首页    期刊浏览 2025年08月19日 星期二
登录注册

文章基本信息

  • 标题:Assessing the role of personality traits in student performance in traditional, hybrid and online classes.
  • 作者:Tidwell, Michael V. ; Southard, Sheryne ; Mooney, Mara
  • 期刊名称:International Journal of Education Research (IJER)
  • 印刷版ISSN:1932-8443
  • 出版年度:2010
  • 期号:June
  • 语种:English
  • 出版社:International Academy of Business and Public Administration Disciplines
  • 摘要:With the rate of online learning growing exponentially each year, virtually every university within the United States of America offers some type of online educational programming. A 2008 study performed by Allen and Seaman for the Sloan Consortium found that online learning grew over 100% between 2002 and 2007. Moreover, the study found that one-fifth of all higher education students are now taking at least one online course. This growth trend is expected to continue for numerous reasons. First, universities can expand their geographic reach with little effort. While many of the large universities recruit nationally or internationally, most universities recruit from a limited geographic region. Distance education provides universities with an opportunity to increase student access to their degree programs. Second, and related, online courses allow flexibility in students' schedules, thereby increasing enrollment. With the current economic environment many students find it necessary to work while going to school, making scheduling an increasingly central issue. Third, with limited construction budgets, the bricks and mortar model of education is more expensive to sustain. While some states, such as Georgia, are projecting significant growth in the college population (University System of Georgia, 2006) other states anticipate a period of moderate enrollment decline, which will intensify the competition among colleges to attract students (Redden, 2008). Offering online education makes it more economical to accommodate these students without the infrastructure investment. This affords universities the ability to adjust course offerings as demand fluctuates. Lastly, distance education allows universities to achieve greater economies of scale through increased enrollment, which leads to increased revenue, while more effectively containing costs.
  • 关键词:Academic achievement;Distance education;Educational psychology;Online education;Personality;Personality and academic achievement;Personality traits

Assessing the role of personality traits in student performance in traditional, hybrid and online classes.


Tidwell, Michael V. ; Southard, Sheryne ; Mooney, Mara 等


INTRODUCTION

With the rate of online learning growing exponentially each year, virtually every university within the United States of America offers some type of online educational programming. A 2008 study performed by Allen and Seaman for the Sloan Consortium found that online learning grew over 100% between 2002 and 2007. Moreover, the study found that one-fifth of all higher education students are now taking at least one online course. This growth trend is expected to continue for numerous reasons. First, universities can expand their geographic reach with little effort. While many of the large universities recruit nationally or internationally, most universities recruit from a limited geographic region. Distance education provides universities with an opportunity to increase student access to their degree programs. Second, and related, online courses allow flexibility in students' schedules, thereby increasing enrollment. With the current economic environment many students find it necessary to work while going to school, making scheduling an increasingly central issue. Third, with limited construction budgets, the bricks and mortar model of education is more expensive to sustain. While some states, such as Georgia, are projecting significant growth in the college population (University System of Georgia, 2006) other states anticipate a period of moderate enrollment decline, which will intensify the competition among colleges to attract students (Redden, 2008). Offering online education makes it more economical to accommodate these students without the infrastructure investment. This affords universities the ability to adjust course offerings as demand fluctuates. Lastly, distance education allows universities to achieve greater economies of scale through increased enrollment, which leads to increased revenue, while more effectively containing costs.

Given these reasons, increased enrollment in online education makes it important to understand student learning in an online environment versus a traditional environment. As dozens of studies have been performed in this area, two lines of research have developed. First, some studies have explored how individual differences impact student performance in online learning environments (e.g. Cheung & Kan, 2002; Didia & Hasnat, 1998; Wojciechowski & Palmer, 2005). The second line of research has examined the types of external factors most conducive to success in online learning programs (Hall, 2008; Liu, 2008). However, to date, few have explored the connections between these two streams of research. More specifically, none have examined whether individual differences are more central to performance in traditional, hybrid, or online classes. The current study fills this void.

This study seeks to accomplish three goals. First, it delineates whether personality traits like the Big 5 factors (neuroticism, extroversion, openness to experience, conscientiousness, and agreeableness) or intelligence are related to student performance. Second, it assesses whether any differences emerge in student performance based on the various course delivery methods. Lastly, it explores whether individual differences and course delivery method interact to influence student performance.

LITERATURE REVIEW

Personality is often conceptualized as one's enduring tendencies, moods, or temperamental makeup that are stable across situations and contexts (Daly & Bippus, 1998; Epstein & O'Brien, 1985; George, 1992). Though years of scholarship suggests its validity as an important factor in understanding human behavior (e.g. George, 1996) others have significant reservations often stemming from overly optimistic expectations regarding its predictive validity (Davis-Blake & Pfeffer, 1989). Specifically, some have argued that personality traits, not situational characteristics, are the central factors necessary to understanding human behavior. In reality, behavior is simultaneously affected by both (Epstein & O'Brien, 1985; George, 1996; Weaver, 1998).

As a result when personality is predicted to play a role in personal outcomes, most scholars limit its scope to very specific outcomes and provide a conceptual basis for the trait selection process (Beatty & McCroskey, 1998; Herold & Fedor, 1998; Weaver, 1998). When it comes to learning there are several traits that have historically helped us understand the learning process and outcomes.

Personality and Learning

Intelligence is the ability to learn, reason, and process information (Schmidt & Hunter, 2000). Over the years, researchers have examined the cognitive processes responsible for differences in intellect. For example, Carroll (1993) maintains that several different intellectual abilities exist and dozens are assessed within modern intelligence tests. Verbal abilities, mathematical skills, memory, information processing, and analytical skills are just a few of the factors being utilized within differential measures of intelligence. Given that most of these specific abilities tend to be intercorrelated, statistical analysis suggests a general mental ability referred to as general intelligence or g exists (Campbell & Catano, 2004; Spearman, 1927; Vigil-Colet & Codorniu-Raga, 2002).

G represents overarching commonality of these disparate processes and has consistently predicted learning and knowledge development (Hunter & Hunter, 1984; Jensen, 1998). According to Jensen (1998), the link between intelligence and learning exists "not because intelligence measures what is taught ... but because g is intrinsic to learning novel material, grasping concepts, distinctions, and meanings." (Section IX, 47). As a result, those with higher levels of intelligence tend to learn more and do so more quickly (Borman, White, Pulakos, & Oppler, 1991; Ree, Carretta, & Teach-out 1995; Schmidt & Hunter, 2000). "The relationship between general intelligence and the ability to acquire knowledge appears ubiquitous" (Brody, 1985, p. 360). That is, when considering learning within any type of environment, particularly a classroom environment, theory suggests intelligence will play an important role. Less certain however, is the link between the Big 5 personality traits and learning.

The five-factor model of personality (A.K.A. the Big 5) is based on the factor analysis of Allport and Odbert's (1936) pool of 17,953 personality adjectives (Piedmont, 1998). Over the years, the Big 5 has become one of the preeminent personality constructs because it measures traits that are stable across time and situations (Costa & McCrae, 1994; Piedmont, 1998). The Costa and McCrae Big 5 Model (1984) is represented by the five factors, and each factor is represented by six separate facets combining to generate its overarching domain (see Chart 1). As a result, the Big 5 has been predictive of a number of relevant individual outcomes, but only three have been linked to academic success and student learning (e.g. Maki & Maki, 2003).

Conscientiousness is sometimes described as the will to achieve (Smith, 1967). Those high in conscientiousness tend to show signs of dependability, thoroughness, and responsibility. However, recent classifications include more scholastic characteristics (Barrick & Mount, 1991; Costa & McCrae, 1988) such as hard work, achievement-orientation, responsibility, and perseverance. As the sub-traits indicate, those possessing this trait tend to consistently perform better on academic challenges. Specifically, a meta analysis by O'Connor and Paunonen (2007) found that conscientiousness is strongly and consistently associated with academic success across dozens of studies (e.g. Bauer & Liang, 2003; Busato, Prins, Elshout, & Haymaker, 2000; Conand, 2006). Indeed, the evidence supporting conscientiousness' relationship to academic success is clear. This relationship has developed over time because those who are more conscientious tend to possess reproduction and application directed learning styles which are more closely linked to higher test scores (Busato, Prins, Elshout, & Hamaker, 1998). In addition, the conscientious tend to take a deep approach to learning, meaning they think critically during the learning process, link new knowledge to old knowledge, associate disparate concepts, and create context between isolated content and life (Zhang, 2002). Unlike surface learning approaches, deep learning leads to greater long-term information retention.

Sometimes interpreted as culture (Hakel, 1974; Norman, 1963) or intellect (Peabody & Goldberg, 1989), openness to experience is the tendency for one to be curious and open-minded, pursue things high in aesthetic value, and be proactive in their interactions with others. It seems logical that high performance within academic settings would be a priority for those high in openness to experience, but this relationship has not always panned out. While there is substantial support for a positive relationship between openness to experience and academic performance (e.g. Farsides & Woodfield, 2003) others have found that no relationship exists (e.g. Diseth, 2003). For example, Dollinger and Orf (1991) found that openness to experience was related to exam grades but unrelated to essay or course grades. Similarly, Rothstein, Paunonen, Rush, & King (1994) found a weak relationship with classroom performance and GPA and no relationship with written performance grades. The negative and non-significant findings are perplexing given that those high in openness tend to engage in deep learning by gathering new and unique information (Zhang, 2002).

Given its link to sociability, extroversion has also been theorized to predict learning outcomes. Defined as one's tendency to be expressive or show initiative (Costa & McCrae, 1992) extroversion is often used to describe communicative phenomena. Those possessing these traits are very talkative and proactive in most of their interactions, spending a significantly larger portion of their time socializing than do introverts (Costa & McCrae, 1992). And since students more central within social networks tend to perform better academically (Russo & Koesten, 2005) it should follow that extroverts would have strong academic performance. This theorizing has found some evidentiary support (e.g. Rothstein, et al. 1994). However, while some scholars suggest links to academic performance tasks where initiative is critical, the results have been mixed at best. For example, Paunonen (1998) found no relationship between student performance and extroversion; similarly, Chamorro-Premuzic and Furnham (2003a, 2003b) found no relationship between extroversion and class performance or extroversion and thesis research. Of greater theoretical importance is that some scholars have found negative relationships. Goff and Ackerman's (1992) exploration generated a negative relationship with student performance while Bauer and Liang (2003) discovered that those higher in extroversion tended to have lower performance. Many scholars argue these negative associations imply that introverts spend their time studying while extroverts spend their time socializing (e.g. Chamorro-Premuzic & Furnham, 2005).

In short, outside of conscientiousness, the relationship between academic performance and personality traits is mixed and deserves greater attention. Given personality theory and previous findings, the following is hypothesized:

H1: Intelligence, extroversion, conscientiousness, and openness to experience will be positively related to classroom performance.

Since many of these findings have varied over the last several years, further exploration is important. However, this exploration should not occur within a vacuum. That is, education has changed dramatically over the last decade and future explorations should take those changes into account. With the changes in educational delivery, it is essential to explore academic performance in a more dynamic educational environment.

Course Delivery and Academic Performance

As noted earlier, many colleges and universities have provided students with several educational delivery format options. The two major types of online course delivery formats are pure online and hybrid. Pure online is defined as any course which is delivered 80% or more online, while a hybrid course typically ranges from 31% - 79% of the course being delivered online. Though traditional face-to-face delivery is still the most prevalent form of college course offering, online education is growing tremendously as professors develop detailed curriculums geared toward the online and hybrid environments. The growth in quantity and popularity of online education has caused scholars to question whether online course delivery formats yield better student performance.

The traditional face-to-face delivery method was initially thought to yield superior student performance because of student access to immediate feedback, professor-pupil interactivity, professor immediacy, and many other factors. However, many of these assumptions were based in anecdotal evidence, not empirical findings. As empirical findings were generated, unexpected results were found. For example, in one of the most comprehensive studies to date, Maki and Maki (2003), examined more than 300 online and 300 lecture students on a variety of independent and dependent variables. In short, they discovered that students in online classes performed better than their counterparts in traditional classes. This finding supports earlier studies suggesting that students in online classes perform better than those in traditional classes (e.g. Maki & Maki, 2002; Maki, Maki, Patterson, & Whitaker, 2000). However, Maki and Maki (2003) maintain that these differences are due to elegant course design, and not the delivery method. Their assertion may be accurate given the insignificant findings within other studies. For example, Sankaran, Sankaran, and Bui (2000) found no differences between delivery methods while Wang and Newlin (2000) discovered that students in traditional classes performed better than those in online classes. Given this disagreement within the literature, it is important to further examine this issue.

While there are many reasons why performance may or may not be different within traditional, hybrid, and online classes, examining student performance in light of student personality may expand our knowledge base regarding the differences in course delivery models. Given the historic role of personality in driving specific behavior, we expect that personality will play a differential role. The following research question is explored:

RQ1: What role does personality play in student performance in online, hybrid, and traditional classes?

METHODOLOGY

Sample and Data Collection

The potential subject pool was comprised of business, psychology, healthcare management, and legal studies majors from a four-year undergraduate southeastern university. These students were enrolled in courses representing one of three different types of delivery methods. The first class type was a traditional format, with less than 30% of the class meetings held on online. The second class type was a hybrid format, with 31%-79% of class meetings held online. The third class type was an online format, with 80% or more of the class meetings held online.

The researchers contacted the course instructors to request permission to enter the classroom to request voluntary student participation in the study. After receiving an overview and explanation of the study, students who wished to participate in the study signed a consent form and completed a survey instrument. Each respondent was assured confidentiality and each student completed the survey instrument in 20-30 minutes. Over the course of two consecutive semesters, 170 students were asked to participate in the study, and 133 completed the surveys (traditional classes, n=63; hybrid classes, n=43; online classes, n=27).

Measurement

Researchers collected objective data from each subject's electronic school records. These data included GPA, course grades, and class type. Subjects provided subjective data by completing portions of the Big-5 personality assessment test. Means, standard deviations, correlational, and regression data were computed for all variables. Analysis of variance was then computed to assess the difference in how students performed based upon each of their personality traits (e.g. intelligence or openness to experience) and class type. SPSS was used for all data analysis.

Independent/Class Variables

The personality assessment instrument used was Costa and McCrae's (1992) NEO Five Factor Inventory-Form S (NEO-Form S), an abbreviated 60-item version of the 240-item NEO Personality Inventory - Revised. The NEO-Form S was used to measure participant's self-reported levels of Conscientiousness, Extroversion, and Openness to Experience. It was constructed by selecting the first twelve items with the largest structure coefficients for each of the five factors. Respondents rated their answers in these areas on a 5-point Likert scale of measurement ranging from 1 = strongly disagree, 2 = disagree, 3 = neutral, 4 = agree, and 5 = strongly agree. Several statements were reverse keyed, and the individual items were summed then averaged to create an overall measure for that personality characteristic. Reliability coefficient alphas ranged from .80 to .95.

Extroversion

Extroversion is measured by a subject's level of warmth, gregariousness, assertiveness, activity, excitement seeking, and positive emotion. Respondents answered questions, such as, "I really enjoy talking to people," and "I laugh easily." After attaining each subject's extroversion score their score was categorized in one of five groups; very high, high, moderate, low, or very low. These rankings and categories were based upon norms developed within previous studies (see Costa & McCrae, 1992, 1994).

Openness

Openness is measured by a subject's level of fantasy, aesthetics, feelings, actions, ideas, and values. Respondents answered questions, such as, "I often try new and foreign foods," and "I am intrigued by the patterns I find in art and nature." After attaining each subject's openness score their score was categorized in one of five groups; very high, high, moderate, low, or very low. These rankings and caterogories were based upon norms developed within previous studies (see Costa & McCrae, 1992, 1994).

Conscientiousness

Conscientiousness is measured by a subject's level of competence, order, dutifulness, achievement striving, self-discipline, and deliberation. Respondents answered questions, such as, "I work hard to accomplish my goals," and "I keep my belongings neat and clean." After attaining each subject's conscientiousness score their score was categorized in one of five groups; very high, high, moderate, low, or very low. These rankings and caterogories were based upon norms developed within previous studies (see Costa & McCrae, 1992, 1994).

Intelligence

Intelligence is operationalized by each subject's comprehensive GPA. Similar to many popular general intelligence tests (e.g. Wonderlic, 2003), comprehensive GPA is comprised of various verbal and mathematical elements because students take dozens of courses from disparate fields of study. Like general intelligence tests, "academic success predictors (e.g. GPA) usually consist of cognitive measures, pertaining to mental ability or intelligence; and non-cognitive measures" (Ridgell & Lounsbury, 2004). GPA is categorized on a four point scale.

Course Delivery Format

As noted above, course delivery format was measured by segmenting the amount of face to face interaction between student and professor. Three categories were developed; traditional classes held less than 30% of the class meetings online, the hybrid classes held 31%-79% of class meetings online, and online classes held 80% or more of the class meetings online.

Dependent/Response Variable

Classroom Performance

Classroom performance was measured by the grade a student received in their traditional, hybrid, or online course. Grades were assigned a score on a scale from 0-4 with each class grade corresponding to a numerical score; F=0, D=1, C=2, B=3, A=4. Researchers collected this data from each subject's electronic school records.

THE RESULTS OF THIS STUDY

Hypothesis one was partially supported. Regression analysis confirms that intelligence ([r.sup.2]=.57, p<.001) predicted classroom performance (Table 2). While conscientiousness ([r.sup.2]=.07, ns),22 extroversion ([r.sup.2] =.09, ns), and openness to experience ([r.sup.2] =.04, ns) were positively related to classroom performance, they were non-significant.

While assessing the RQ, analysis of variance data suggests that there is a main effect for intelligence relative to performance over the three types of courses (F(1, 3)=4.28; p<.001) (Table 2). This linear relationship indicates that as intelligence increases so does student performance, regardless of type of delivery method.

Intelligence and conscientiousness has an interactive effective relative to student performance (F(1, 4)=2.335; p<.10). Specifically, student performance is dependent on levels of intelligence and conscientiousness. This relationship is non-linear.

There is an interaction between openness to new experience and class type (F(1, 2)=4.151; p<.05) and conscientiousness and class type (F(1, 4)=2.565; p<.05) relative to student performance. Surprisingly, students in hybrid classes outperformed students in other classes.

DISCUSSION

Personality and Classroom Performance. As expected, this study represents yet another confirmation that more intelligent students perform better than those lower in intelligence in classroom settings. Consistent with a substantial body of scholarship, regression analysis confirms that intellectual ability is positively related to classroom performance (e.g., Butaso, et al, 2000; Ridgell & Lounsbury, 2004). Intelligence "reflects a broader and deeper capability for comprehending our surroundings--"catching on," "making sense" of things, or "figuring out" what to do" (Gottfredson, 1997, p. 13). Since students in today's dynamic learning environments are confronted with multiple teaching methods (e.g. lectures, group work, PowerPoint presentations, etc) often within the same class and on the same day, an ability to quickly "catch on" or "figure things out" may enhance learning. These findings suggest this is true regardless of learning environment because intelligence helps students "get up to speed."

Though some scholars have found a positive relationship between openness to experience and classroom performance (Blickle, 1996; Eyong & Schniedergans, 2004, Lounsbury, et al., 2003), like other works, this study found no significant relationship (Busato, et al., 2000; Chamorro-Premuzic & Furnham, 2003). These varied results are consistent with findings in the O'Connor and Paunonen (2007) meta-analysis that the correlation between openness to new experience and classroom performance is tenuous at best. This study's results, and those of others, are likely mixed for two reasons. First, each study uses a different outcome measure. For example, this study, like others (e.g. Rothstein et al., 1994) uses the comprehensive measure of course grade while others use limited measures of performance, such as exam grades (e.g. Dollinger & Orf, 1991). Second, while high openness may initially stimulate subject matter intellectual curiosity it may not translate into higher performance in class or on specific class assignments.

Similarly, this data did not reveal a relationship between extroversion and classroom performance. This is consistent with studies by Chamorro-Premuzic and Furnham (2003a, 2003b) and Paunonen (1998). However, this contradicts some empirical findings (e.g. Eyong & Schniederjans, 2004; Rothstein et al., 1994) and social network theorizing in this area which notes that students more central within social networks tend to perform better academically (Russo & Koesten, 2005). Positive results were likely elusive because being gregarious and expressive, and maintaining a central position within one's social network does not necessarily lead to higher classroom performance.

Several researchers maintain that of the Big 5 five personality dimensions, conscientiousness has the strongest relationship with classroom performance (e.g., Busato, et al., 2000; Conard, 2006; O'Connor & Paunonen, 2007). Indeed, this theorizing is supported by the correlational finding that conscientious students outperform their counterparts across class settings. However, when regression analyses were performed, the contributions of conscientiousness became insignificant because of the role of intelligence. Specifically, when students are highly intelligent their levels of conscientiousness are less of a factor than when they are of low to moderate intelligence. Stated plainly, if one is highly intelligent his/her level of conscientiousness plays a diminished role because the student will still learn quicker and retain more than his/her counterparts. If one is moderately intelligent, s/he must maintain a strong work ethic to ensure peak performance. This is true across the class types studied herein. The practical implication of this finding is that highly intelligent students can be placed in virtually any setting and perform well. While conscientious students will still generally perform better than their counterparts in most settings, the differences are more pronounced when they are moderately intelligent.

Class Type, Personality, and Classroom Performance. Today's educational environment is dynamic and in constant flux. This study makes a value-added contribution to current learning and educational scholarship by exploring personality within the context of today's dynamic educational environment. Specifically, we explored how personality influences educational outcomes within the three major classroom settings. The most notable result is that the more intelligent and conscientious students within hybrid classes outperformed others in virtually every other scenario. That is, while students in hybrid classes did not necessarily perform better than their counterparts, when intelligence and conscientiousness were considered, significant differences in performance became apparent. As the first work to explore these elements within these contexts, this study makes two significant contributions to the literature.

First, students enrolled in hybrid classes who are high in conscientiousness outperformed their classmates in virtually every other class setting. That is, hybrid students that possess a will to achieve tended to outperform their classmates lower in these traits across all other class types. At first glance this finding was somewhat perplexing because one would expect that those who take initiative would perform better under the non-constraining conditions of an online class, as some scholars have suggested (e.g. Maki & Maki, 2003). This theorizing may be best explained by the level of social presence (e.g. immediacy) and flexibility present when taking a hybrid class.

Social presence is defined as the level of inclusiveness, connectedness, and immediacy a learner experiences (Tu & McIsaac, 2002). Students experience immediacy and inclusiveness when a professor reduces the psychological distance with the student by speaking directly to the student's needs and concerns. While students in traditional classes have greater opportunities to experience social presence because of the physical presence of the professor, hybrid students have similar opportunities because they meet with the professor on a regular basis. As a result, students who have been in physical contact with their professor and classmates may benefit from a level of connectedness not experienced by their online counterparts. Hybrid students also enjoy the flexibility online classes provide. Since these students have the ability to work at their own pace within the confines of the online course, they also have the opportunity to complete work at times conducive to their personal schedules, thereby avoiding the rigid time constraints of traditional classes. Coupling flexibility with the inclusiveness of physical contact can lead to improved outcomes.

Second, students enrolled in hybrid classes who are high in openness to experience outperformed their peers across class settings. Since a hybrid class represents the nexus between the traditional and online learning environment, students who are high in openness excel when presented with a continued variation of their learning experience offered in a hybrid setting. Hybrid classes create a more stimulating learning environment, thereby enhancing deeper learning which is a driving force to the academic performance of students open to new experiences.

CONCLUSIONS

Personality traits play an increasingly central role in understanding how students interact with the changing dynamics of today's learning environments. Since students are expected to operate under multiple teaching methods they must possess the traits necessary to deal with the variety they are confronted with on a daily basis. The current work explicates these elements. As expected, highly intelligent students performed better than others across all class types. More importantly, however, the current study differentiates the roles of intelligence, conscientiousness, and openness to experience within these class settings. Specifically, the hybrid classroom format creates a learning environment that is conducive to learning for students with certain traits because it incorporates the best features of the online and traditional classes; namely flexibility and professor immediacy.

IMPLICATIONS AND FUTURE DIRECTIONS

This study's finding regarding intelligence is consistent with the literature noting intelligence is the most significant correlate of classroom performance. While conscientiousness was also found to be an indicator of classroom performance, the inclusion of intelligence in the model greatly reduced its significance. The additional findings in this study regarding the relatively insignificant impact of a student's openness to new experience and level of extroversion on classroom performance are consistent with much of the literature, although external factors, such as outcome measures, has historically produced varied results. Further exploration in this area might benefit from research models incorporating students from a wider array of disciplines and examining additional personality traits, such as self-discipline.

This study also builds upon the literature by contributing an additional finding that students who are high in conscientiousness and enrolled in hybrid classes outperformed their peers across class types, and students high in openness to experience also outperformed their peers across class settings. These findings provide valuable insight for administrators faced with the challenge of creating post-secondary curricula in an effort to balance the immediacy of traditional courses with the flexibility offered by distance education. Based on these findings, further research is recommended to explore the unique dynamics of hybrid classes and the way in which this alternative delivery format can be most effectively utilized in higher education.

LIMITATIONS

This study's results are limited in two ways. First, while studying one organization is not uncommon (e.g. Busato, et al., 2000), these results may not always generalize beyond the type of university studied or the region in which it was studied. Future works should explore various types of universities (e.g. research oriented schools or teaching-oriented schools) in different geographic regions to assess the broader implications of the current work's findings. Second, while the sample size provided adequate power given the number of factors studied (e.g. Bayram, Deniz & Erdogan, 2008; Offir, Bezalel & Barth, 2007), a larger sample would offer a broader base from which to assess the theory. The marginal sample size led to several missing cells (see Table 3 for missing cells and DF analysis) in the ANOVA analysis. When assessing variables like intelligence or conscientiousness within a university environment, it's common to have the majority of a university population score high because it takes moderate to high levels of intelligence and conscientiousness to qualify for admission into most universities. Therefore, no subjects scored in the lowest categories of these measures. This may be one cause for missing cells within these variables. As a result, the breadth of the dataset could have been enhanced by a larger sample size. Future studies should gather data from a more sizable pool of participants.

REFERENCES

Allen, I., & Seaman, J. (2008). Staying the course: Online education in the United States. Retrieved from http://www.sloan-c.org/publications/survey/pdf/staying_the_course.pdf.

Allport, G.W., & Odbert, H.S. (1936). Trait names: A psycho-lexical study. Psychological Monographs, 47 (1, Whole No. 211).

Barrick, M., & Mount, M. (1991). The big five personality dimensions and job performance: A metaanalysis. Personnel Psychology, 44, 1-26.

Bauer, K.W., & Liang, Q. (2003). The effecdt of personality and precollege characteristics on firstyear activities performance. Journal of College Student Development, 44, 277-290.

Bayram, S., Deniz, L., & Erdogan, Y. (2008). The role of personality traits in web based education. The Turkish Online Journal of Educational Technology, 7 (2): Retrieved from http://www.tojet.net/articles/725.htm.

Beatty, M.J., & McCroskey, J. C. (1998). Interpersonal communication as temperamental expression: A commmunibiological paradigm. In J.C. McCroskey, J.A. Daly, M.M. Martin, and M.J. Beatty (Eds.), Communication and Personality: Trait Perspectives (pp. 41-68). Cresskill, N.J.: Hampton Press.

Blickle, G. (1996). Personality traits, learning strategies and performance. European Journal of Personality, 10, 337-352.

Borman W. C., White, L. A., Pulakos E. D., & Oppler, S. H. (1991). Models of Supervisory Job Performance Ratings. Journal of Applied Psychology, 76, 863-872.

Busato, V.V., Prins, F.J., Elshout, J.J., & Hamaker, C. (1998). The relation between learning styles, the Big Five personality traits and achievement motivation in higher education, Personality and Individual Differences, 26 (1): 129-140.

Busato, V.V., Prins, F.J., Elshout, J.J., & Haymaker, C. (2000). Intellectual ability, learning style, personality, achievement motivation and academic success of psychology students in higher education. Personality and Individual Differences, 29, 1057-1068.

Campbell, S., & Catano, V. M. (2004). Using Measures of Specific Abilities to Predict Performance in the Canadian Forces Operator Occupations. Military Psychology, 16(3): 193-201.

Carroll, J.B. (1993). Human Cognitive Abilities: A Survey of Factor Analytic Studies. Cambridge, U.K.: Cambridge University Press.

Chamorro-Premuzic, T., & Furnham, A. (2003a). Personality traits and academic examination performance. European Journal of Personality, 17, 237-250.

Chamorro-Premuzic, T., & Furnham, A. (2003b). Personality predicts academic performance: Evidence from two longitudinal university samples. Journal of Research in Personality, 37, 319-338.

Chamorro-Premuzic, T., & Furnham, A. (2005). Personality and intellectual competence. New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

Cheung, L., & Kan, A. (2002). Evaluation of factors related to student performance in a distancelearning business communication course. Journal of Education for Business, 77 (5): 257-263.

Conard, M.A. (2006). Aptitude is not enough: How personality and behavior predict academic performance, Journal of Research in Personality, 40, 339-346.

Costa, P.T., & McCrae, R.R. (1980). Influence of extraversion and neuroticism on subjective wellbeing: Happy and unhappy people. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 38, 668378.

Costa, P.T., & McCrae, R. R. (1992). Reviews NEO personality inventory and NEO five-factor inventory professional manual. Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources.

Costa, P.T., & McCrae, R.R. (1994). "Set like plaster"? Evidence for the stability of adult personality. In T.F. Heatherton and J.L. Weinberger (Eds.), Can personality change? Washington, D.C.: American Psychological Association.

Daly, J.A., & Bippus, A.M. (1998). Personality and Interpersonal Communication: Issues and Directions. In J.C. McCroskey, J.A. Daly, M.M. Martin, and M.J. Beatty (Eds.), Communication and Personality: Trait Perspectives (pp. 1-40), Cresskill, N.J.: Hampton Press.

Davis-Blake, A., & Pfeffer, J. (1989). Just a mirage: The search for dispositional effects in organizational research. Academy of Management Review. 14: 385-400.

DePaulo, B.M. (1993). The ability to judge others from their expressive behaviors. Fifty Years of Personality Psychology. (K.H. Craik, Ed.). New York: Plenum Press.

Didia, D., & Hasnat, B. (1998). The determinants of performance in the university introductory finance course. Financial Practice and Education, 8 (1), 102-107.

Diseth, A. (2003). Personality and approaches to learning as predictors of academic achievement. European Journal of Personality, 17, 143-155.

Dollinger, S. J., & Orf, L. A. (1991). Personality and performance in "personality": Conscientiousness and Openness. Journal of Research in Personality, 25, 276-284.

Dutton, J., Dutton, M., & Perry, J. (2002). How do online students differ from lecture students? Journal of Asynchronous Learning Networks, 6 (1): Retrieved from http://www.alnweb.org/alnweb/journal/Vol6_issue1/ 6dutton.htm.

Epstein, S., & O'Brien, E.J. (1985). The person-situation debate in historical and current perspective. Psychological Bulletin, 98, 513-537.

Eyong, B.K., & Schniedergans, M.J. (2004). The role of personality in web-based distance education courses. Communications of the ACM, 47 (3): 95-98.

Farsides, T., & Woodfield, R. (2002). Individual differences and undergraduate academic success: the roles of personality, intelligence, and application. Personality and Individual Differences, Personality and Individual Differences, 34, 1225-1243.

George, J.M. (1992). The role of personality in organizational life: Issues and evidence. Journal of Management, 18 (2).

George, J.M. (1996). Trait and State Affect. In K. Murphy (ed.) Individual Differences and Behavior in Organizations. Jossey-Bass.

Hair, P., & Hampson, S.E. (2006). The role of impulsivity in predicting maladaptive behaviour among female students. Personality and Individual Differences, 40, 943-952.

Hakel, M. D. (1974). Normative personality factors recovered from ratings of personality descriptors: The beholder's eye. Personnel Psychology, 27, 409-421.

Hall, M. (2008). Predicting student performance in web-based distance education courses based on survey instruments measuring personality traits and technical skills. Online Journal of Distance Learning Administration, 11 (3). Retrieved from http://www.westga.edu/~distance/ojdla/fall113/hall113.html.

Halsne, A., & Gatta, L. (2002). Online versus traditionally-delivered instruction: A descriptive study of learner characteristics in a community college setting. Online Journal of Distance Learning Administration, 5 (1). Retrieved from

http://www.westga.edu/~distance/ojdla/spring51/halsne51.html.

Herold, D.M., & Parsons, C.K. (1985). Assessing the feedback environment in work organizations: Development of the job feedback survey. Journal of Applied Psychology, 70.

Hunter, J. E., & Hunter, R. F. (1984). Validity and Utility of Alternative Predictors of Job Performance. Psychological Bulletin, 761, 72-93.

Jensen, A.R. (1998). The g Factor: The Science of Mental Ability. Wesport, CT: Praeger.

Liu, S. (2008). Student interaction experiences in distance learning courses: A phenomenological study. Online Journal of Distance Learning Administration, XI (I).

Lounsbury, J.W., Sundstrum, E., Loveland, J.M., & Gibson, L.W. (2003). Intelligence, "big five" personality traits, and work drive as predictors of course grade. Personality and Individual Differences, 35, 1231-1239.

Maki, R.H., Maki, W.S., Patterson, M., & Whittaker, P.D. (2000). Evaluation of a Web-based

introductory psychology course: learning and satisfaction in online versus lecture courses. Behavior Research Methods, Instrument, & Computers, 32(2): 230-239.

Maki, W. S., & Maki, R. H. (2002). Multimedia comprehension skill predicts differential outcomes of web-based and lecture courses. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 8, 85-98.

Maki, R.H.& Maki, W.S. (2003). Prediction of learning and satisfaction in web-based and lecture courses. Journal of Educational Computing Research, 28(3): 197-219.

Norman, W. T. (1963). Toward an adequate taxonomy of personality attributes: Replicated factor structure in peer nomination personality ratings. Journal of Abnormal Social Psychology, 66, 574-583.

O'Connor, M.C., & Paunonen, S.V. (2007). Big five personality predictors of post-secondary academic performance. Personality and Individual Differences, 43, 971-990.

Offir, B., Bezalel, R., & Barth, I. (2007). Introverts, extroverts, and achievement in a distance learning environment, American Journal of Distance Education, 21 (1): 3-19.

Paunonen, S.V. (1998). Hierarchical organization of personality and prediction of behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 538, 556.

Peabody, D., & Goldberg, L. R. (1989). Some determinants of factor structures from personality-trait descriptors. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 57, 552-567.

Piedmont, R.L. (1998). The revised NEO Personality Inventory: Clinical and research applications. New York: Plenum Press.

Redden, E. (2008). Demographic Boom and Bust. Retrieved from www.insidehighered.com/news/2008/03/20/projections.

Ree, M. J., & Carretta, T. R. (1995). Group Differences in Aptitude Factor Structure on the ASVAB. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 55, 268-277.

Ridgell, S.D., & Lounsbury, J.W. (2004). Predicting academic success: general intelligence, "big five" personality traits and work drive. College Student Journal, 38 (4), 607-619.

Russo, T.C., & Koesten, J. (2005). Prestige, centrality and learning: A social network analysis of an online class. Communication Education, 54(3), 254-261.

Sankaran, S. R., Sankaran, D., & Bui, T. X. (2000). Effect of student attitude to course format on learning performance: An empirical study in web vs. lecture instruction. Journal of Instructional Technology, 27, 66-73. Wang, A. Y., & Newlin, M. H. (2000). Characteristics of students who enroll and succeed in web-based psychology classes. Journal of Educational Psychology, 92, 137-143.

Schmidt, F.L., & Hunter, J.E. (2000). Select on Intelligence. In E.A. Locke (Ed), Handbook of Principles of Organizational Behavior, 3-14. Blackwell Business Publishing.

Smith, G. M. (1967). Usefulness of peer ratings of personality in educational research. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 27, 967-984.

Spearman, C. (1927). The Abilities of Man. London: MacMillan. Tu, C. H., & McIsaac, M. S. (2002). The relationship of social presence and interaction in online classes. The American Journal of Distance Education, 16(3): 131-150.

University System of Georgia Office of Research and Policy Analysis (2006). Retrieved from www.usg.edu/research/students/enroll/trends2018.pdf.

Vigil-Colet, A., & Codorniu-Raga, M. J. (2002). How Inspection Time and Paper and Pencil Measures of Processing Speed are Related to Intelligence. Personality and Individual Differences, 337, 1149-1161.

Weaver III, J.B. (1998). Personality and self-perceptions about communication. In J.C. McCroskey, J.A. Daly, M.M. Martin, and M.J. Beatty (Eds.), Communication and Personality: Trait Perspectives (pp. 95-118). Cresskill, N.J.: Hampton Press.

Wojciechowski A., & Palmer, L.B. (2005). Individual student characteristics: Can any be predictors of success in online classes? Online Journal of Distance Learning Administration,8 (2). Retrieved from http://www.westga.edu/~distance/ojdla/summer82/wojciechowski82.htm.

Wonderlic Personnel Test, Inc. (2003). Wonderlic Personnel Test: User's manual. Libertyville, IL: Wonderlic Personnel Test, Inc.

Zhang, L. (2002). Does the big five predict learning approaches? Personality and Individual Differences, 34, 1431-1446.

About the Authors:

Michael V. Tidwell is Dean of the College of Business at Bloomsburg University of Pennsylvania. He completed his doctoral degree at Washington State University and has published in several scholarly journals including the International Journal of Management Theory and Practices, the Journal of Business Communication, and the journal of Nonprofit Management and Leadership. In addition to speaking at numerous national and international conferences, he has spent time working in Corporate Communications within the high tech industry and traveling, teaching, and/or consulting in Europe, Middle-East, India, S. Africa, and Kenya.

Sheryne Southard earned her Juris Doctor from the Sandra Day O'Connor College of Law at Arizona State University and a Bachelor of Science in Business Management from the University of Nevada. She has extensive experience in pure online and hybrid instruction and the development of strategies to improve student success and engagement in alternative delivery courses. She serves as the faculty online coordinator. In 2009, Dr. Southard was recognized as the Grand Award Winner of the Pearson-Prentice Hall Online Teaching Competition.

Mara Mooney is a graduate of Lafayette College in Easton, PA (B.A., cum laude, Government & Law; Phi Beta Kappa) and Emory University School of Law in Atlanta, GA (Notes & Comments Editor, Emory Law Journal; Dean's Fellow in Legal Research and Writing). She is the author of the article, "The Nutrition Labeling and Education Act of 1996: A Proposal for a Less-Restrictive Scientific Standard," which was published in the Emory Law Journal and noted as "worth reading" in the National Law Journal, and the textbook, Fundamentals of Georgia Real Estate Law, published by Carolina Academic Press. Prior to joining the faculty at Clayton State University, she practiced law with the multinational firm, Alston & Bird, LLP, in Atlanta, Georgia, and taught at two ABA- approved legal assistant studies programs. At Clayton State University, Ms. Mooney teaches traditional, hybrid, and online courses in ethics, contracts, torts, legal research & writing, and real property law.

Michael V. Tidwell

Bloomsburg University of Pennsylvania

Sheryne Southard

Mara Mooney

Clayton State University
Table 1
Correlational Data

                               Course
                Intelligence   Grade    Neuroticism   Extroversion

Class Type      0.00           -0.07    -0.02         -0.03
Intelligence                   0.60     -0.21         -0.01
Course Grade                            -0.12         0.08
Neuroticism                                           -0.27
Extroversion
Openness
Agreeableness

                Openness   Agreeableness   Conscientiousness

Class Type      0.21       -0.03           -0.05
Intelligence    0.09       0.03            0.19
Course Grade    0.08       -0.04           0.17
Neuroticism     -0.08      -0.26           -0.27
Extroversion    0.16       0.14            0.12
Openness                   0.01            0.00
Agreeableness                              0.26

Bold and italicized: Correlation is significant at the 0.01 level
(1-tailed).
Bold: Correlation is significant at the 0.05 level (1-tailed).

Table 2
Results of Regression Analysis

                     Un standardized                Standardized

Model                B                 Std. Error   Beta

(Constant)           0.82              1.03
Class type           -0.08             0.08         -0.07
tnteffigence         0.60              0.08         0.57
Neuroticism          0.05              0.09         0.04
Extroversion         0.11              0.09         0.09
Ooenness             0.06              0.11         0.04
Agreeableness        -0.11             0.12         -0.07
Conscientio.isness   0.09              0.10         0.07

Model                t

(Constant)           0.80
Class type           -0.99
tnteffigence         7.59
Neuroticism          0.49
Extroversion         1.20
Ooenness             0.51
Agreeableness        -0.94
Conscientio.isness   0.94

Bold and Italics: Correlation is significant at the 0.01 level
(1-tailed).
Bold: Correlation is significant at the 0.05 level (1-tailed].

Table 3
Results of ANOVA for Course Grades

                                        Sums of         Missing
Source                                  Squares   df    Cells

Corrected Model                         82.30     85
Intercept                               296.95    1
Class Type                              0.92      2     0
Intelligence                            12.36     3     0
Extroversion                            0.10      2     2
Conscientiousness                       1.13      3     1
Openness to Experience                  1.01      3     1
Class Type * Intelligence               0.90      4     0
Class Type * Extroversion               2.55      4     0
Class Type Openness to Experience       3.93      2     4
Class Type * Conscientiousness          4.86      4     2
Intelligence * Extroversion             074       3     0
Intelligence * Openness to Experience   0.64      2     3
Intelligence * Conscientiousness        4.42      4     3
Extroversion * Conscientiousness        0.83      4     2
Extroversion * Openness to Experience   1.81      2     1
Conscientiousness * Openness to         0.20      1     5
  Experience
Error                                   21 78     46
Total                                   1359      132
Corrected Total                         104.08    131

Table 4
Marginal Means

Groups

               Conscien-
Intelligence   tiousness   Mean    Std. Error

1              2           a.
               3           1       0638
               4           2       0231
               5           2       0.437
2              2           a.
               3           2.778   0.229
               4           2.411   0 149
               5           3 214   0 231
3              2           a
               3           3.167   0 269
               4           3 325   0 161
               5           3.137   0.177
4              2           4       0688
               3           4       0688
               4           3.9     0.275
               5           3.722   0.191

Groups           Groups

Intelligence     Class Type   Openness   Mean    Std. Error

1                1            2          2       0 487
                              3          2       0.397
                              4          3.112   0.131
                              5          3.133   0.162
2                2            2          a.
                              3          a.
                              4          3 104   0 171
                              5          3 385   0 167
3                3            2          a
                              3          a.
                              4          2 318   0 193
                              5          2 733   0.205
4

Groups           Groups

                              Conscien-
Intelligence     Class Type   tiousness   Mean    Std Error

1                1            2           4       0.683
                              3           3       0.281
                              4           2.763   0.142
                              5           3.167   0.154
2                2            2           a.
                              3           2 833   0.269
                              4           3 167   0.166
                              5           3 75    0.22
3                3            2           a.
                              3           28      0 303
                              4           2 479   0 216
                              5           3.062   0.223
4

a This level combination of factors is not observed, thus the
corresponding population marginal mean is not estimable.
联系我们|关于我们|网站声明
国家哲学社会科学文献中心版权所有