An alternative model of self-forgiveness.
McConnell, John M. ; Dixon, David N. ; Finch, W. Holmes 等
Throughout life, people inevitably transgress by offending others,
themselves, or a religious/spiritual figure by failing to uphold
others', personal, or spiritual standards. When transgressors have
empathy for their victims, take responsibility, and their transgressions
are sufficiently severe, they often experience remorse as guilt and
self-condemnation through shame (Tangney & Dearing, 2002). In other
words, guilt and shame are painful intropunitive feelings focused on
behaviors or characterological flaws, respectively (Fisher & Exline,
2006; Lewis, 1971; Tangney & Dearing, 2002). Although transgressors
can experience guilt and shame simultaneously, it is important to
distinguish the behavioral nature of guilt from the characterological
focus of shame. Whereas shame-free guilt is associated with
interpersonal and intrapsychic reparative actions, guilt-free shame is
related to social withdrawal and avoidance motivation (Tangney &
Dearing, 2002). Living through these feelings often diminishes
transgressors' self-worth and self-respect (Dillon, 2001). In order
to resolve negative self-evaluations, shame-free guilty transgressors
may become concerned with self-forgiveness. Guilt-free shameful persons,
however, may avoid self-forgiveness altogether unless they are able to
reframe their offenses from character flaws into behavioral terms (Hall
& Fincham, 2008; Tangney & Dearing, 2002).
To accomplish self-forgiveness, transgressors often apologize to
and seek expressions of forgiveness from their victims. Hall and Fincham
(2005) suggested that transgressors' various levels of attributions
(e.g., internal vs. external cause), transgression severity, empathy,
guilt, shame, conciliatory behaviors (e.g., apologies), and perceived
interpersonal forgiveness all play roles in the process of
self-forgiveness. In a follow up study, however, Hall and Fincham (2008)
found only severity of transgressions, guilt, conciliatory behaviors,
and perceived forgiveness related to self-forgiveness above and beyond
the variance accounted for by the passage of time. Further,
attributions, empathy, and shame were unrelated to self-forgiveness when
they accounted for time. The results of Hall and Fincham (2008) suggest
(1) taking responsibility is a mere precondition to self-forgiveness,
(2) empathy plays an insignificant role in self-forgiveness, and (3)
shameful individuals may not accomplish self-forgiveness. In the current
study, we compared models derived from Hall and Fincham's 2005 and
2008 models (Figures 1 & 2) against our a priori alterative mediation model (Figure 3) to investigate self-forgiveness following
interpersonal offenses.
Self-Forgiveness Path Models
We present an overview of Hall and Fincham's (2005) proposed
antecedents of self-forgiveness followed by descriptions of models
(Figures 1 & 2) derived from Hall and Fincham (2005/2008) and our a
priori alternative mediation model (Figure 3).
Attributions
Various attributions about transgressions, or the extent to which
transgressors assume blame, may impact transgressors' guilty and
shameful feelings. External, unstable, global, uncontrollable, and
prideful attributions are likely to superficially increase
self-forgiveness because they minimize taking responsibility for
offenses (Fisher & Exline, 2006; Hall & Fincham, 2005; Tangney,
Boone, & Dearing, 2005), thereby decreasing levels of guilt and
shame. For instance, self-forgiveness was negatively correlated with
guilt and shame, but positively correlated with narcissism and victim
blaming (Strelan, 2007; Zechmeister & Romero, 2002). These
attributions are likely to be indicative of pseudo-self-forgiveness
(Enright, 1996; Hall & Fincham, 2005). In other words, transgressors
may quickly "forgive" themselves by essentially ignoring their
culpabilities.
For example, someone may believe, "I forgive myself for
hitting him. He deserved it anyway." Because real self-forgiveness
may involve more time and effort (Fisher & Exline, 2006),
pseudo-self-forgiveness may appear on the surface to have the same
benefits of self-forgiveness, but it may not produce the same lasting
transformational rewards as its more extensive counterpart (Hall &
Fincham, 2005). As transgressors increasingly take ownership of their
faults, they may experience increasing levels of guilt and shame.
However, attaining self-forgiveness might be especially difficult
because internal, stable, specific, controllable, and humble
attributions may inhibit forgiveness of self (Fisher & Exline, 2006;
Hall & Fincham, 2005; Tangney et al., 2005). However, Hall and
Fincham (2008) found changes in attributions were not associated with
self-forgiveness beyond the variance accounted for by time. In this
sense, taking responsibility may be a necessary precondition to
self-forgiveness, but relate little to the process of self-forgiveness.
Severity of transgressions
Severity of transgressions seems to impact transgressors'
experiences of guilty and shameful feelings. Indeed, severe offenses can
lead to greater guilt and shame (Fisher & Exline, 2006;
Ingersoll-Dayton & Krause, 2005). Transgressors perhaps experience
deeper feelings of guilt and shame due to their beliefs that they caused
more harm to their victims. Hall and Fincham (2008) found changes in
perceptions of transgression severity impacted levels of
self-forgiveness beyond the variance accounted for by time. Researchers
have infrequently explored severity of transgressions within the
self-forgiveness literature. To our knowledge, researchers have yet to
develop an empirically validated scale to assess transgression severity.
Empathy
Empathy, a multidimensional construct (Davis, 1980, 1983), is the
cognitive ability of transgressors to accurately perspective-take and to
recognize their victims' affective experiences combined with the
ability to personally experience their victims' affective and
cognitive experiences (Coke, Batson, & McDavis, 1978; Feshbach,
1975). Empathic feelings may be an important tool for transgressors in
first recognizing that they require self-forgiveness (Enright, 1996).
Transgressors, knowing and feeling the true positions in which they put
their victims, may have strong feelings of guilt and shame associated
with their transgressions. Yet transgressors' levels of empathy
appear more strongly related to guilt than shame (Tangney & Dearing,
2002).
These higher levels of painful intropunitive feelings may cause
transgressors to not feel or believe others have forgiven them, even if
they have explicitly stated, "I forgive you." Some
transgressors with high levels of empathy may continue to believe
self-forgiveness is not acceptable because it signals disrespect (Hall
& Fincham, 2005). Empathic transgressors may feel uncomfortable
elevating themselves by letting go of guilt and shame if victims are
still experiencing any deprivation. Given that empathy has a long
empirical history of facilitating other-forgiveness (e.g., McCullough,
et al., 1998; McCullough, Worthington, & Rachal, 1997), some may
have difficulty believing empathic feelings do not play key roles in
self-forgiveness. To date, researchers have provided inconsistent
evidence for the directionality and strength of the relationship between
empathy and self forgiveness (cf. Barbetta, 2002; Hall & Fincham,
2008; Macaskill, Maltby, & Day, 2002; Rangganadhan & Todorov,
2010; Zechmeister & Romero, 2002).
[FIGURE 1 OMITTED]
Guilt and shame
Individuals who commit transgressions may experience guilt and
shame through tension, sorrow, or regret, although researchers have
observed important differences between guilt and shame (Tangney, 1995).
Transgressors are most likely to experience guilt in relation to
specific behaviors, whereas they are more likely to experience shame in
relation to some apparent character flaws (Lewis, 1971). When
individuals believe they are inherently flawed, they may ignore the
outside world because they remain focused on their own personal distress
(Tangney & Dearing, 2002). Shame may be unrelated or weakly related
with self-forgiveness because it is naturally more
"self-centered" than guilt (Hall & Fincham, 2005, 2008;
Lewis, 1971; Tangney, 1995). The self-focus and painful nature of shame
is more likely to lead transgressors into destructive behaviors,
self-criticisms, avoidance, denials, and lashing out at those who pose
threats to their characters (Tangney, 1995; Tangney et al., 1996;
Tangney, Miller, Flicker, & Barlow, 1996). Shameful transgressors
may avoid conciliatory behaviors because they believe changing
themselves is improbable. Several independent trait and state studies
have exhibited this empathic concern of guilt and egocentric nature of
shame (Tangney & Dearing, 2002). Therefore, transgressors who
experience guilt may initiate healthy forms of reparative interactions,
such as admissions, conciliatory behaviors, and reconciliations. On the
other hand, transgressors who experience shame may be more likely to
initiate maladaptive behaviors, such as denials and separations. This
may shed an interesting light on why Hall and Fincham (2008) found
decreases in guilt, but not shame, correlated with higher levels of
self-forgiveness beyond the variance accounted for by time.
Transgressors, who on the surface appear to be blameless and deny their
need for self-forgiveness, may actually be feeling great internal pain
through shame (Tangney & Dearing, 2002).
[FIGURE 2 OMITTED]
Conciliatory behaviours
Some researchers have found conciliatory behaviors positively
related with self-forgiveness (Fisher & Exline, 2006; Hall &
Fincham, 2008). In one study, however, apology and self-forgiveness were
both positively and negatively correlated (Exline, DeShea, &
Holeman, 2007). Moreover, evidence suggests conciliatory behaviors aimed
at transgressors' religious/spiritual figures, rather than their
victims, may act to inhibit their self-forgiveness (Hall & Fincham,
2008). Overall, research suggests that conciliatory behaviors may play a
role in self-forgiveness, but future research still needs to explore
more fully how tertiary factors may impact the effects of conciliation.
Perceived forgiveness
Several studies have demonstrated that transgressors'
perceived forgiveness from victims or religious/spiritual figures is
positively associated with self-forgiveness (Hall & Fincham, 2008;
Martin, 2008; McConnell & Dixon 2012, in press; Witvliet, Ludwig,
& Bauer, 2002). Nevertheless, Layer, Roberts, Wild, and Walters
(2004) found some transgressors felt that religious/spiritual figures
had forgiven them, yet still struggled with the process of
self-forgiveness. Conciliatory behaviors are thought to increase
perceived forgiveness from victims (Hall & Fincham, 2005), perhaps
due to their ability to actually increase empathy and forgiveness from
victims (e.g., McCullough et al., 1997; 1998). Transgressors may move
past associated feelings by having senses of being restored through
forgiveness (Bauer et al., 1992). For transgressors, it may be a small
jump from believing their victims have forgiven them to believing it is
now acceptable to self-forgive.
Model 1: Hall and Fincham's (2005) self-forgiveness model (see
Figure 1)
To be concerned with self-forgiveness, transgressors must commit
actions against victims; for clarity, these actions will remain
interpersonal, rather than intrapersonal or spiritual. The current study
did not measure transgressions, as they are exogenous variables
indicating that an interpersonal transgression has occurred. When
transgressions occur, attributions and severity of transgressions
produce the experiences of transgressors' guilty, shameful, or
self-forgiving feelings. First, attributions that take responsibility
increase transgressors' experiences of guilt and shame (Hall &
Fincham, 2005; Tangney, Wagner, & Gramzow, 1992). Further,
attributions that deny blame may be indicative of
"pseudo-self-forgiveness," while attributions that reflect
taking responsibility will amplify the inability to self-forgive.
Likewise, less severe transgressions will increase self-forgiveness,
whereas more severe transgressions will increase guilt and shame (Hall
& Fincham, 2005). Subsequently, once guilt and shame are
experienced, they must be resolved for self-forgiveness to be
accomplished. Hall and Fincham (2005) did not clarify any mechanism that
resolves shame, as it is a more persistent emotion and is often
associated with avoidance responses (Tangney, 1995). Therefore, lower
levels of shame will be indicative of self-forgiveness, while higher
levels of shame will intensify intrapersonal unforgiveness (Hall &
Fincham, 2005). In the same way, lower levels of guilt will increase
self-forgiveness, however higher levels of guilt will increase
transgressors' use of conciliatory behaviors (Hall & Fincham,
2005). Subsequently, conciliatory behaviors will positively impact
perceived forgiveness and self-forgiveness, while perceived forgiveness
also increases the likelihood that transgressors will forgive themselves
(Hall & Fincham, 2005). According to this model, persons who feel
guilty will have greater empathic feelings towards their victims and
will experience lower levels of self-forgiveness.
[FIGURE 3 OMITTED]
Model 2: Hall and Fincham's (2008) self-forgiveness model
As previously discussed, Hall and Fincham (2008) reported that only
severity of transgressions, guilt, conciliatory behaviors, and perceived
forgiveness predicted forgiveness after accounting for time. Therefore,
attributions, empathy, and shame may be unrelated or weakly related to
transgressors experiences of self-forgiveness. In order to test the
model commensurate with Hall and Fincham (2008), we removed
attributions, empathy, and shame. For this alternative model (Figure 2),
all other relations remain unchanged.
Model 3: Alternative model
Mediation occurs when a third variable (e.g., perceived
forgiveness) best explains the relation between a predictor (e.g.,
conciliatory behavior) and an outcome (e.g., self-forgiveness) as
outlined in Frazier, Tix, & Barron (2004). Mediation models
inherently infer an experimental-causal chain. Researchers, however,
often pragmatically use mediation models when using
measurement-of-mediation survey designs (Spencer, Zanna, & Fong,
2005).
Some believe apologies and forgiveness from victims are
prerequisites for self-forgiveness (e.g., Griswold, 2007; Hughes, 1994).
Further, apologies from transgressors typically influence victims,
through their empathic responses, to forgive their transgressors
(McCullough et al., 1997; 1998). As victims make their forgiveness known
to their transgressors, it is likely to increase transgressors'
feelings of being forgiven, and thus, increases self-forgiveness.
Therefore, conciliatory behavior may mediate the relation between
perceived forgiveness and self-forgiveness, which may clarify the
inconsistent role of apologies with self-forgiveness (Exline et al.,
2007; Fisher & Exline, 2006; Hall & Fincham, 2008). Further,
since severity of transgressions appears more distally related to
self-forgiveness in Hall and Fincham (2005 and 2008), the other
antecedents in the model may mediate the relation between transgression
severity and self-forgiveness. To test this line of reasoning, we
created an alternative, more parsimonious model (Figure 3) a priori by
removing the paths between severity of transgressions/conciliatory
behaviors and self-forgiveness. If model fit--as indicated by
parsimony-adjusted indices--increases from Model 2 to Model 3, it would
indicate that our alternative model is more parsimonious. Finally, if
relations between severity of transgressions/conciliatory behaviors and
self-forgiveness were negligible in Model 2, it would indicate full,
rather than partial, mediation (Frazier et al., 2004).
The Present Research
Investigating how people forgive themselves is important because
people often believe self-forgiveness is more difficult than forgiving
others (e.g., Worthington, 2006), yet accomplishing self-forgiveness is
more strongly associated with overall well-being (Avery, 2008). Hall and
Fincham (2005) made a significant advancement in the field of
self-forgiveness when they proposed their theoretical model, yet
researchers have attempted little confirmation of the model. Recent
evidence (Hall & Fincham, 2008) suggests that time better accounted
for the variance explained in self-forgiveness than attributions,
empathy, shame. Severity of transgressions, guilt, conciliatory
behaviors, and perceived forgiveness maintained strong relations to
self-forgiveness after Hall and Fincham (2008) accounted for time.
Contrarily, Rangganadhan and Todorov (2010) found that their path
model, which highlighted the importance of shame and personal-distress
empathy in dispositional self-forgiveness, fit better than a portion of
Hall and Fincham's (2005) model. However, Rangganadhan and Todorov
(2010) did not test Hall and Fincham's entire model because they
only included dispositional aspects. A second limitation of Rangganadhan
and Todorov (2010) was their use of path diagram modeling, rather than a
full structural model that uses multiple measurements to create latent
variables. The current study extended the self-forgiveness literature by
both exploring the whole model in relation to specific interpersonal
offenses and through the use of structural models. The present study
shed light on the confusing nature of attributions, empathy, guilt,
shame, and conciliatory behaviors in previous literature. Via structural
equation modeling using maximum likelihood estimation (AMOS 16.0;
Arbuckle, 2007), the current study sought to test Hall and
Fincham's (2005 and 2008) models against an alternative
self-forgiveness model. We hypothesized that an alternative model would
be the best fitting, most parsimonious model.
Method
Participants
Participants (male [n = 148], female [n = 257], unspecified [n =
1]) were a convenience sample of 406 undergraduate students at a
Midwestern university who completed the questionnaires for partial
fulfillment of a psychology course requirement. Individuals were
recruited via a posting on a university psychology department website
and were allowed to participate if they were at least 18 years old. An
initial 530 participants signed up, however 124 people were not included
in the study due to incomplete data or random responding.
Participants' ages ranged from 18-41 (M = 20.34, SD = 2.80) and
they represented various ethnic, religious, and marital statuses. We
discuss implications of these demographics in the limitations section.
Procedure
Our institution's ethical review board approved our study
prior to us initiating data collection. After viewing a posted
recruitment letter online, participants sent an email to the primary
investigator indicating their interest in completing the research
experiment and were provided hyperlinks to online modules. The online
modules instructed participants to recall an event in the past two years
in which they had offended someone by something they said or did. The
event only needed to be one in which other persons felt that
participants harmed them, regardless if participants believed they were
innocent or at fault. Participants filled in counterbalanced
questionnaires in approximately 25 to 35 minutes. We collected data from
July 2008 through December 2008.
Materials
The Causal Dimension Scale (CDS; Russell, 1982).
The CDS consists of three separate subscales--Causality, Stability,
and Controllability--which all have previously demonstrated desirable
levels of internal consistency reliabilities ([alpha] = .87, .84, and
.73, respectively) as well as discriminant validities when compared to
each other (Russell, 1982). Participants were instructed to fill in the
CDS in respect to their "impressions or opinions of the cause or
causes of the event." All nine-items were rated with a nine-point
bipolar-type scale (e.g., Is the cause(s) something: Permanent vs.
Temporary ; Inside of You vs. Outside of You). Higher scores reflected
beliefs of more internal causation, permanence, and controllability.
Multidimensional Forgiveness Inventory (MFI; Tangney, Boone,
Dearing, & Reinsmith, 2002). The MFI consists of sixteen scenarios
(e.g., "Imagine that you have sex with you best friend's
boyfriend/girlfriend/spouse") on which participants rate the
degrees to which they believe they would forgive, ask for forgiveness,
self-forgive, take time to forgive others, take time to self-forgive,
blame others, blame self, be hurt, and be angry. Cronbach's alpha for the MFI ranged from .73 to .85 in a previous sample (Tangney et al.,
2002). Further, the MFI has demonstrated desirable levels of convergent
and discriminant validities (Tangney et al., 2002). We used the
Propensity to Blame Others (BO), Propensity to Blame Self (BS),
Propensity to Ask Forgiveness (AF), and Propensity to Forgive Self (PFS)
subscales. Responses to each scenario were, "How likely would you
be to: 'Try to blame someone or something else for the event (BO);
'Think about the situation over and over blaming yourself for the
damage done' (BS); 'Ask them to forgive you' (AF); and
'Forgive yourself' (PFS)." All subscales were quantified
on a five-point Likert-type scale (1: Not at All; 5: Very Likely), with
higher scores reflecting more blaming others, blaming self, asking for
forgiveness, and self-forgiveness.
Test of Self-Conscious Affect--Version 3 (TOSCA-3; Tangney,
Dearing, Wagner, & Gramzow, 2000). The TOSCA-3 consists of sixteen
scenarios (e.g., "You make plans to meet a friend for lunch. At 5
o'clock, you realize you stood him up") that participants rate
the degrees to which they believe they would feel various thoughts and
emotions (e.g., externalization, guilt, shame). We used the
Externalization (PE; e.g., "My boss distracted me just before
lunch"), Guilt (e.g., "You'd think you should make it up
to him as soon as possible"), and Shame (e.g., "I'm
inconsiderate") subscales, which have previously demonstrated
desirable levels of internal consistency reliabilities ([alpha] = .80,
.83, .88, respectively; Tangney & Dearing, 2002) and discriminant
validity in non-clinical samples when compared to objective measures
(Rusch, et al., 2007). All subscales were rated on a five-point
Likert-type scale (1: Not at All; 5: Very Likely), with higher scores
reflecting more blaming others, guilt, and shame.
Transgression Semantic Differentiation (TSD). We developed the TSD
to quantify participants' perceived severity of their
transgressions. Its 24-items are intended to quantify participants'
beliefs about (1) how they view their transgressions (good vs. bad) and
(2) the victims' views of their transgressions (good vs. bad) by
using a thesaurus and having two experts in the field of forgiveness
review items for face validity. The scale consisted of two
subscales--Transgressor Perception (TSD-TP) and Victim Perception
(TSD-VP)--each with 12 items rated on a seven-point bi-polar scale
(e.g., 1: Mild vs. 7: Harsh; 1: Harmless vs. 7: Harmful; 1: Inoffensive
vs. 7: Offensive). Instructions for both subscales were for participants
to select the number that most applies to their beliefs or beliefs about
how their victims view the event. Higher scores reflected more severe
offenses. Internal consistency for the TSD-TP and TSD-VP are reported in
Table 1.
Communication Emotional Response Scale (CERS; Batson, Bolen, Cross,
& Neuringer-Benefiel 1986). The CERS consists of six adjectives
(sympathetic, compassionate, tender, softhearted, moved, and warm)
embedded in a list of 16 distracter adjectives. Participants responded
to these items while considering how they felt towards their victims.
The CERS has demonstrated a desirable level of internal consistency
reliability ([alpha] = .82) as well as convergent validity with other
empathy scales in a previous study (Batson et al., 1986). The 22
adjectives were quantified on a seven-point Likert-type scale ranging
from 1 (Not at All) to 7 (Extremely), with greater scores indicating
higher degrees of empathy.
Interpersonal Reactivity Index (IRI; Davis, 1980). The IRI consists
of four separate subscales--Perspective Taking (PT), Fantasy (FS),
Empathic Concern (EC), and Personal Distress (PD)--that participants
respond to as they typically feel. Because the orthogonal nature of the
IRI would have caused issues with creating a latent variable (Davis,
1980; 1983; Kline, 2005), we used only the two highly correlated
affective (EC) and cognitive (PT) subscales. EC (e.g., "I often
have tender, concerned feelings for people less fortunate than me")
and PT (e.g., "I try to look at everybody's side of a
disagreement before I make a decision") have demonstrated desirable
levels of internal consistency reliabilities for males ([alpha] =.72
& .75) and females ([alpha] = .70 & .78) in a previous study
(Davis, 1980). Additionally, EC's and PT's test-retest
reliabilities for males (.72 & .61) and females (.70 & .62) have
been satisfactory (Davis, 1980). The IRI has previously demonstrated
desirable levels of convergent and discriminant validities (Davis,
1983). All 28-items were quantified on a five-point Likert-type scale
ranging from 1 (Does Not Describe Me Well) to 5 (Describes Me Very
Well), with higher scores reflecting greater empathy.
Personal Feelings Questionnaire-2 (PFQ-2; Harder & Zalma,
1990). The PFQ-2 consists of six guilt (e.g., Mild Guilt), ten shame
(e.g., Self-Consciousness), and six distracter adjectives (e.g.,
Sadness). Participants responded to these items while considering how
they felt towards their victims. The PFQ-2 has demonstrated desirable
levels of internal consistency reliabilities for the guilt ([alpha] =
.72) and shame ([alpha] = .78) scales as well as desirable test-retest
reliabilities (.85 & .91) in a previous study (Harder & Zalma,
1990). Further, the PFQ-2 has previously demonstrated desirable levels
of convergent and discriminant validities (Harder, Rockart, &
Cutler, 1993; Harder & Zalma, 1990). The PFQ-2 is rated on a
five-point Likert-type scale, ranging from 0 (You Never Experience the
Feeling) to 4 (You Experience the Feeling Continuously or Almost
Continuously), with higher scores reflecting greater empathy.
State Shame and Guilt Scale (SSGS; Marschall, Sanftner, &
Tangney, 1994). The SSGS consists of 15-items assessing guilt (e.g.,
"I feel bad about what I did"), shame (e.g., "I feel
small"), and pride (e.g., "I feel proud"), with
five-items each. We used the Guilt and Shame subscales, which have
demonstrated desirable levels of internal consistency reliabilities for
guilt ([alpha] = .82) and shame ([alpha] = .89) in previous research
(Tangney & Dearing, 2002). The SSGS was developed from a rich
empirical and theoretical background and has demonstrated adequate
construct validity (Tangney & Dearing, 2002). Participants were
asked to respond to items in relation to their offenses. The SSGS is
quantified on a five-point Likert-type scale ranging from 1 (Not Feeling
This Way at All) to 5 (Feeling This Way Very Strongly), with higher
scores reflecting greater levels of guilt and shame.
Conciliatory Behaviors Scale (CBS; McCullough et al., 1997). We
modified the CBS to more fully represent the construct of conciliatory
behaviors. The original scale consists of two items ("I tried to
make amends or compensations" and "I took steps toward
reconciliation: Wrote them, called them, expressed love, showed concern,
etc.") that have demonstrated a desirable level of internal
consistency reliability (a = .74). Additionally, one item ("I
attempted to say I was sorry") from the three-item apology scale
(McCullough et al., 1997) was modified and added to the CBS. The
underlined components of the item represent the modification. Further,
two items ("I have expressed personal responsibility and
guilt/shame for the offense," and "I have expressed my
intentions to not repeat the offense") were added in order to
include the missing components of an effective apology (Olshtain, 1989;
Weiner et al., 1991). Participants were asked to respond to items in
relation to their offenses. The five-item scale was quantified on a
seven-point Likert-type scale ranging from 1 (Strongly Disagree) to 7
(Strongly Disagree), with higher scores reflecting greater conciliation.
We created two parcels from these items in order to provide the latent
construct of conciliatory behaviors with a sufficient amount of observed
variables. We further discuss the controversial methodology of parceling
in the results section.
The Heartland Forgiveness Scale (HFS; Thompson et al., 2005). To
quantify participants' levels of perceived forgiveness, we modified
the Heartland Forgiveness of Others subscale (HFSO). We adjusted all six
items of the HFSO to assess transgressors' perceptions of the
victims' forgiveness by changing the subject of each sentence to
assess perceptions of receiving forgiveness, rather than extending
forgiveness (e.g., "The victim will continue to punish me").
We created two parcels from these items in order to provide the latent
construct of perceived forgiveness with a sufficient amount of observed
variables.
To quantify different items relating to forgiveness of self, we
modified the HFS Self-Forgiveness subscale (HFSS). For the focus of this
study, we modified all six items of the HFSS to correctly assess
self-forgiveness in relation to a specific event, rather than
dispositional self-forgiveness. For example, "Although I feel bad
at first when I mess up, over time I can give myself some slack,"
was modified to "Although I felt bad when I messed up, over time I
have given myself some slack."
The original HFS subscales have shown Cronbach's alphas
ranging from .72 to .87 across three samples as well as test-retest
reliabilities for a 3-week interval, .72-.77, and a 9-month interval,
.68-.69) (Thompson et al., 2005). The original HFS has demonstrated
desirable levels of convergent validity with other forgiveness measures
(Thompson et al., 2005). All items of the HFSO and HFSS were quantified
with a seven-point scale ranging from 1 (Definitely False) to 7
(Definitely True), with greater scores reflecting greater perceived
forgiveness and self-forgiveness.
Transgression-Related Interpersonal Motivations Inventory (TRIM;
McCullough et al., 1998). The 12-item TRIM consists of two oblique
subscales--Avoidance (e.g., "He/she avoids me") and Revenge
(e.g., "He/she will make me pay")--which have both
demonstrated desirable levels of internal consistency reliabilities
([alpha] = .86 & a = .90, respectively), test-retest reliability,
and convergent and discriminant validity in a previous study (McCullough
et al., 1998). To quantify the degree to which the participants'
believed their victims forgave them we modified all twelve items of the
TRIM to assess correctly the transgressors' perception of the
victims' forgiveness. As with the HFSO, we changed the subject of
each sentence to assess perceptions of receiving forgiveness, rather
than extending forgiveness. Participants were asked to respond to items
in relation to their offenses. The TRIM was quantified on a seven-point
Likert-type scale ranging from 1 (Definitely False) to 7 (Definitely
True), with greater scores reflecting greater perceived forgiveness.
State Self-Forgiveness Scale (SSFS; Wohl, DeShea, & Wahkinney,
2008). The SSFS consists of two oblique subscales--Self-Forgiving
Feelings and Actions (SSFA; a = .86) and Self-Forgiving Beliefs (SFB; a
= .91)--which have demonstrated desirable levels of internal consistency
reliabilities as well as convergent and discriminant validities when
compared to objective measures in a previous study (Wohl et al., 2008).
Participants were asked to respond to questions preceded by "As I
consider what I did that was wrong ..." Example items include,
"I punish myself" (SSFA) and "I believe I am
acceptable" (SFB). All items were quantified with a four-point
scale ranging from 1 (Not at All) to 4 (Completely"), with higher
scores representing greater self-forgiveness.
The Marlowe-Crowne Social Desirability Scale (M-C SDS; Crowne &
Marlowe, 1960). To observe if any scales related to socially
desirability, we utilized the M-C SDS. The M-C SDS contains 33-items
(e.g., "I have never intensely disliked anyone") that, if
reporting in socially desirable ways, participants are expected to
answer True (T) or False (F). Quantifying the M-C SDS involves assigning
a value of one (T = T or F = F) or a value of zero (T [not equal to] T
or F [not equal to] F) to the participants' responses. Therefore,
higher scores on the M-C SDS represent higher levels of social
desirability. The M-C SDS has demonstrated desirable levels of internal
consistency reliability ([alpha] = .88) and test-retest reliability
(.89) (Crowne & Marlowe, 1960).
Attention questions
Due to the large amounts of items necessary for this study, we
randomly added two items within the main scales to check for attention
(e.g., "After reading this question please select the number
4").
Demographic questionnaire
To assess participants' demographics, we administered a
demographic questionnaire that consisted of questions regarding age,
gender, race, religious affiliation, marital status, and semesters in
college.
Results Data Preparation Excluding cases
We excluded participants based on (1) not recalling an offense, (2)
not filling in any questionnaires, (3) incorrectly marking either of the
two attention questions, or (4) completing the module in less than 25
minutes. To determine the minimum duration for valid participants, we
rank-ordered participants' total response times across the
instruments in juxtaposition to their responses on the two attention
questions. Ninety-two percent of participants who completed modules in
less than 25 minutes also failed to correctly mark the attention
questions. Based on these four criteria, we eliminated 124 of 530
participants, leaving N = 406 for analyses.
Missing data imputation
We assumed missing data were Missing Completely At Random (MCAR)
because the data appeared randomly dispersed among participants and
individual items. In addition, the low percentage of missing data (.02%)
allowed for confidence in concluding MCAR. We utilized the statistical
package AMOS 16.0 to conduct multiple imputation (i.e., generating and
pooling multiple plausible values for missing data) in order to complete
the data set (Arbuckle, 2007).
Parceling
When there are not enough observed variables to sufficiently create
a latent variable, as is the case within the newly developing field of
self-forgiveness, researchers may choose to parcel, or distribute single
items into several subset totals (Little, Cunningham, Shahar, &
Widaman, 2002). We parceled the CBS and HFSO with a method recommended
by Little and colleagues (2002). Using Principal Axis Factoring with
promax rotation, we found that both the CBS and the HFSO were
sufficiently unidimensional. Then, we randomly assigned individual items
into two parcels for CBS and three parcels for HFSO.
Scale properties
See Table 1 for a summary of scale properties. We conducted
internal consistency reliabilities on all scales we used in the current
study. Nearly all scales elicited adequate results ([alpha] = .70 - .95)
and were used for analyses, except CDS, which was excluded because of
low internal consistency ([alpha] = .45 to .57). We conducted
multicollinearity diagnostics with variance inflation factors, which
ranged from 1.02 to 3.98, indicating little redundancy among observed
variables measuring similar constructs. We discuss implications of these
scale properties in the limitations section.
Intercorrelations
We used raw data for all structural equation modeling analyses.
Table 2 presents the intercorrelation matrix of the analyzed variables
[Editor's Note: Due to its size, Table 2 is only available online
at www.nspb.net.]. For exploratory purposes, we used the MC-SDS to
observe if certain scales related to social desirability. Social
desirability was most highly related to severity of transgressions
(TSD-TP: r = -.12; TSD-VP: r = .14), attributions (PE: r = -.15), shame
(PFQ: r = -.11; TOSCA-3: r = .15), and perceived forgiveness (HFSO,
Parcel 1: r = .11) at the .05 level. Other variables were unrelated to
social desirability (Table 2). Social desirability only accounted for
2.25% or less of the variability with other scales. These correlations
suggested that social desirability minimally related to
participants' responses.
Determining and Comparing Model Fits
Model Chi-Square ([chi square]) compares the difference in fit
between researcher's specified models with just-identified models
(i.e., all parameters estimated). It has been demonstrated that high
correlations among the observed variables and large sample sizes tend to
inflate [chi square], which often leads to erroneously rejecting true
models (Kline, 2005). However, Normed Chi-Square ([chi square]/df) is
less sensitive to sample size than [chi square]. Some have offered
several different cutoff values for [chi square]/df (see Kline, 2005).
Therefore, for the purpose of the current study, various cutoff values
indicated great (< 2), good (2-3), adequate (3-5), and poor model fit
(> 5).
The Comparative Fit Index (CFI) and the Tucker-Lewis Index (TLI)
are both incremental fit indices, which compare increases in fit
relative to independence models; that is, models assuming zero
covariance matrices. TLI also penalizes model complexity and therefore
prefers models that are more parsimonious (Kline, 2005). Researchers
have long sought a "golden-rule" cutoff value for interpreting
incremental fit indices, but have experienced considerable difficulty
(Hu & Bentler, 1999; Marsh, Hau, & Wen, 2004). Some suggested
conventional cutoff values (i.e., [greater than or equal to] .90) might
even be too stringent (Marsh et al., 2004), whereas others proposed more
rigorous values (i.e., [greater than or equal to] .95) (Hu &
Bentler, 1999). Thus, values [greater than or equal to] .90 signify
reasonably good fit, while values [greater than or equal to] .95 point
to excellent fit (Kline, 2005). The Root Mean Square Error of
Approximation (RMSEA), also a parsimony-adjusted index (i.e. models with
more estimated parameters are penalized), is immune to sample size
fluctuations because it computes the model-implied covariance
matrix's ability to reproduce the population covariance matrix.
Values of [less than or equal to] .05 evidenced close, .05-.08 indicated
adequate, and > .08 represented poor approximate fit (Kline, 2005).
For non-hierarchical models, that is to say, models that are not a
subset of one another (cf. Model 1 vs. Model 2 or 3), we used Akaike
information criterion (AIC) and Bayesian information criterion (BIC).
AIC and BIC are computed by recreating equally sized, hypothetically
replicated samples (Kline, 2005). Both of these fit indices are
parsimony-adjusted, however BIC adjusts for model complexity more so
than AIC (Kline, 2005). Unlike previously mentioned fit indices, AIC and
BIC both have added advantages of not assuming multivariate normality,
which is not always observed in psychological data (Micceri, 1989).
Although regression weights are usually fairly accurate under cases of
multivariate non-normality, some indices are inappropriately inflated
(e.g., [chi square], [chi square]/df, & RMSEA) or deflated (e.g.,
CFI, TLI), leading to greater chances of rejecting true models (Kline,
2005). There are no set cutoff values for these comparative fit indices,
however, models with lower values provide better fit to the data (Kline,
2005).
Structural Equation Modelling
Model 1: Hall and Fincham (2005). Initial computation of Hall and
Fincham's (2005) SEM elicited a negative error variance associated
with TSD-TP's error term. However, the negative value was not
significantly different from zero (V = -25.11, SE = 17.70, p =.16).
Therefore, we computed an admissible solution by fixing its variance to
zero. Unexpectedly, more externalizing attributions were positively, not
negatively, related with guilt ([beta] = .19, p = .003) and shame
([beta] = .15, p = .003); empathy ([beta] = .28, p < .001) was
positively, not negatively, related to self-forgiveness; and
attributions (P = .10, p = .09) along with conciliatory behaviors
([beta] = -.03, p = .63) were unrelated to self-forgiveness. All other
predicted relations were maintained or weakly maintained (Figure 1).
Nonetheless, these predicted relations should not be entirely trusted
because of poor model fit [[chi square] = 1563.73, df = 262, p <
.001; [chi square]/df = 5.97; CFI = .730; TLI = .691; RMSEA = .111 (CI =
.105-.116); AIC = 1689.73; BIC = 1942.13]; however, significant
multivariate non-normality (Table 3) might have increased the chance of
rejecting a true model.
Model 2: Hall and Fincham (2008). Initial computation of Hall and
Fincham's (2008) SEM elicited a negative error variance associated
with TSD-TP's error term. However, the negative value was not
significantly different from zero (V = -124.90, SE = 76.13, p = .10).
Therefore, we fixed the variance at zero. Whereas severity of
transgressions ([beta] = .06, p = .17) and conciliatory behaviors
([beta] = .03, p = .59) were unrelated with self-forgiveness, all other
predicted relations were maintained at the .001 level (Figure 2). This
pattern of results indicated that offense severity and conciliatory
behaviors were mediated by guilt and perceived forgiveness. Model 2
elicited poor to adequate model fit [[chi square] = 358.14, df = 98, p
< .000; [chi square]/df = 3.65; CFI = .918; TLI = .899; RMSEA =
.081(CI = .072-.09); AIC = 434.14; BIC = 586.38]. Nevertheless,
multivariate non-normality (Table 3) may have decreased model fit. In
this case, Hall and Fincham (2008) might truly approximate a great
fitting model and thus the results of Model 2 are inconclusive.
Model 3: Alternative model
Initial computation of the alternative model elicited a negative
error variance associated with TSD-TP's error term. However, the
negative value was not significantly different from zero (V = -136.95,
SE = 86.23, p = .12). Thus, we again fixed the variance at zero. All
predicted relations were strongly maintained at the .001 level (Figure
3). Most notably, guilt accounted for 47.61% of the variance in
self-forgiveness. The alternative model elicited adequate to good model
fit [[chi square] = 360.20, df = 100, p < .000; [chi square]/df =
3.60; CFI = .918; TLI = .901; RMSEA = .080(CI = .071-.89); AIC = 432.20;
BIC = 576.43]. Nevertheless, multivariate non normality (Table 3) may
have decreased model fit and thus, the alternative model might truly
approximate a great fitting model.
Comparing structural models
Because Model 1 vs. Model 2 or 3 are non-hierarchical comparisons,
we could use only AIC and BIC to directly test the equal fit hypotheses.
When comparing Model 1 with Model 2/3, both AIC
([DELTA]1255.59/[DELTA]1257.53) and BIC ([DELTA]1355.75/[DELTA]1365.70)
had large value decreases, suggesting that Hall and Fincham (2008) and
the alternative are more preferred over Hall and Fincham (2005). Model 2
elicited negligible relations for severity of transgressions and
conciliatory behaviors when predicting self-forgiveness. In addition,
there was a small increase in fit between Model 2 [[chi square] =
358.14, df = 98, p < .000; [chi square]/df = 3.65; CFI = .918; TLI =
.899; RMSEA = .081(CI = .072-.09)] and Model 3 [[chi square] = 360.20,
df = 100, p < .000; [chi square]/df = 3.60; CFI = .918; TLI = .901;
RMSEA = .080(CI= .071-.89)]. Taken together, this supports full that are
not a subset of one another (cf. Model 1 vs. Model 2 or 3), we used
Akaike information criterion (AIC) and Bayesian information criterion
(BIC). AIC and BIC are computed by recreating equally sized,
hypothetically replicated samples (Kline, 2005). Both of these fit
indices are parsimony-adjusted, however BIC adjusts for model complexity
more so than AIC (Kline, 2005). Unlike previously mentioned fit indices,
AIC and BIC both have added advantages of not assuming multivariate
normality, which is not always observed in psychological data (Micceri,
1989). Although regression weights are usually fairly accurate under
cases of multivariate non-normality, some indices are inappropriately
inflated (e.g., [chi square], [chi square]/df, & RMSEA) or deflated
(e.g., CFI, TLI), leading to greater chances of rejecting true models
(Kline, 2005). There are no set cutoff values for these comparative fit
indices, however, models with lower values provide better fit to the
data (Kline, 2005).
Structural Equation Modeling
Model 1: Hall and Fincham (2005). Initial computation of Hall and
Fincham's (2005) SEM elicited a negative error variance associated
with TSD-TP's error term. However, the negative value was not
significantly different from zero (V = -25.11, SE = 17.70, p =.16).
Therefore, we computed an admissible solution by fixing its variance to
zero. Unexpectedly, more externalizing attributions were positively, not
negatively, related with guilt ([beta] = .19, p = .003) and shame
([beta] = .15, p = .003); empathy ([beta] = .28, p < .001) was
positively, not negatively, related to self-forgiveness; and
attributions ([beta] = .10, p = .09) along with conciliatory behaviors
([beta] = -.03, p = .63) were unrelated to self-forgiveness. All other
predicted relations were maintained or weakly maintained (Figure 1).
Nonetheless, these predicted relations should not be entirely trusted
because of poor model fit [[chi square] = 1563.73, df = 262, p <
.001; [chi square]/df = 5.97; CFI = .730; TLI = .691; RMSEA = .111 (CI =
.105-.116); AIC = 1689.73; BIC = 1942.13]; however, significant
multivariate non-normality (Table 3) might have increased the chance of
rejecting a true model.
Model 2: Hall and Fincham (2008). Initial computation of Hall and
Fincham's (2008) SEM elicited a negative error variance associated
with TSD-TP's error term. However, the negative value was not
significantly different from zero (V = -124.90, SE = 76.13, p = .10).
Therefore, we fixed the variance at zero. Whereas severity of
transgressions ([beta] = .06, p = .17) and conciliatory behaviors
([beta] = .03, p = .59) were unrelated with self-forgiveness, all other
predicted relations were maintained at the .001 level (Figure 2). This
pattern of results indicated that offense severity and conciliatory
behaviors were mediated by guilt and perceived forgiveness. Model 2
elicited poor to adequate model fit [[chi square] = 358.14, df = 98, p
< .000; [chi square]/df = 3.65; CFI = .918; TLI = .899; RMSEA =
.081(CI = .072-.09); AIC = 434.14; BIC = 586.38]. Nevertheless,
multivariate nonnormality (Table 3) may have decreased model fit. In
this case, Hall and Fincham (2008) might truly approximate a great
fitting model and thus the results of Model 2 are inconclusive.
Model 3: Alternative model. Initial computation of the alternative
model elicited a negative error variance associated with TSD-TP's
error term. However, the negative value was not significantly different
from zero (V = -136.95, SE = 86.23, p = .12). Thus, we again fixed the
variance at zero. All predicted relations were strongly maintained at
the .001 level (Figure 3). Most notably, guilt accounted for 47.61% of
the variance in self-forgiveness. The alternative model elicited
adequate to good model fit [[chi square] = 360.20, df = 100, p <
.000; [chi square]/df = 3.60; CFI = .918; TLI = .901; RMSEA = .080(CI =
.071-.89); AIC = 432.20; BIC = 576.43]. Nevertheless, multivariate
non-normality (Table 3) may have decreased model fit and thus, the
alternative model might truly approximate a great fitting model.
Comparing structural models. Because Model 1 vs. Model 2 or 3 are
non-hierarchical comparisons, we could use only AIC and BIC to directly
test the equal fit hypotheses. When comparing Model 1 with Model 2/3,
both AIC ([DELTA]1255.59/[DELTA]1257.53) and BIC
([DELTA]1355.75/[DELTA]1365.70) had large value decreases, suggesting
that Hall and Fincham (2008) and the alternative are more preferred over
Hall and Fincham (2005). Model 2 elicited negligible relations for
severity of transgressions and conciliatory behaviors when predicting
self-forgiveness. In addition, there was a small increase in fit between
Model 2 [[chi square] = 358.14, df = 98, p < .000; [chi square]/df =
3.65; CFI = .918; TLI = .899; RMSEA = .081(CI = .072-.09)] and Model 3
[[chi square] = 360.20, df = 100, p < .000; [chi square]/df = 3.60;
CFI = .918; TLI = .901; RMSEA = .080 (CI= .071-.89)]. Taken together,
this supports full conciliatory behaviors and perceived forgiveness from
their victims. According to the results of this study, conciliatory
behaviors do not directly impact self-forgiveness, rather perceived
forgiveness fully mediates their relations. However, guilt appeared to
impact self-forgiveness most strongly when compared to other variables
explored in this study. Therefore, transgressors' conciliatory
behaviors and perceived forgiveness may partially mediate the relations
between guilt and self-forgiveness. Alternatively, there also may be
other mechanisms unaccounted for in the alternative model that dissolve
guilt, such as relationship closeness, beliefs surrounding the
appropriateness of self-forgiveness, self-respect, or existential variables. Future studies should pay close attention to the possible
indirect or direct relations between guilt and self-forgiveness.
Therapeutic Implications
The results of the current study, alongside Hall and Fincham
(2008), suggest therapists may aid clients in forgiving themselves by
focusing on dissolving guilt. For instance, clients might lessen their
guilt by exploring the possibility of unrealistic beliefs about
transgression severity (Worthington, 2006). In this sense, transgressors
may catastrophize the extent to which their actions impacted their
victims. They also may resolve their guilt by utilizing conciliatory
behaviors, real or symbolic, in attempts to make amends (Hall &
Fincham, 2005; 2008). Through this process, clients may experience a
sense of being restored by their victims and consequently their journeys
toward self-forgiveness could be accelerated (Enright, 1996). On the
other hand, even if their victims forgave them, clients still could hold
feelings of unforgiveness. Therapists are in key positions to challenge
their clients' maladaptive beliefs while also providing empathic,
safe environments in which their clients can learn to let go
(Worthington, 2006). Further, some clients may experience shame and
appear uninterested in self-forgiveness. Therapists could perhaps aid
their clients in shifting from experiencing shame to feeling guilt by
changing foci from unchangeable character flaws to unfixed behavioral
errors (Hall & Fincham, 2005; 2008; Tangney & Dearing, 2002).
Mental health workers also may find Enright's (1996),
Flanigan's (1996), and Worthington's (2006) stage models of
self-forgiveness helpful in treating clients who desire to forgive
themselves.
Conclusion
With the recent expansion of self-forgiveness literature,
researchers identified a number of variables believed to play roles in
the process of self-forgiveness (cf. Hall & Fincham, 2005, 2008). We
found several of these proposed antecedents--severity of transgressions,
guilt, conciliatory behaviors, and perceived forgiveness--appeared to
represent the best fitting model of self-forgiveness (Figure 3).
Moreover, each of the proposed paths elicited highly significant or
strong relations. Contrary to a recent study investigating the
dispositional aspects of Hall and Fincham's (2005) model
(Rangganadhan & Todorov, 2010), we found that a mediating model
quite similar to Hall and Fincham (2008) elicited the best fitting and
most parsimonious model when we included both dispositional and trait
measures in a structural model.
References
Arbuckle, J. L. (2007). Amos 16.0 User's Guide. Chicago, IL:
Amos Development Corporation.
Avery, C. M. (2008). The relationship between self-forgiveness and
health: Mediating variables and implications for well-being (Doctoral
dissertation). Dissertation Abstracts International: Section B. Sciences
and Engineering, 69, 1939.
Barbetta, F. (2002). An exploration of the relationship between
empathy, forgiving others and self-forgiveness. (Doctoral dissertation).
Dissertation Abstracts International: Section B. Sciences and
Engineering, 64(1), 407B.
Batson, C. D., Bolen, M. H., Cross, J. A., &
Neuringer-Benefiel, H. E. (1986). Where is the altruism in the
altruistic personality? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
50, 212-220. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.50.1.212
Bauer, L., Duffy, J., Fountain, E., Halling, S., Holzer, M., &
Jones, E.,Rowe, J. E. (1992). Exploring self-forgiveness. Journal of
Religion and Health, 31, 149-160.
Coke, J. S., Batson, C. D., & McDavis, K. (1978). Empathic
mediation of helping: A two-stage model. Journal of Personality and
Social Psychology, 36, 752-766. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.36.7.752
Crowne, D. P., & Marlowe, D. (1960). A new scale of social
desirability independent of psychopathology. Journal of Consulting and
Clinical Psychology, 24, 349-354.
Davis, M. (1980). A multidimensional approach to individual
differences in empathy. Catalog of Selected Documents in Psychology, 10,
85-103.
Davis, M. (1983). Measuring individual differences in empathy:
Evidence for a multidimensional approach. Journal of Personality and
Social Psychology, 44, 113-126. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.44.1.113
Dillon, R. S. (2001). Self-forgiveness and self-respect. Ethics,
112, 53-83.
Enright, R. D. (1996). Counseling within the forgiveness triad: On
forgiving, receiving forgiveness, and self-forgiveness. Counseling and
Values, 40, 107-127.
Exline, J. J., DeShea, L., & Holeman, V. T. (2007). Is apology
worth the risk? Predictors, outcomes, and ways to avoid regret. Journal
of Social and Clinical Psychology, 26, 479-504. doi:
10.1521/jscp.2007.26.4.479
Feshbach, N. D. (1975). Empathy in children: Some theoretical and
empirical considerations. The Counseling Psychologist, 5(2), 25-30. doi:
10.1177/001100007500500207
Fisher, M. L., & Exline, J. J. (2006). Self-forgiveness versus
excusing: The roles of remorse, effort, and acceptance of
responsibility. Journal of Self and Identity, 5, 127-146. doi:
10.1080/15298860600586123
Flanigan, B. (1996). Forgiving yourself: A step-by-step guide to
making peace with your mistakes and getting on with your life. New York:
Macmillan.
Frazier, P. A., Tix, A. P., & Barron, K. E. (2004). Testing
moderator and mediator effects in counseling psychology research.
Journal of Counseling Psychology, 51, 115-134. doi:
10.1037/0022-0167.51.1.115
Griswold, C. L. (2007). Forgiveness: A philosophical exploration.
New York: Cambridge University Press.
Hall, J. H., & Fincham F. D. (2005). Self-forgiveness: The
stepchild of forgiveness research. Journal of Social and Clinical
Psychology, 24, 621-637. doi: 10.1521/jscp.2005.24.5.621
Hall, J. H., & Fincham F. D. (2008). The temporal course of
self-forgiveness. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 27,
174-202. doi: 10.1521/jscp.2008.27.2.174
Harder, D. W., Rockart, L., & Cutler, L. (1993). Additional
validity evidence for the Harder Personal Feelings Questionnaire-2
(PFQ2): A measure of shame and guilt proneness. Journal of Clinical
Psychology, 49, 345-348.
Harder, D. W., & Zalma, A. (1990). Two promising shame and
guilt scales: A construct validity comparison. Journal of Personality
Assessment, 55(3 & 4), 729-745. doi:
10.1207/s15327752jpa5503&4_30
Hu, L. T., & Bentler, P. M. (1999). Cutoff criteria for fit
indexes in covariance structure analysis: Conventional criteria versus
new alternatives. Structural Equation Modeling, 6, 1-55. doi:
10.1080/10705519909540118
Hughes, P. M. (1994). On forgiving oneself: A reply to Snow. The
Journal of Value Inquiry, 28, 557-560.
Ingersoll-Dayton, B., & Krause, N. (2005). Self-forgiveness: A
component of mental health in later life. Research on Aging, 27,
267-289. doi: 10.1177/0164027504274122
Kline, R. B. (2005). Principles and Practice of Structural Equation
Modeling (2nd ed.). New York: The Guilford Press.
Layer, S. D., Roberts, C., Wild, K., & Walters, J. (2004).
Postabortion grief: Evaluating the possible efficacy of a spiritual
group intervention. Research on Social Work Practice, 14, 344-350. doi:
10.1177/1049731504265829
Lewis, H. B. (1971). Shame and guilt in neurosis. New York:
International University Press.
Little, T. D., Cunningham, W. A., Shahar, G., & Widaman, K. F.
(2002). To parcel or not to parcel: Exploring the question, weighing the
merits. Structural Equation Modeling, 9, 151-173.
Macaskill, A., Maltby, J., & Day, L. (2002). Forgiveness of
self and others and emotional empathy. The Journal of Social Psychology,
142, 633-665. doi: 10.1080/00224540209603925
Marschall, D., Sanfter, J., & Tangney, J. P. (1994). The State
Shame and Guilt Scale. George Mason University, Fairfax, VA.
Marsh, H. W., Hau, K., & Wen, Z. (2004). In search of golden
rules: Comment on hypothesis-testing approaches to setting cutoff values
for fit indexes and dangers in overgeneralizing Hu and Bentler's
(1999) findings. Structural Equation Modeling, 11, 320-341. doi:
10.1207/s15328007sem1103_2
Martin, A. M. (2008). Exploring forgiveness: The relationship
between feeling forgiven by God and self-forgiveness for an
interpersonal offense. (Doctoral dissertation). Retrieved from
University Microforms International (3321224).
McConnell, J. M., & Dixon, D. N. (2012). Perceived forgiveness
from God and self-forgiveness. Journal of Psychology and Christianity.
McCullough, M. E., Worthington, E. L. Jr., & Rachal, K. C.
(1997). Interpersonal forgiving in close relationships. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 73, 321-336. doi:
10.1037/0022-3514.73.2.321
McCullough, M. E., Rachal, K. C., Sandage, S. J., Worthington, E.
L. Jr., Brown, S. W., & Hight, T. L. (1998). Interpersonal forgiving
in close relationships: II. Theoretical elaboration and measurement.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 75, 1586-1603. doi:
10.1037/0022-3514.75.6.1586
Micceri, T. (1989). The unicorn, the normal curve, and other
improbable creatures. Psychological Bulletin, 105, 156-166. doi:
10.1037/0033-2909.105.1.156
Olshtain, E. (1989). Apologies across languages. In S. Blum-Kulka,
J. House, & G. Kasper (Eds.), Cross-cultural pragmatics: Requests
and apologies (pp. 157). Norwood, NJ: Ablex.
Rangganadhan, A. R., & Todorov, N. (2010). Personality and
self-forgiveness: The roles of shame, guilt, empathy, and conciliatory
behavior. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 29, 1-22. doi:
10.1521/jscp.2010.29.1.1
Rusch, N., Corrigan, P. W., Bohus, M., Jacob, G. A., Brueck, R.,
& Lieb, K. (2007). Measuring shame and guilt by self-report
questionnaires: A validation study. Psychiatry Research, 150, 313-325.
doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2006.04.018
Russell, D. (1982). The causal dimension scale: A measure of how
individuals perceive causes. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 42, 1137-1145. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.42.6.1137
Spencer, S. J., Zanna, M. P., & Fong, G. T. (2005).
Establishing a causal chain: Why experiments are often more effective
than meditational analyses in examining psychological processes. Journal
of Personality and Social Psychology, 89, 845-851. doi:
10.1037/0022-3514.89.6.845
Strelan, P. (2007). Who forgives others, themselves, and
situations? The roles of narcissism, guilt, self-esteem, and
agreeableness. Personality and Individual Differences, 42, 259-269. doi:
10.1016/j.paid.2006.06.017
Tangney, J. P. (1995). Recent advances in the empirical study of
shame and guilt. American Behavioral Scientist, 38, 1132-1145. doi:
10.1177/0002764295038008008
Tangney, J. P., Barlow, D. H., Wagner, P. E., Marschall, D. E.,
Borenstein, J. K., Santfer, J., ... Gramzow, R. (1996). Assessing
individual differences in constructive vs. destructive responses to
anger across the lifespan. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
70, 780-796. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.70.4.780
Tangney, J. P., Boone, A. L., & Dearing, R. L. (2005).
Forgiving the self: Conceptual issues and empirical findings. In E. L.,
Worthington, Jr. (Ed.), Handbook of forgiveness (pp.143-158). New York:
Routledge.
Tangney, J. P., Boone, A. L., Dearing, R. L., & Reinsmith, C.
(2002, August). Individual differences in the propensity to forgive:
Measurement and implications for psychological and social adjustment.
Paper presented at the 1999 American Psychological Association
Convention, Boston, MA.
Tangney, J. P., Dearing, R. L., Wagner, P.E., & Gramzow, R.
(2000). The Test of Self-Conscious Affect--3 (TOSCA-3). George Mason
University, Fairfax VA.
Tangney, J. P., & Dearing, R. L. (2002). Shame and guilt. New
York: The Guilford Press.
Tangney, J. P., Miller, R. S., Flicker, L., & Barlow, D. H.
(1996). Are shame, guilt, and embarrassment distinct emotions? Journal
of Personality and Social Psychology, 70, 1256-1269.
Tangney, J. P., Wagner, P. E., & Gramzow, R. (1992). Proneness
of shame, proneness to guilt, and psychopathology. Journal of Abnormal
Psychology, 103, 469-478. doi: 10.1037/0021 843X.101.3.469
Thompson, L. Y., Snyder, C. R., Hoffman, L., Michael, S. T.,
Rasmussen, H. N., Billings, L. S., . Roberts, D. E. (2005).
Dispositional forgiveness of self, others, and situations. Journal of
Personality, 73, 313-360. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-6494.2005.00311.x
Weiner, B., Graham, S., Peter, O., & Zmuidinas, M. (1991).
Public confession and forgiveness. Journal of Personality, 59, 281-312.
doi: 10.1111/j.1467-6494.1991.tb00777.x
Witvliet, C. V., Ludwig, T. E., & Bauer, D. J. (2002). Please
forgive me: Transgressors' emotions and physiology during imagery
of seeking forgiveness and victim responses. Journal of Psychology and
Christianity, 21, 219-233.
Wohl, M. J. A., DeShea, L., & Wahkinney, R. L. (2008). Looking
within: Measuring state self-forgiveness and its relationship to
psychological wellbeing. Canadian Journal of Behavioural Science, 40,
1-10. doi: 10.1037/0008-400x.40.1.1.1
Worthington, E. L. (2006). Forgiveness and reconciliation: Theory
and application. New York: Routledge.
Zechmeister, J. S., & Romero, C. (2002). Victim and offender
accounts of interpersonal conflict: Autobiographical narratives of
forgiveness and unforgiveness. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 82, 675-686. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.82.4.675
Table 1--Summary Table of Scale Properties
[alpha] X SD VIF
TSD-TP .89 59.53 11.63
TSD-VP .77 60.74 13.82
CDS .45- ([dagger]) ([dagger])
TOSCA-3:PE .73 40.65 8.53
MFI:BO .82 16.36 6.35
MFI:BS .76 18.82 5.47
IRI:EC .70 27.69 4.03
IRI:PT .78 24.70 4.97
CERS .87 20.38 8.45
SSGS: Shame .88 12.94 5.45
PFQ: Shame .84 27.51 7.84
TOSCA-3: .76 50.22 9.17
Shame
SGGS: Guilt .83 16.61 5.02
PFQ: Guilt .81 19.31 5.23
TOSCA-3: Guilt .73 65.70 6.84
(Parcel 1) CBS .80 16.93 3.95
(Parcel 2) CBS .81 11.79 2.88
MFI:AF .76 32.90 5.45
TRIM .95 61.20 18.83
(Parcel 1) .80 10.55 3.18
HFSO
(Parcel 2) .81 10.49 3.49
HFSO
(Parcel 3) .80 10.74 3.16
HFSO
MFI:FS .72 22.60 5.25
SSFA .90 21.30 5.72
SFB .94 27.96 6.42
HFSS .74 30.37 6.52
MC-SDS .78KR 13.20 3.00
[alpha] Skewness Kurtosis
TSD-TP 3.91 -.22 .34
TSD-VP 3.96 -.14 -.14
CDS ([dagger]) ([dagger]) ([dagger])
TOSCA-3:PE 1.63 .32 * .38
MFI:BO 1.60 .74 * .07
MFI:BS 1.72 .43 * .16
IRI:EC 1.62 -.43 * .01
IRI:PT 1.41 -.16 -.14
CERS 1.37 .20 -.65 *
SSGS: Shame 3.36 .30 * -.86 *
PFQ: Shame 2.39 -.25 * -.36
TOSCA-3: 1.94 -.23 -.30
Shame
SGGS: Guilt 3.16 -.33 * -.57 *
PFQ: Guilt 2.82 -.28 * 0.32
TOSCA-3: Guilt 2.10 -.73 * .87 *
(Parcel 1) CBS 2.84 -1.25 * 1.43 *
(Parcel 2) CBS 2.73 -1.66 * 2.35 *
MFI:AF 1.25 -.70 * -.07
TRIM 2.21 -.41 * -.95 *
(Parcel 1) 2.96 -1.04 * .50 *
HFSO
(Parcel 2) 2.31 -.76 * -.41
HFSO
(Parcel 3) 3.55 -.84 * -.05
HFSO
MFI:FS 1.67 .33 * .05
SSFA 3.36 -.06 -.71 *
SFB 3.01 -.62 * -.39
HFSS 1.93 -.18 -.45
MC-SDS 1.12 .14 -.50 *
Note. [alpha] = Cronbach's Alpha; KR = Kuder-Richardson-20
Alpha; X = Mean; SD = Standard Deviation; VIF = Multicollinearity.
Items were parceled in all analyses in order to provide enough
indicators for the latent variable.
* Indicates significant non-normality. ([dagger]) The researcher
excluded CDS from all analyses due to poor internal consistency
reliabilities.
Table 3--Summary Table of Goodness-of-Fit Indices
[chi square] df P [chi square]/df CFI
H&F (2005) 1563.73 262 .000 5.97 .730
H&F (2008) 358.14 98 .000 3.7 .918
Alternative 360.20 100 .000 3.6 .918
TLI RM-SEA RM-SEA (CI) AIC
H&F (2005) .691 .111 .105-.116 1689.73
H&F (2008) .899 .051 .072-.090 434.14
Alternative .901 .080 .071-.089 432.20
MIC BIC ABIC Multivariate
Normality
H&F (2005) -- 1942.13 -- 51.03 *
H&F (2008) -1255.59 586.38 -1355.75 33.62 *
Alternative -1.94 576.43 -9.95 33.62 *
Note. * Indicates significant non-normality