father's occupational level), respondents
were asked about feelings of loneliness, number of friends, and
leisure-time behavior.
RESULTS
Occupational Identity
As expected, correlations between the items of a single status were
highest; for identity achievement these correlations varied between .48
and .73; for moratorium between .12 and .45; for foreclosure between .62
and .85; for diffused-diffused between .13 and .45; and for
diffused-luck between .15 and .46. Reliability coefficients
(Cronbach's alpha) over the item sets of the five status types
varied between .65 and .94, in accord with the values reported by Dellas
and Jernigan (1981).
In assigning respondents to an identity status, we followed the
convention of Dellas and Jernigan (1987) of combining diffused-diffused
and diffused-luck into one status--diffusion. Respondents were assigned
to this status if they checked off four or more items of the two-item
sets.
Of the 294 respondents (data for 6 respondents could not be used
because of missing responses on DISI-O items), we were able to classify
261 (89%). Studies of Dellas and Jernigan (1981, 1987) yielded similar
results. Distribution was as follows: identity achievement, 17% of the
respondents; moratorium, 28%; foreclosure, 15%; and diffusion, 40%.
No relationship was found between status classification and age,
||Chi~.sup.2~(15, N = 261) = 11.10, p |is greater than~ .05; gender,
||Chi~.sup.2~(3, N = 261) = 7.26, p |is greater than~ .05; and
father's occupational level, ||Chi~.sup.2~(18, N = 255) = 13.93, p
|is greater than~ .05.
Occupational Identity, School, Relationships and Leisure Time.
We expected and found a significant relationship between school
performance and occupational identity, ||Chi~.sup.2~(3, N = 261) =
13.58, p |is less than~ .01 (to avoid proliferation of n |is less than~
5 cells, Categories 2 and 3 of the school performance variable were
combined). School performance of 91% of those assigned to the identity
achievement status was good; for those in moratorium and foreclosure,
70%; and diffused, 60%. We further found a relationship between type of
training and status classification, ||Chi~.sup.2~(9, N = 261) = 23.22, p
|is less than~ .01. Foreclosure was more frequent among students with
intermediate vocational and advanced occupational training (i.e., the
occupation-oriented programs). Evidently, the choice of
occupation-oriented training goes with an occupational orientation which
is fixed fairly early and without a great deal of reflection.
Table 2.
Reliability coefficients and number of respondents per identity
status
IDENTITY STATUS RELIABILITY
DELLAS & PRESENT N %
JERNIGAN, 1981 STUDY
IDENTITY ACHIEVEMENT .91 .89 44 17%
MORATORIUM .84 .74 75 28%
FORECLOSURE .92 .94 38 15%
DIFFUSED-DIFFUSED .73 .70
DIFFUSED-LUCK .64 .65
DIFFUSION (DD + DL) 104 40%
Also investigated (using ANOVA and chi-square) was the association
between occupational identity and two other life domains: relationships
and leisure time. Occupational identity proved unrelated to feelings of
loneliness, F(3, 255) = .57, p |is greater than~ .05; the number of good
friends, ||Chi~.sup.2~(6, N = 261) = 2.46, p |is greater than~ .05; or
the ability to spend leisure time enjoyably, F(3, 257) = 1.79, p |is
greater than~ .05.
Occupational Identity and Social Support Via Personal Networks
The structure of the personal networks in the three domains was
similar to that found in a study by Meeus et al. (1991). In each of the
domains (school, leisure time, and relationships), the social support
provided by friends proved more important or no less important than that
offered by either parent. By way of nine analyses of variance (three
domains by three reference persons), we next determined the connection
between occupational identity and social support via personal networks.
As expected, we found this connection to exist in the case of friends
relative to school, F(3, 247) = 2.67, p |is less than~ .05. Successive
application of Scheffe's test and a t test showed that students in
the identity achievement and moratorium statuses experienced more social
support from friends regarding school than did those in the foreclosure
and diffused statuses, t(249) = 2.69, p |is less than~ .01. No other
significant connections were found between personal network support and
occupational identity. That is, parental social support was not related
to occupational identity in any of the three domains, nor was social
support by friends related to occupational identity relative to the
domains of leisure time and relationships.
DISCUSSION
The Dutch version of the DISI-O seems an adequate instrument for
measuring the formation and development of occupational identity. The
internal consistency of the scales for the various identity status
classifications varies from sufficient to very high, and is comparable
to that of the original instrument. Moreover, if a few simple rules are
applied, approximately 90% of the respondents can readily be classified
as belonging to one of the four identity status categories. We note
that, among Dutch eighteen-year-olds, occupational identity is still
only weakly developed.
Occupational identity formation is related to the school career.
Youngsters who perform well at school evince a stronger occupational
identity than do those whose school performance is poor. Success in
school reduces status insecurity in adolescence because it holds the
promise of attractive future occupations. This encourages young people
to become occupation-oriented.
There appears to be no connection between occupational identity
formation and the development of relationships or leisure-time behavior.
Adolescents may have few or many friends, their leisure time may be
spent in satisfactory or unsatisfactory ways--but none of this affects
their occupational identity. This underscores the domain-specific
character of development during adolescence, which was assumed by
Marcia. The careers at school and at work are of a piece; they
constitute a single domain of development, one in which a person's
social status is determined.
Although relational development as such is not connected with
occupational identity formation, we do not imply that significant others
play no role in the process. In late adolescence, peers are more
important referents than are parents. The social support provided by
friends relative to problems at school encourages development of
occupational identity. The social support offered by parents in this
domain is less important, as is their support in the domains of
relationships and leisure time. Further, social support by friends in
the domains of relationships and leisure does not influence occupational
identity formation. This confirms the view that development in the
domain of school/occupation on the one hand, and development in the
domains of relationships and leisure time on the other, are relatively
unconnected processes.
REFERENCES
Archer, S., & Waterman, A. (1988). Psychological individualism:
Gender differences or gender neutrality? Human Development, 31, 65-81.
Berzonsky, M. (1985). Diffusion within Marcia's identity-status
paradigm: Does it foreshadow academic problems? Journal of Youth and
Adolescence, 14, 527-538.
Blos, P. (1967). The second individuation process of adolescence.
Psychoanalytic Study of the Child, 23, 162-186.
Bosma, H. (1985). Identity development in adolescence. Groningen:
doctoral dissertation.
Brittain, C. (1968). An exploration of the bases of peer-compliance
and parent-compliance in adolescence. Adolescence, 13, 445-458.
Conger, J., & Petersen, A. (1984). Adolescence and youth:
Psychological development in a changing world. New York: Harper &
Row.
Cote, J., & Levine, C. (1988). A critical examination of the ego
identity status paradigm. Developmental Review, 8, 147-185.
Cross, H., & Allen, J. (1970). Ego identity status, adjustment,
and academic achievement. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology,
34, 288.
Dellas, M., & Jernigan, L. (1981). Development of an objective
instrument to measure identity status in terms of occupation crisis and
commitment. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 41, 1039-1050.
Dellas, M., & Jernigan, L. (1987). Occupational identity status
development, gender comparisons, and internal-external control in
first-year air force cadets. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 16,
587-600.
Diederen, J. (1983). De keuze van een beroep |Occupational choice~.
Nijmegen: ITS.
Diekstra, R., Garnefski, N., Heus, P. de, Zwart, R. de, Praag, B.
van, & Warnaar, M. (1991). Scholierenonderzoek 1990: Gedrag en
gezondheid |Survey on Schoolchildren 1990: Behavior and health~. Den
Haag: NIBUD.
Engel, U. (1988). Youth, mobility and social integration. In J.
Hazekamp, W. Meeus, & Y. te Poel (Eds.), European contributions to
youth research (pp. 81-93). Amsterdam: Free University Press.
Erikson, E. (1968). Identity: Youth and crisis. New York: Norton
& Co.
Fischer, C. (1982). To dwell among friends: Personal networks in town
and city. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Grotevant, H., & Thorbecke, W. (1982). Occupational identity
formation. Developmental Psychology, 18, 396-405.
Hirschi, T. (1972). Causes of delinquency. Berkeley: University of
California Press.
Hurrelmann, K. (1990). Parents, peers, teachers and other significant
partners in adolescence. International Journal of Adolescence and Youth,
2, 211-236.
Hurrelmann, K., Engel, U., Holler, B., & Nordlohne, E. (1988).
Status insecurity and psychosomatic disorders in adolescence. Journal of
Adolescence, 25, 205-215.
Junger-Tas, J. (1985). De theorie van sociale controle of sociale
binding |The theory of social control~. Tijdschrift voor Criminologie,
27, 244-265.
Kandel, D., & Lesser, G. (1969). Parental and peer influences on
educational plans of adolescents. American Sociological Review, 34,
213-223.
Kreutz, H. (1974). Soziologie der Jugend. Munich: Juventa.
Linden, F. van der, & Roeders, P. (1983). Schoolgaande jongeren,
hun leefwereld en welbevinden |Schoolchildren, their life world and
well-being~. Nijmegen: Hoogveld Instituut.
Marcia, J. (1966). Development and validation of ego-identity status.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 3, 551-558.
Marcia, J. (1980). Identity in adolescence. In J. Adelson (Ed.),
Handbook of
adolescent psychology (pp. 159-187). New York: Wiley.
Meeus, W. (1989). Parental and peer support in adolescence. In K.
Hurrelmann & U. Engel (Eds.), The social world of adolescents (pp.
167-185). New York: De Gruyter.
Meeus, W. (1991). Jeugd en identiteit. De adolescente identiteit in
theorie en empirie: Erikson en Marcia |Youth and identity. Adolescent
identity: Erikson and Marcia~. In C. Klaassen (Ed.), Jeugd als sociaal
fenomeen |Youth as a social phenomenon~ (pp. 83-105). Amersfoort: Acco.
Meeus, W., Raaijmakers, Q., & Vollebergh, W. (1991). Political
intolerance and youth centrism in adolescence: An overview of Dutch
research and some recent longitudinal findings. In G. Breakwell (Ed.),
Social psychology of political and economic cognition (pp. 97-120).
London: Academic Press.
Meijers, F. (Ed.). (1989). Jongeren op weg |Youngsters on their way~.
Leiden: sectie Jongerenstudies en jeugdbeleid.
Meijers, F., & Wesselingh, A. (1983). Over de reproduktie van het
onderwijs. De maatschappelijke ongelijkheidsdiskussie opnieuw bekeken
|On the educational reproduction of social inequality~. Comenius, 3,
93-114.
Nurmi, J. (1989). Development of orientation to the future during
early adolescence: A four-year longitudinal study and two
cross-sectional comparisons. International Journal of Psychology, 24,
195-214.
Nurmi, J. (1991). How do adolescents see their future? A review of
the development of future orientation and planning. Developmental
Review, 11, 1-59.
Orlofsky, J. (1978). Identity formation, achievement, and fear of
success in college men and women. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 7,
49-62.
Verhafstadt-Deneve, L. (1991). Adolescentiepsychologie |Adolescent
psychology~. Leuven-Apeldoorn: Garant.
Waterman, A. (1982). Identity development from adolescence to
adulthood: An extension of theory and a review of research.
Developmental Psychology, 18, 341-358.