Does program trading cause stock prices to overreact?
Moser, James T.
Program trading constitutes a substantial fraction of the trading
activity in the New York Stock Exchange. The volume of stocks exchanged
in orders labeled program trades typically averages 10 percent of total
volume. This article investigates the claim that program trading causes
stock prices to overreact. Stock price overreaction is important because
investors are thought to use stock prices as guides to the best uses of
their capital. If prices overreact to new information, they cannot
provide accurate guidance. Evidence of such stock price overreaction
would be indicative of excessive volatility.
Previous researchers have considered the possible effect of program
trading on volatility, a proposition which requires a model for the
natural evolution of volatility. Because such a model lacks general
acceptance, this article offers an alternative approach. I investigate
whether program trades increase the odds of price reversions.
Prices reverse when previously encountered price decreases are
immediately followed by an increase in price; or when a previously
encountered price increase is immediately followed by a price decrease.
Price reversions can be expected to occur when price changes are larger
than the value changes warranted by new information, that is, when
prices overreact to new information. In instances of overreaction,
informed traders will find current prices out of line with their
valuations. The trades of these informed traders will then bring prices
back toward their original levels. This depiction follows that of Black
(1985).
This article examines whether program trading should be classified
as a type of noise trading or as a type of information trading. If
levels of program trading increase the likelihood that a price reversal
will occur, we can conclude that program trading is a type of noise
trading. On the other hand, if program trading is unrelated to the
likelihood of encountering a price reversal, then program trading should
be categorized as information trading. I examine a 34-month period of
daily program trading activity and stock prices and use a logit
specification to consider the proposition that trading activity changes
the probability of stock price reversals. The results do not support the
claim that program trading causes stock price overreactions.
Literature review
Much of the literature on program trading considers its effect on
stock price volatility. Stoll and Whaley (1986, 1987, 1988, 1990)
examine the consequences of program trading occurring on
"triple-witching days," that is, dates when multiple
derivative contracts on stocks simultaneously expire. As heavy program
trading frequently occurs on these expiration dates, Stoll and
Whaley's evidence of higher volatility suggests that program
trading can be linked to increased volatility. Edwards (1988) studies
the impact of stock-index futures and finds that volatility does not
increase after the introduction of multiple derivative contracts. Since
these contracts are frequently involved in program trading strategies,
an increase in stock price volatility would be consistent with a program
trading effect. Maberly, Allen, and Gilbert (1989) note that this result
depends on the sample period. Harris (1989) finds only a slight increase
in volatility during the 1980s, suggesting that the increased program
trading activity that was facilitated by futures trading had, at most, a
very modest effect on volatility. Martin and Senchack (1989, 1991) find
that the volatility of stocks included in the Major Market Index (MMI)
rose after the MMI futures contract was introduced. Their risk
decomposition indicates that the systematic risk of these stocks rose.
Since the MMI futures contract is frequently involved in program
trading, this finding suggests that program trading led to higher
volatility.
Program trading grew rapidly during the 1980s. If this activity
increases volatility, then the 1980s should have shown higher than usual
volatility. Several investigators have examined the period for changes
in volatility. Froot, Perold, and Stein (1991) investigate returns on
the Standard and Poor 500 since the 1930s. They find that changes in
volatility are conditional on the length of the holding period. There is
strong evidence of an increase in return volatility during the 1980s for
15-minute holding periods. When longer holding periods are examined, it
is much less evident that volatility has changed. Miller (1990) suggests
a conceptual distinction between the volatility of price changes and
price-change velocity. While statistical tests frequently demonstrate no
change in volatility levels, the speed of price adjustments does appear
to have increased during the 1980s. Froot and Perold (1990) decompose price changes into bid-ask bounce, nontrading effects, and
noncontemporaneous cross-stock correlations. They demonstrate that price
adjustments occurred more rapidly during the 1980s.
Direct investigation of the effects of program trading finds
temporary increases in volatility which are most prominent in index
arbitrage activities. Much of this evidence is reviewed by Duffee,
Kupiec, and White (I 990). Grossman (1988) regresses various measures of
daily price volatility on program trading intensity, finding no
significant effect. A Securities and Exchange Commission study (1989)
finds a positive association between daily volatility of changes in the
Dow Jones Index and levels of program trading activity. Furbush (I 989)
finds a significant relationship between price volatility and program
trading activity in the three days prior to the October 19, 1987, market
break. Harris, Sofianos, and Shapiro (I 990) and Neal (1991) investigate
intraday program trading, finding that responses to program trades are
similar to those found for block trades. Using "GARCH"
estimation procedures, Moser (1994) finds a modest increase in the
volatility of returns for one-day holding periods associated with sell
program activity. Thus, the evidence is inconclusive. The logit
specification developed in the next section offers an alternative
approach to examining this question.
Data sets and sample description
Trading activity data for this study are from the New York Stock
Exchange (NYSE).(1) The data set includes aggregate trading volume and
share volumes involved in programmed trades. The sample consists of 717
daily observations from the period January 1, 1988, through October 31,
1990. Program trades are presently classified as buys, sells, and short
sales.
Program trading activity is the number of shares included in orders
identified as program trades. The NYSE defines program trades as orders
involving 15 or more stocks having a combined market value in excess of
one million dollars. The program trades of this sample include shares
exchanged through SuperDOT.(2)
Price reversals are constructed from a data set of percentage
changes, denoted [R.sub.t], in the Dow Jones Industrial Averages. This
stock index is useful in that it is computed from prices for heavily
traded stocks which are frequently involved in program trades. Thus, if
program trading does lead to price overreaction, this effect should be
most pronounced in these stocks. Reversals, denoted [r.sub.t], are
computed for the stock return sample as follows:
[Mathematical Expression Omitted] Equation I specifies an indicator
variable assigned a value of one on sample dates when the unanticipated
portion of the return at t-j-1 has the opposite sign as that of the
unanticipated return realized at t-j for the holding period from t-j-1
through t-j; on other dates, the indicator variable is set to zero.
Equation 2 states that unanticipated returns are computed as actual
returns minus their corresponding expectations. Expected returns are
generated assuming that stock prices can be described by a martingale;
that is, E([R.sub.t]) = 0. As the next section points out, considering
various values of the lag j permits longer intervals for prices to
correct following a price overreaction.
Estimating reversal probabilities conditional on trading
activity
Let Z represent a vector of index values with each element measuring
the propensity of the market to produce a reversal. The proposition that
program trading encourages overreaction as demonstrated by stock price
reversals, implies that the index should be related to levels of program
trading activity. If this is true, the data should allow us to reject
the null hypothesis that program trading has no effect. Defining X as a
matrix of k measures of the trading activity variables, we write
Z=X[beta]
so that levels of the index are predicted by activity levels and
their coefficients. The overreaction null predicts that [beta] will
differ from zero. The level of this index can also be described as
determining the probability of
encountering a reversal conditional on trading activity. The vector
of these probabilities can be written as P=F(Z). Taking F() to be the
cumulative logistic probability function, these probabilities of
reversals are given by
[Mathematical Expression Omitted]
Taking logs and rearranging gives the following logit specification:
[Mathematical Expression Omitted]
Equation 3 is estimated using the method of maximum likelihood. The
expression for the log likelihood is
[Mathematical Expression Omitted]
where T is the number of observed changes in returns and [x.sub.t-j]
are the activity variables observed at dates t-j. Lagging the activity
variables coincides with the null under investigation. When j=1, the
null hypothesis under investigation asks, does heavy trading activity at
date t-1 consistently cause stock prices to overreact? If program
trading activity caused an overreaction on this date, and if a price
correction occurred in the one trading period since the overreaction,
then a price reversal is realized, provided the information arriving at
t does not overwhelm the amount of price correction. If these conditions
hold, then the coefficients on the activity variables will differ from
zero, reflecting the average impact from trading activity. Thus, the
test specification jointly considers three questions:
1) Does overreaction occur? 2) Are price corrections realized the
following day? 3) Is the amount of the price correction
masked by the value of newly arriving
information?
The null hypothesis of no effect can be rejected only if the answer
to each of these questions is yes.
The third of these conditions is addressed a priori. Amounts of
price corrections are masked by valuations of new information only if
the value of new information is larger and has the same price change
implications as the previous overreaction. As the distribution of value
changes based on new information is likely to be symmetric, it is not
likely that more than half of the overreactions will be masked by value
changes attributable to new information. Further, if price changes due
to overreactions are generally smaller than those caused by value
changes due to new information, then the problem of overreaction may not
be as large as often portrayed. These considerations reduce the problem
of new information masking overreactions to an efficiency concern rather
than a bias concern. This leaves two conditions. As the inferences that
can be drawn regarding the primary question of overreaction depend on
the answer to the second question, I attempted to lessen the dependence
on the length of the correction interval. I did this by examining longer
correction intervals. Specifically, I extended the hypothesis to
consider whether trading activity at t-j produces overreactions on that
date which are corrected over the interval from t-j to t. Thus I
considered the possibility that correction intervals last longer than
one day. This still left open the possibility that corrections occur in
less than one day.
Table I reports estimates of the logit specification given in
equation 3 for price correction intervals of one through five trading
days. Coefficients on the activity variables are generally small.
Evidence of price reversals attributable to buy program activity is
present for correction intervals of four trading days. Though
statistically significant, the impact on price reversal probabilities is
small. To gauge the relevance of this coefficient, I evaluated it at
average levels of trading activity. Price reversals for this correction
interval occur in 19.69 per cent of the sampled trading days. Taking
this as the unconditional probability of a reversal, the conditional
probability of a reversal increases by approximately.00037 for each
increment of 1,000 shares executed in buy programs above the
sample-average level of 8,044,000. At one standard deviation above this
average level of trading activity-- 17.5 million shares--buy programs
increase the probability of a reversal by 3.49 percent, with the
reversal from this activity being realized over the succeeding four
trading days.
[TABULAR DATA 1 OMITTED]
Comparing the coefficients across the three categories of trading
activity included in these regressions, nonprogram trading appears to be
a more reliable cause of price reversals. Coefficients on nonprogram
trading differ reliably from zero for the two-, three-, and five-day
correction intervals. Again the magnitudes of these effects are small.
At one standard deviation above average nonprogram trading activity, the
probability of a reversal increases by 3.4 percent for the three-day
correction interval. The magnitudes of impacts on reversal probabilities
for the four- and five-day correction intervals are similar.
Pseudo [R.sup.2] values are computed following Judge et al. (I 985)
for each specification. The low values of these [R.sup.2] values implies
that trading activity explains a very small portion of the overall
variation in reversals. To consider the explanatory power of our
specifications, I conducted a likelihood ratio test. Under the null
hypothesis of no effect, the maximum value of the likelihood function is
[Mathematical Expression Omitted]
where n is the number of reversals and T the number of sample dates.
Specifications can be tested using a likelihood ratio test for the
difference between this maximum log likelihood and the log likelihood
obtained from the estimation procedure. For the sample of one-period
correction intervals, the maximum log likelihood under the null
hypothesis is -495.29, which is only slightly smaller than the actual
value of --494.24. The critical value of twice this difference is 7.81
for the 95 percent level of confidence. Thus, the data fails to reject
the null hypothesis; that is, the results for the one-day correction
interval do not support an association between trading activity and
price reversals. These differences are 6.85, 7.85, 6.56, and 5.96 for
the two-, three-, four-, and five-day correction intervals,
respectively. The critical value is exceeded only at the three-day
correction interval. This implies that trading activity does lead to
price overreactions which are subsequently corrected in three trading
days. The individual coefficients indicate that it is nonprogram trading
which produces these overreactions rather than buy or sell trading
activity.
Recall that the price correction intervals considered in this paper
are whole trading days. Fractional trading days are not considered.
Thus, overreactions with a subsequent correction within the same trading
day cannot be detected using a sample of daily returns as in this
article. Previous research does investigate within-day reversals.
Harris, Sofianos, and Shapiro (1990) and Neal (1991) find that the price
impact of an average program trade is similar to that found for block
trades. We conclude that price reversals, where found, are generally
small. This implies that current trading mechanisms are usually quick to
resolve those price overreactions attributable to program trading. Given
the current effectiveness of these mechanisms, changes such as the
imposition of transaction taxes or other institutional arrangements
appear to be unwarranted.
Conclusion
Descriptions of stock market results frequently give the impression
that program trading causes prices to overreact to current information.
Some have proposed policy changes intended to dampen the effects of the
extent of these overreactions. This article introduces a procedure to
test the proposition that program trading causes price overreactions.
Given the evidence presented, it appears that program trading activity
does not cause price overreactions.
NOTES
(1) I am indebted to Deborah Sosebee and her staff at the NYSE. They
provided the data on program trading and patiently answered many
questions. (2) Most but not all program trades at the NYSE are routed
through SuperDOT, a computerized routing system. Large brokerage houses
can arrange to have their program trades executed by floor brokers, but
this method is more costly and slower. The weekly summaries of program
trading reported in the financial press include program trades executed
off the SuperDOT system. These data are unavailable on a daily basis.
Program trading reported in the weekly summaries for the period 1/ 1 /88
through 9/22/90 averaged 16.4 million shares per day. Program trades in
this sample over the same period averaged 15.9 million shares. This
suggests that program trades executed off the SuperDOT system account
for only about 3 percent of program trading activity.
REFERENCES
Black, Fisher A., "Noise," Journal of Finance, Vol. 52,
1985, pp. 4-24.
Duffee, Greg, Paul Kupiec, and Patricia White, "A primer on
program trading and stock market volatility: A survey of the issues and
the evidence," Board of Governors of the Federal Reserve System,
finance and economics discussion paper, No. 109, January 1990.
Edwards, Franklin R., "Does futures trading increase stock
market volatility," Financial Analysts Journal January/February
1988, pp. 63-69.
Froot, Kenneth A., and Andre F. Perold, "New trading practices
and short-run market efficiency," NBER, working paper, No. 3498,
1990.
Froot, Kenneth A., Andre F. Perold, and Jeremy C. Stein,
"Shareholder trading practices and corporate investment
horizons," NBER, working paper, No. 3638, 1991.
Furbush, Dean, "A study of program trading and price movements
around the 1987 market break," Securities and Exchange Commission,
working paper, May 1989.
Gallant, A. Ronald, Peter E. Rossi, and George Tauchen, "Stock
prices and volume," Review of Financial Studies, Vol. 5, 1992, pp.
199-242.
Grossman, Sanford, "An analysis of the implications for stock
and futures price volatility of program trading and dynamic hedging strategies," Journal of Business, Vol. 61, 1988, pp. 275-298.
Harris, Lawrence, "S&P 500 cash stock price
volatilities," Journal of Finance, Vol. 44, 1989, pp. 1155-1176.
Harris, Lawrence, George Sofianos, and James E. Shapiro,
"Program trading and intraday volatility," New York Stock
Exchange, working paper, No. 90-03, 1990.
Judge, George G., W.E. Griffiths, R. Carter Hill, Helmut Lutkepohl,
and Tsoung-Chao Lee, The Theory and Practice of Econometrics, New York:
John Wiley and Sons, 1985.
Maberly, Edwin D., David S. Allen, and Roy F. Gilbert, "Stock
index futures and cash market volatility," Financial Analysts
Journal, November/December 1989, pp. 75-77.
Martin, John D., and A.J. Senchack, Jr., "Program trading and
systematic stock price behavior," Financial Analysts Journal,
May/June 1989, pp. 61-67.
__, "Index futures, program trading, and the covariability of
the major market index stocks," Journal of Futures Markets, Vol.
11, 199 1, pp. 95 -111.
Miller, Merton H., "Index arbitrage and volatility,"
Financial Analysts Journal, July/August 1990, pp. 6-7.
Moser, James T., "Trading activity, program trading, and the
volatility of stock returns," Federal Reserve Bank of Chicago,
working paper, 1994.
Najand, Mohammad, and Kenneth Yung, "A GARCH examination of the
relationship between volume and price variability in futures
markets," Journal of Futures Markets, Vol. 11, 1991, pp. 613-621.
Neal, Robert, "Program trading on the NYSE: A descriptive
analysis and estimates of the intra-day impact on stock returns,"
University of Washington, working paper, February 1991.
New York Stock Exchange, "Market volatility and investor
confidence," report to the board of directors of the New York Stock
Exchange,1990.
Schwert, G. William, "Why does stock market volatility change
over time?" Journal of Finance, Vol. 44, 1989, pp. 11 15-1153.
Securities and Exchange Commission, memo on program trading, 1989.
Stoll, Hans R., and Robert E. Whaley, "Expiration day effects of
index options and futures," Monograph Series in Finance and
Economics, New York: New York University, No. 1986-3, 1986.
__, "Program trading and expiration day effects," Financial
Analysts Journal, March/April 1987, pp. 16-28.
__, "Futures and options on stock indexes: Economic purpose,
arbitrage, and market structure," Review of Futures Markets, Vol.
7, 1988, pp. 224-248.
__, "Program trading and individual stock returns: Ingredients
of the triple-witching brew," Journal of Business, Vol. 63, No. 1,
Pt. 2, 1990, pp. s165-s192.
James T. Moser is a senior economist in the research department at
the Federal Reserve Bank of Chicago.