The moral minority: impact of gender, education, work experience and age on moral development in business students.
Steele, Nathan L. ; Branson, Leonard ; Martin, Rachel C. 等
INTRODUCTION
Even preceding the current economic tumult caused in part by
unethical decision making by large corporations, the state of corporate
ethics was much in question. The highly publicized collapse of such
companies as Enron and WorldCom, due to the unethical actions of a few
executives, has generated ripples throughout the economy and the
public's perception of business ethics in practice and in the
educational arena. Boddy (2010) claims that corporate psychopaths are
more commonly found at higher levels of organizations. Upper level
executives and managers at the heart of many of these debacles have
possessed elite credentials from top rated business schools. Jeffrey
Skilling, the infamous former CEO of Enron, received his MBA from
Harvard Business School (Schwartz, 2002). Andrew Fastow, the former CFO
of Enron, graduated from the highly regarded Kellogg School of
Management at Northwestern (Barboza & Schwartz, 2002). Clifford
Baxter, who left Enron immediately prior to its collapse, received his
MBA from NYU (Jennings, 2004). Scott Sullivan, named CFO of the Year by
CFO Magazine in 1998 for his work at WorldCom, received his MBA from
Oswego State (Yang & Grow, 2002). In response to such incidences,
there has been greater public outcry for and scrutiny of the inclusion
of ethical education in business curricula (Desplaces, Melcher,
Beauvais, & Bosco, 2007; Lopez, Rechner, & Olson-Buchanan, 2005;
Pettijohn, Pettijohn, & Taylor, 2008). In fact, the largest
accrediting body for business schools, the Association for the
Advancement of Collegiate Schools of Business (AACSB), has taken great
care to specify that not only should the schools they accredit maintain
ethical codes for their students, administrators, and faculty (AACSB,
2010), but that ethical understanding and reasoning are part of the
fundamental knowledge base expected for students of business and
management. The apparent position of the public and educators in general
is that not enough has been done to educate business practitioners in
both how and why to make ethical decisions. This position assumes that
business education, among other things, can enhance the ethics of these
individuals. As indicated in Frank Ofobike and Gradisher (2010), the
task of teaching ethics in a way that will result in ethical conduct is
a challenge for educators.
Ethics itself is the branch of philosophy that encompasses
"the study of moral judgments--value judgments about what is
virtuous or base, just or unjust, morally right or wrong, morally good
or bad or evil, morally proper or improper" (Moore & Bruder,
2002, p. 210). Business ethics in particular has also been specified in
terms of its relation to the judgment of right and wrong. It has been
defined within the literature as "a specialized study of right and
wrong. It concentrates on how moral standards apply particularly to
business policies, institutions, and behavior" (Velasquez, 2006, p.
16) and alternately as involving "any formal (i.e., identifiable)
activity taking place among individuals, organizations, or other
entities operating within or related to a business context that involves
the explicit interaction and/or application of ethical (i.e., moral)
standards" (Schwartz & Weber, 2006, p. 386). From these
definitions, we may postulate that what is assumed to be affected by
ethics education in particular is the moral reasoning ability of
students.
This study investigates individual differences within and, to a
degree, the success of business education in cultivating the moral
development of business students. By examining the differential levels
of moral reasoning possessed by undergraduate and graduate students
within business and accounting fields of study, we will be able to
better understand the differences that lead to greater and lesser levels
of moral reasoning capacity and development, as well as to identify
indirectly whether or not progressively more educated business students
(MBAs) are more fully developed than their less educated business
student counterparts (undergraduates).
LITERATURE REVIEW
Moral Reasoning
From the original work of Piaget (1932), moral reasoning has been
considered a developmental issue, one that is not inborn in its full
capacity, but rather cultivated through education and exposure to
others. These early developmental theories of moral reasoning were
further fleshed out in the work of Kohlberg (1971), who proposed that
humans go through six basic stages of moral reasoning development,
contained in three levels--pre-conventional, conventional, and
post-conventional--linearly and progressively (i.e., never regressing
after having attained higher levels of moral reasoning).
In Kohlberg's theory, the first level of moral reasoning
development--preconventional--is primarily contained in the early
childhood experiences of an individual and generally concluded with
elementary school (Dawson, 2003). Development during this level is
generally of the ilk of operant conditioning, through punishment and
reward of immoral and moral acts respectively and thusly solicited
through these positive and negative conditioned responses. The second
level of moral reasoning development--conventional--can carry on
throughout the course of adult life. In the first of the two stages of
this level, the third overall, the individual begins to recognize what
is moral by the abstract standards adhered to by others around them, but
only adheres themselves out of pressure to conform (Dawson, 2003). It is
in the transition to the second of the two conventional stages--the
fourth stage overall--that the individual gains a sense of the larger
societal implications and their actions begin to be guided by this
larger moral compass of what is truly "ethical" in the
preservation of that larger societal structure (Dawson, 2003). The
remaining level of development--the post-conventional--is achieved by
very few, and pertains to the development of universal conceptions of
what is moral by principals of fairness rather than the immediate
impacts on society and others (Dawson, 2003).
Measurement of Moral Reasoning
Much work has been conducted to divine the differences between
individuals exhibiting high and low levels of moral reasoning. To wit,
several measures have been constructed over the years. Piaget's
original cognitive development interview method was modified by Kohlberg
to the Moral Judgment Interview (MJI), wherein the interviewee is
presented with several moral dilemmas they respond to. The MJI was so
intricate and exhaustive in both its conduct and scoring that few if any
practitioners utilize it today (Gibbs, Basinger, & Grime, 2003). In
order to facilitate the assessment of larger samples of moral reasoning
development, Rest (1990) developed the Defining Issues Test (DIT) and
its successor the Defining Issues Test 2 (DIT2). These tests were seen
as great improvements in the objective assessment of moral reasoning,
using moral dilemmas followed by statements that were rank ordered in
importance and relevance to the dilemma by responders. One of the latest
psychometric tools developed for assessing moral reasoning, and the one
used in the present research, is the Moral Judgment Test (MJT) developed
by Lind. Lind (2010a; 2010b) developed this measure in order to assess
both the competency aspect of morality (the underlying cognitive
structure) as well as the attitudinal component (the emotional aspect of
how one responds to moral dilemmas).
Individual Differences and Moral Reasoning
Findings have been mixed in assessing the relationship of
individual differences with the development of moral reasoning. Some
studies have found differences between the genders, such that females
present higher levels of moral reasoning (Rest et al., 1999). Still
others have suggested that these differences are hardly noteworthy as
they account for far less of the effect on moral reasoning than
education. For example, one investigation found that education had 250
times greater effect on moral reasoning development than gender
possessed (Rest et al., 1999).
Education does seem to be a prominent factor in the development of
moral reasoning. Rest and Thoma conducted a longitudinal study,
demonstrating a positive linear relationship between education and moral
development (Bouhmama, 2001). Participants tracked for six years post
secondary education developed significant differences between those who
pursued college and those who did not. The moral reasoning of those who
attended college continued to increase while those who did not attend
college remained virtually the same (King & Mayhew, 2002). Within
the broader topic of education, several studies have been conducted to
investigate the role of academic major on moral reasoning development.
Of these, some have found that academic major does predict differences
in the moral reasoning level of students (Bay & Greenberg, 2001;
Rest, 1990). Others have found less clear relationships or a lack
thereof with academic major Rest, 1990).
Although there has been little empirical study of the relationship
between work experience and moral reasoning development, some findings
indicate a positive relationship between the two. For example, Dawson
(1997) found ethical reasoning differences between men and women in
sales such that: 1) females showed higher levels of moral reasoning
development than their male counterparts at all age and experience
levels, 2) moral reasoning increased for both sexes as age and work
experience increased, and 3) differences between males and females
declined as age and experience increased. Other studies also indicate
positive relationships between moral reasoning and work experience as
well as the declination in gender differences as work experience
increases (Franke, Crown, & Spake, 1997; Windsor & Cappel,
1999). Person's (2009) research concluded that female gender,
accounting major, full-time work experience and workplace ethics
training have a positive influence on students ethicality.
While the concept of moral reasoning development is constructed in
such a way as to implicitly suggest that it increases with age, the
findings have not been unanimous in this regard. In fact one of the more
common findings is a declination of moral reasoning with age (e.g.,
Rest, Davison, & Robbins, 1978). Still others have shown positive
relationships between moral reasoning development and age (e.g., Gibbs
& Widaman, 1982; Nisan & Kohlberg, 1982; Rest, 1983; Trevino,
1992). At present, it simply is not known if age is a reliable predictor
of moral reasoning development.
In summary, the findings on moral reasoning development indicate
that gender may have a role that is moderated by work experience, that
education seems to positively influence it, and that age is mixed as a
predictor, but likely positive in its relationship up to a certain point
of development after which declination begins.
HYPOTHESES OF THIS STUDY
In order to investigate the moral reasoning development of
participants, the MJT was employed as a measure of moral competence in
particular. The test itself consists of two dilemmas with participants
rating their initial feelings about each dilemma followed by six
positive arguments and six negative arguments relevant to each dilemma
which participants are then asked to rank using a scale of "I
strongly reject" (-4) to "I strongly accept" (+4). For
example, the "Worker's Dilemma" is as follows:
Due to some seemingly unfounded dismissals, some factory workers
suspect the managers of eavesdropping on their employees through an
intercom and using this information against them. The managers
officially and emphatically deny this accusation. The union
declares that it will only take steps against the company when
proof has been found that confirms these suspicions. Two workers
then break into the administrative offices and take tape
transcripts that prove the allegation of eavesdropping.
Having read this dilemma, participants are then asked, "Do you
accept or reject the following arguments in favor of the two
workers' behavior? Suppose someone argued they were right ..."
after which a series of arguments follow--such as "because they
didn't cause much damage to the company," "because due to
the company's disregard for the law, the means used by the two
workers were permissible to restore law and order," "because
most of the workers would approve of their deed and many of them would
be happy about it," etc.--and participants are required to respond
whether they accept or reject each of these arguments on the -4 to +4
scale.
Scoring the MJT produces two scores: the C-score, which represents
the participant's moral judgment competence, and a score that
reflects which of the six stages of moral reasoning in Kohlberg's
moral development theory best represents the participant's current
state of development (Lind, 2010a; 2010b).
The authors of this study used a broad cross sectional sample that
included undergraduates as well as graduate students with varying levels
of professional experience and academic majors to test our hypotheses.
Because the majority of research on gender has found no significant
effect of gender on moral reasoning development (Rest et al., 1999), we
anticipate no differences between men and women in moral competence
scores:
[H.sub.0]1: There will be no difference in the competency scores of
female and male participants.
Because education has been found in the main to have a positive
impact on moral reasoning development between populations that did and
did not attend college, we assume the moral reasoning development of
graduate students should be significantly higher than the development of
undergraduate students:
[H.sub.a]2: Graduate student participants will have significantly
higher moral competence scores than do undergraduate participants.
Although the results of studies on academic major have been mixed,
it has been found that students who pursue accounting as a major score
significantly higher in moral reasoning as measure by the DIT than do
students whose majors are business administration or liberal arts (Bay
& Greenberg, 2001). As a result, we hypothesize that accounting
majors will have higher levels of moral development than other majors:
[H.sub.a]3: Participants majoring in accounting will have
significantly higher moral competence scores than participants who are
majoring in other disciplines.
The finding that accounting majors had higher levels of moral
reasoning than did other majors has been in part attributed to the more
rigorous ethical training accounting majors receive as part of their
normal curriculum (Branson, Edbrooke-Richardson, & Moe, 2007). As an
extension of our hypothesis on the effect of majoring in accounting
versus other fields, we also postulate that participants working in the
field of accounting will have higher levels of moral development than
those working in other fields, due to the persistent exposure to ethical
requirements and considerations.
[H.sub.a]4: Participants working in the field of accounting will
have significantly higher moral competence scores than those working in
other fields.
Lastly, results are mixed regarding the influence of age on moral
reasoning development. It appears that the declination of moral
reasoning occurs later in life. Consequently, we assume there will be no
effect of age on moral reasoning development within our sample whose
age-while not homogenous--is not representative of the broader spectrum
of ages considered in Kohlberg's stages and other work on the
topic.
[H.sub.0]5: Age will not be a significant predictor of the moral
competence scores of participants.
METHODOLOGY
Sample and Data Collection
One hundred twenty four participants (62 female, 62 male) at a
small Midwestern university were administered both the MJT as well as a
demographic questionnaire. Participants were voluntary, and selected
from senior level accounting and business administration courses at the
undergraduate and graduate levels, in which the measures were
administered. Participants were instructed on and asked to complete the
MJT, and then answer demographic questions on a separate measure giving
their age in number of years, indicating their level of education as
undergraduate junior, undergraduate senior, or graduate student,
identifying their academic major as accounting, business or other with
space available to write in a response, and identifying their work
experience and present field of work. The demographics of participants
can be seen in more detail in Table 1, below.
RESULTS
The mean C-scores, with higher scores representing higher levels of
moral competence, for participants are presented by category in Table 2
below, alongside the p scores for t-tests conducted between category
means.
As can be seen in Table 2, the only categories where significant
differences were found (beside a marginal result of p > .05 for
Business versus "Other") were between Accounting majors and
those students identified as "Other" majors. It should be
noted that the nine participants in the "Other" category were
double majors in accounting and business administration, and that this
cell n was substantially smaller than the cell n for accounting majors
(64) was substantially higher, potentially creating an illusory effect
based on violation of the underlying assumptions of equivalent variance.
Simple linear regression analysis found no significant ability of age to
predict moral competence, and in fact, the relationship appeared to be
marginally negative in posthoc correlational analysis (r = -0.12, ns).
LIMITATIONS OF THIS STUDY
While this study presents a strong insight into the moral reasoning
development (or lack thereof) in students of a school of business and
the influence of individual differences thereupon, and bolsters previous
findings from the literature, it is mainly limited in two ways: the
sample size, and the homogeneity of participants. Participants were not
representative of a broad crosssection in age, work experience, level of
education, college major, and geographic background due in part to their
self-selection into this study as students within the same disciplines
and region. The sample for this study was taken from one college within
a single university in the Midwest. The conclusions do not generalize
readily to the population of business decision makers, nor to the
students of other schools of business. In addition, the cell sizes of
categories under investigation were limited in some instances by
subject-variables present in our sample (e.g., double-majors, work
experience years, etc.), and so inferences drawn from the analysis must
be made with an eye to this reduced statistical power, and
quasi-experimental use of naturally occurring subject-variables as
independent variables of study. Lastly, there is a limitation present in
this study that is common to many studies of moral reasoning
development: Age is problematic as a variable. Age is at some level
inextricable from other variables that are typically of interest. It is
unlikely that one can gain more education or work experience without
having also gained more years of age. This confounding makes the
influence of age on moral reasoning difficult--if not impossible--to
statistically segregate.
RECOMMENDATIONS FOR FUTURE RESEARCH
It is important that future research be undertaken to explain
inconsistencies between prior findings. For example, this study found no
significant differences between the genders in moral reasoning
development. Prior studies have suggested a potential gender difference,
or possible interaction between gender and education whereby the
difference in the scores of the genders converge as level of education
increases (Branson, Edbrooke-Richardson, & Moe, 2007). Future
research may clarify these relationships. College major and type of work
performed may or may not systematically impact moral reasoning
development. The dynamics of college major and type of work need to be
more fully researched by utilizing larger cross-sectional samples from
larger pools of universities and programs, themselves from a greater
diversity of regions.
CONCLUSION
The results of this investigation of moral reasoning development
support our null hypotheses regarding the influence of age and gender on
moral development with no significant differences found between the
genders and no significant prediction of moral competence by age. This
result is inconsistent with the Branson Edbrooke-Richardson and Moe
(2007) study that concluded that gender does have an effect on moral
reasoning, but its impact diminished with increasing levels of
education.
Our hypothesis that level of education would significantly
influence moral reasoning development was not only not supported, but
the results--though nonsignificant--in fact point to a possible decline
in moral competence from undergraduate students to graduate students.
This is moderately disturbing, and conflicts with earlier findings that
found that graduate students compared favorably in DIT scores with
junior high and high school students (Rest, 1990). It is also
inconsistent with the Branson, Edbrooke-Richardson and Moe (2007) study
that found that level of education had more impact on moral reasoning
development than gender, age, work experience, or academic major.
Because major was not a component of prior analyses, this could be
construed as a paucity of moral reasoning development in graduate
accounting and business majors when compared with majors from other
fields of graduate study--though further research will be needed to
investigate this possibility. This would not be entirely out of line
with prior findings that although accounting undergraduates outpace
their liberal arts and business administration brethren in moral
reasoning development, they also lagged significantly behind psychology
major undergraduates (Bay & Greenburg, 2001).
The finding that accounting majors were significantly lower in
moral competence than double majors in accounting and business
administration is a suspect one due to the previously discussed
qualities of categorical cell size. It is presumable that this
difference is less likely due to some greater development on the part of
double majors than it is to the substantially greater variability of the
substantially larger accounting subsample versus the small subsample of
double majors.
It is both heartening and disheartening to find that professionals
working in the accounting fields did not differ significantly from those
in other business fields and those in unrelated fields. While one might
be tempted to weave a tale of no greater moral scrutiny on the part of
accountants than the common man, it should be remembered that absence of
evidence of difference is never a safe basis for such postulation.
The findings of the present study taken in total are congruent to
the larger body of results suggesting little if any change occurs as a
function of aging or pursuing study beyond the undergraduate level,
regardless of business subdiscipline. This "non-result" should
represent a red flag to educators and the public alike that not enough
is being done to cultivate the moral reasoning development of students
of business and accounting, especially between the undergraduate and
graduate levels. If the moral reasoning development of students is left
up to their own experiences and interpersonal encounters in the world,
according to Kohlberg's (1971) theories, they will rarely develop
beyond what their social and legal environment absolutely requires of
them. In a financial and business world with few true preventive
constraints on ethical decision making and reporting behaviors, this may
be a harbinger of more Enron's and WorldCom's to come unless
positive steps to enhance moral reasoning education are taken.
REFERENCES
AACSB (2010). Eligibility Procedures and Accreditation Standards
for Business Accreditation. Retrieved March 13, 2010 from
http://www.aacsb.edu/accreditation/AACSBSTANDARDS-2010-TRACKING.pdf
Barboza, D., & J. Schwartz (2002). "The Finance Wizard
Behind Enron's Deals". The New York Times, February 6, A1, C9.
Bay, D. D., & Greenberg, R. R. (2001). The relationship of the
DIT and behavior: A replication. Issues in Accounting Education, 16(3):
367-380.
Bouhmama, D. (2001). Relation of formal education to moral judgment
development. The Journal of Psychology, 122(2): 155-158.
Boddy, C.R., Leaders without ethics in global business: Corporate
psychopaths. Journal of Public Affairs, 10 (3): 121-138.
Branson, L, Edbrooke-Richardson, S., & Moe, B., (2007). Impact
of gender, age, work experience, and education on moral development:
Implications for business education. The Journal of Business and
Behavior Sciences, 15(2): 24-41.
Dawson, L. M. (1997). Ethical differences between men and women in
the sales profession. Journal of Business Ethics, 16, 1143-1152.
Dawson, T. L. (2003). A stage is a stage is a stage: A direct
comparison of two scoring systems. The Journal of Genetic Psychology,
164(3): 335-364.
Desplaces, D. E., Melcher, D. E., Beauvais, L. L., & Bosco, S.
M. (2007). The impact of business education on moral judgment
competence: An empirical study. Journal of Business Ethics, 74(1):
73-87.
Franke, G. R., Crown, D. F., & Spake, D. F. (1997). Gender
differences in ethical perceptions of business practices: A social role
theory perspective. Journal of Applied Psychology, 52(6): 920-934.
Frank, G., Ofobike, E., & Gradisher, S. (2010). Teaching
business ethics: A quandary for accounting educators. Journal of
Education for Business, 85, 132-138.
Gibbs, J. C., Karen B., & Grime, R. (2003). Moral judgment
maturity: From clinical to standard measures." Positive
Psychological Assessment: A Handbook of Models and Measures, Eds. S.
Lopez and C.R. Snyder. Washington D.C.: American Psychological
Association.
Gibbs, J. C., & Widaman, K. F. (1982). "Social
Intelligence: Measuring the Development of Socio-moral Reflection.
Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.
Jennings, M. M. (2004). Incorporating ethics and professionalism
into accounting education and research: A discussion of the voids and
advocacy for training in seminal works in business ethics. Issues in
Accounting Education, 19(1): 7-26.
King, P. M., & Mayhew, M. J. (2002). Moral judgment development
in higher education: Insights from the Defining Issues Test. Education,
31(3): 247-270.
Kohlberg, L. (1971). From Is to Ought: How to Commit the
Naturalistic Fallacy and Get Away with It in the Study of Moral
Development. New York: Academic Press.
Lind, G. (2010a). "What is the psychological and
methodological background of the MJT?" The Moral Judgment Test
Website. Retrieved March 13, 2010 from
http://www.uni-konstanz.de/ag-moral/mut/mjt-engl.htm#theory
Lind, G. (2010b). The meaning and measurement of moral judgment
competence revisited - A dual-aspect model. In: D. Fasko & W.
Willis, eds., Contemporary Philosophical and Psychological Perspectives
on Moral Development and Education. Cresskill. NJ: Hampton Press, pp.
185 - 220.
Lopez, Y., Rechner, P. L., & Olson-Buchanan, J. B. (2005).
Empirical assessment of the influence of business education, culture,
and demographic factors. Journal of Business Ethics, 60(4): 341-358.
Moore, B. N., & Bruder, K. (2002). Philosophy: The Power of
Ideas 5e. Boston: McGraw-Hill Higher Education.
Nisan, M., & Kohlberg, L. (1982). Universality and variation in
moral judgment: A longitudinal and cross-sectional study in Turkey.
Child Development, 53(4): 865-876.
Pettijohn, C., Pettijohn, L., & Taylor, A. (2008). Salesperson
perceptions of ethical behaviors: Their influence on job satisfaction
and turnover intentions. Journal of Business Ethics, 75(4): 547-557.
Piaget, J. (1932). The Moral Judgment of the Child. London: Kegan
Paul, Trench, Trubner and Co.
Persons, O. (2009) Using a corporate code of ethics to assess
students' ethicality: Implications for business education. Journal
of Education for Business. 84 (6): 357-366.
Rest, J. R. (1983). Morality. Eds. J. Flavell & E. Markman.
Handbook of Child Psychology Volume III: Cognitive Development 4th Ed.
NewYork: Wiley.
Rest, J. R. (1990). DIT Manual, 3rd Ed. Minnesota: Center for the
Study of Ethical Development.
Rest, J. R., Davison, M. L., & Robbins, S. (1978). Age trends
in judging moral issues: A review of cross-sectional, longitudinal, and
sequential studies of the Defining Issues Test. Child Development,
49(2): 263-279.
Rest, J. R., Thoma, S. J., Narvaez, D., & Bebeau, M. J. (1999).
Postconventional Moral Thinking: Neo-Kohlbergian Approach. New Jersey:
Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Schwartz, J. (2002). Darth Vader, Machiavelli, Skilling set intense
pace. The New York Times, February 7, 2002, C1, C7.
Schwartz, M. S., & Weber, J. (2006). A business ethics national
index (BENI): Measuring business ethics activity around the world.
Business & Society, 45(3): 382-405.
Trevino, L. K. (1992). Moral reasoning and business ethics:
Implications for research, education, and management. Journal of
Business Ethics, 11, 445-459.
Velasquez, M. G. (2006). Business Ethics: Concepts and Cases 6e.
Upper Saddle River, New Jersey: Pearson Education, Inc.
Windsor, J. C., & Cappel, J. J. (1999). A comparative study of
moral reasoning. College Student Journal, 33, 281-287.
Yang, C., & Grow, B. (2002). "What Did Ebbers Know?"
BusinessWeek, Retrieved March 13, 2010 from
http://www.businessweek.com/bwdaily/dnflash/jan2005/
nf20050114_5183_db016.htm
About the Authors:
Nathan Steele is Assistant Professor of Management at University of
Illinois Springfield. His research interests are in the area of the
impacts of social psychology on business practices.
Leonard Branson is Professor and Chair of Accountancy at the
University of Illinois Springfield. His research interests are in areas
related to behavioral accounting and curriculum development.
Rachel Martin is a master's graduate of University of Illinois
at Springfield who is currently working and living in Israel. Her
research interests focus on the importance of moral reasoning on
business practices.
Nathan L. Steele
Leonard Branson
Rachel C. Martin
University of Illinois Springfield
Table 1
Participants Demographics
Variable Category n Proportion
Age
20-26 years 67 54%
27-33 years 37 30%
34-40 years 8 6%
41-47 years 6 5%
48-54 years 5 4%
> 54 years 1 1%
Gender
Female 62 50%
Male 62 50%
Academic Major
Accounting 65 52%
Business 49 40%
Other 10 8%
Education Level
Undergraduate 47 38%
Graduate 77 62%
Job Type
Accounting 44 36%
Business 48 40%
Other 29 24%
Table 2
Moral Competence by Category
M p
Gender
Female / Male 9.77 / 12.61 0.10
Academic Major
Accounting / Business 10.01 / 11.42 0.43
Accounting / Other 10.01 / 18.06 0.02
Business / Other 11.42 / 18.06 0.05
Education Level
Undergraduate / Graduate 12.48 / 10.36 0.23
Job Type
Accounting / Business 11.43 / 11.48 1.00
Accounting / Other 11.43 / 10.30 0.60
Business / Other 11.48 / 10.30 0.59