Young learners: the home literacy environments of Australian four-year-olds.
Brown, P. Margaret ; Byrnes, Linda J. ; Raban, Bridie 等
This study investigated the home literacy environments (HLEs) of
4-year-old children attending an early childhood program prior to school
entry and the association between the HLE and children's interests
in and attitudes to literacy. One hundred and thirty-eight parents and
140 children participated in the study. Parents completed a
questionnaire and children were interviewed. Results showed that,
generally, these parents created "somewhat rich" literacy
environments, and that traditional literacy (as opposed to
techno-literacy) materials predominated. Frequency of the parents'
own reading was positively associated with the frequency with which
parents read to their children. Richness of parents' reading habits
was positively associated with the frequency with which parents read to
their children, but not necessarily associated with the children's
own interests in or orientation to literacy.
Keywords: parent and child, child voice, parent involvement,
literacy
**********
From birth, children are immersed in a world full of objects,
happenings, and people, and through their interactions with and within
their worlds, they seek to uncover meanings. The driving force behind
this immersion is the child becoming part of the cultural group. In
doing so, children seek to learn the group's customs and rituals
and acquire skills so that they can operate in the group (Rogoff, 2003;
Vygotsky, 1978). All cultural groups share meanings that are conveyed
not only through behavior but also, in the case of human groups, almost
exclusively through the use of symbols that best express those meanings.
There are many symbolic forms that children notice and seek to
understand. Of these, the spoken and written forms of language are the
most critical for communication within and across groups.
Family Literacy Practices
Young children are supported in their acquisition of language by
family members. Family members communicate with infants in ways that
help children to "break the code." Family members capitalize
on children's interest in the written word and scaffold their
learning (Whitmore, Martens, Goodman, & Owocki, 2004). It is now
widely held that it is within the family context that most children
acquire an early understanding of literacy before they reach school age
(Weinberger, 1996). All families have literacy practices (see, e.g.,
Auerbach, 1989; Feiler & Webster, 1998; Whitmore et al., 2004) and
seek to imbue within their children those they most value. These
practices are transmitted not just by parents, but also by siblings,
grandparents, and extended family members and friends (Gregory, 2001)
and are typically enmeshed within the daily lives of the family
(Auerbach, 1989). Cambourne (1988) further suggested that seven
conditions underpin young children's literacy development: (1)
immersion in written language, (2) demonstration and modeling of
literate behavior, (3) expectations that children will become literate,
(4) children taking responsibility for their own learning, (5) parents
accepting approximations of children's literacy attempts, (6)
employing literacy in their everyday lives, and (7) receiving positive
feedback about their attempts. Given the uniqueness of the daily lives
of families, these practices and conditions are not necessarily similar
or standard across all families.
What is standard, however, is that each family's practices aim
to support the child's literacy development (Auerbach, 1989). They
are purposeful and reflect the family's literacy needs (Whitmore et
al., 2004). The literacy medium used in families is not always the same.
Meaning may be created and expressed through print and other symbol
systems. For example, some families routinely use literacy activities,
such as story book reading, singing rhymes, and storytelling. These
practices are usually referred to as traditional literacy activities.
Other families tend to favor activities that support their everyday
activities and, as Saracho (2007) noted, help children to understand the
world in which they live. Examples of these may include logos, shop and
toy catalogs, bus timetables, fast-food outlet menus, and television
programs. These practices are often referred to as environmental
literacy activities. Other families have embraced new technologies, such
as computers and mobile phones, that combine print-based materials and
expressive and receptive devices, such as image processing, hypermedia
systems, and speech recognition programs (Pirrie, 1999). This type of
literacy has been described by Marsh (2004) as
"technoliteracy." Families do not necessarily use only one
type of literacy practice. Most families are more likely to use all
three modes of literacy to varying degrees.
A number of studies have investigated particular aspects of family
literacy practices and their links with young children's later
literacy achievement. Most of these studies have investigated
traditional literacy practices, such as story book reading. For example,
Weigel, Martin, and Bennett (2006) collected data from 85 U.S. parents
and their preschool children and found indicators of positive
associations between parental literacy habits and parental reading
beliefs, parental reading beliefs and parent-child literacy and language
activities in the home, and parent-child literacy activities and
children's print knowledge and interest in reading. Harris, Loyo,
Holahan, Suzuki, and Gottlieb (2007) obtained similar results in their
study of 600 mothers. They used structural equation modeling to predict
reading ability in young children and found a positive association
between access to books and affirmative attitudes about the benefits to
be gained through reading to children.
In a longitudinal study of 168 middle- and upper-class
English-speaking children in Canada, Senechal and LeFevre (2002) also
found a strong benefit for children who had access to books, reporting
an association between children's experience with books and their
development of vocabulary and listening comprehension skills and, in
turn, their reading ability, in Grade 3.
In addition, they found a relationship between parents consciously
teaching children about reading and writing words, and the development
of early literacy skills. In another U.K. study, Weinberger (1996)
investigated predictors of literacy in 42 children and found significant
factors at entry into school associated with literacy outcomes at age 7
(as defined by the level of book being read): (1) having a favorite book
at age 3, (2) being read to by parents at least once each day, (3) being
read story books, (4) knowing nursery rhymes, (5) having access to
library books, (6) looking at books on their own at nursery school, (7)
knowing the names of some of the letters of the alphabet, (8) being able
to write their first name, and (9) copying a paragraph of text.
Roberts, Jurgens, and Burchinal (2005) attempted to find an
association between (1) four specific measures of family literacy
practices (the frequency of shared book reading, maternal book reading
strategies, children's enjoyment of reading and maternal
sensitivity); (2) a global measure of the quality and responsiveness of
the home environment; and (3) children's early literacy skills
between ages 3 and 5. The participant group consisted of 72 African
American children and their mothers/carers. Although some significant
associations were found with individual measures, the global measure of
quality and responsiveness, rather than discrete practices, was found to
be the strongest predictor of children's early literacy skills.
These studies all support the importance of preschool
children's access to books. Not surprisingly, then, in terms of
particular practices undertaken by parents, the impact of story book
reading on later literacy outcomes has been closely scrutinized. For
example, Bus, van Ijzendoorn, and Pellegrini (1995) undertook a
quantitative meta-analysis of studies investigating the frequency with
which books are read to preschoolers. They found a relationship between
parents reading to preschoolers and language growth, early literacy, and
reading achievement. They also noted a particular link between book
reading and the development of written language. Contrary to results
from a number of studies published subsequently (e.g., Natsiopoulou,
Souliotis, Kyridis, & Hatzisavvides, 2006; Weinberger, 1996), Bus et
al. found that family socioeconomic status did not have an impact on the
effect of book reading.
Parental Beliefs
Other researchers have examined connections between mothers'
literacy beliefs and preschool children's literacy development.
Weigel et al. (2006), for example, collected data from 79 mothers and
found two quite distinct views of literacy learning. One group, known as
"facilitative" mothers, actively engaged their children in
learning through providing opportunities for growth in vocabulary,
knowledge, and morals. In contrast, "conventional" mothers
believed more strongly that it was the role of the school to educate
their children and so provided fewer growth opportunities for their
child. Essentially, then, depending on their beliefs about the role of
adults (parents and teachers) in children's literacy acquisition,
each group of mothers engaged in different literacy activities with
their children, with the children of facilitative mothers typically
growing up in homes that were more literacy enriching. Weigel and
colleagues found that this had implications for later outcomes, with the
children of facilitative mothers being more interested in reading, and
more knowledgeable about print.
In contrast, few research studies have investigated environmental
or "techno-literacy" family literacy practices. One example of
techno-literacy practices comes from the work of Marsh and Thompson
(2001), who, in a U.K. study of 18 families and their 3- and 4-year-old
children, found that much of the children' s reading concentrated
on popular cultural and media texts.
The above studies seem to imply at least two things. First, family
literacy practices and parental beliefs are associated with particular
literacy outcomes for children. That is, different environments may lend
more or less support to a child's developing literacy, and this can
occur through intentional parental support and also though simple
exposure and exemplars. However, this work has focused primarily on
traditional forms of literacy. Contemporary home literacy practices may
now include environmental and techno-literacy activities, and the impact
of these on children's development has yet to be fully explored.
Given this, it is possible that these newer forms of literacy enrich the
home literacy environment. Two major questions can then be asked: Is it
possible to define and measure the richness of families' literacy
environments? And, in what ways might the relative richness of the home
literacy environment influence the child's interests and attitudes
toward literacy?
METHOD
Participants
The study reported here comprises data collected from the first of
three cohorts of parents and children in the Young Learners'
Project (funded by the Australian Scholarships Group and the Australian
Research Council Project No. LP LP0883437). The larger study follows
children from 4-year-old preschool into the first year of formal
schooling. The participants in Cohort 1 were 138 parents and 140
children (there were two sets of twins, so 140 questionnaires were
completed by the parents and 140 interviews undertaken with the
children). The children were attending 4-year-old preschool in the
Melbourne metropolitan area in the state of Victoria, Australia.
Overall, the child sample included children in 15 rooms in six different
preschool centers. Ages of the children at the commencement of the study
ranged between 3 years, 7 months and 5 years, 5 months. This range of
ages, though somewhat wide, is common in Victorian 4-year-old preschool
programs. The younger children will turn 4 during the year, and others
may possibly repeat the preschool year before going to school. There
were 69 girls and 71 boys in the sample. The parents completing the
questionnaires were 127 mothers and 11 fathers. Ninety-five percent of
the parents lived with a spouse or partner; 82% were born in Australia;
and English was the main language spoken in 95% of the homes.
Seventy-eight percent of these parents had a university, or equivalent,
qualification. Eighty-six percent of the children lived with their
siblings, and 50% were the first-born child.
Data Collection
The data collection took place in the first 6 months of the
preschool year. Parents completed a questionnaire about their views,
attitudes, and practices toward literacy to provide a description of
their home literacy environments. The questionnaire had three sections,
which focused on parents' own literacy habits and preferences,
their literacy habits with their child, and their child's own
independent literacy habits and preferences. Another section focused on
the parents' connections with their child's educational
program. The data from the first three sections are reported here. In
terms of the parents' own literacy habits, they were asked about
(1) the main language in which they read; (2) the frequency (daily,
several times a week, once a week, occasionally, never) and duration (20
minutes or less, 21 to 40 minutes, 41 to 60 minutes, more than 60
minutes) of their reading; and (3) the types of materials they read
(traditional, environmental, or new technology--defined below). A
similar set of questions was asked in relation to their reading with
their child and their child's independent reading.
The children were interviewed (using a schedule named the ORIENT,
which was developed specifically for this project to measure young
children's orientation to literacy [Byrnes & Brown, 2007]) by
one of the researchers at the center they attended and at a time
arranged with their preschool teacher to avoid disruption to the
program. In the ORIENT, children were presented with four exemplars of
each of the above types of reading materials (traditional [a story book,
a nonfiction book, a birthday card, and a young children's
magazine], environmental [a McDonald's menu, a ToysRUs catalog, a
bus timetable, and a poster of a popular child's movie: Cars], and
new technology [a mobile telephone and a computer]). To match the
parents' questions, children were asked if they liked to send or
receive texts, play games on the computer, look up information on the
computer (e.g., look at the website of a toy store), or share
information with others (e.g., send e-mails); and asked which of them
they liked to use. The children were then asked four other questions:
"Do you like looking at books?"
"Are you looking forward to learning to read?"
"Is it going to be easy for you to learn to read?"
"Do you think you will be good at reading?"
Although reading and writing were a focus of the questionnaire and
the child interview, only the results relating to reading are analyzed
and reported here.
Parents who gave their permission for themselves and their child to
participate in the study were given the questionnaire by their
child's preschool teacher; they were asked to return the completed
questionnaire, in a sealed envelope, to the relevant preschool teacher.
Of the 153 parents who agreed to participate, 140 questionnaires were
completed and returned, representing a return rate of 92%. For data
analysis, only matched responses in the parent questionnaires and child
interview questions were used, because some participants did not respond
to every question.
Data Analysis
Simple frequency counts were used for the descriptive data. Where
associations between variables were under scrutiny, two statistics were
used. Chi squared analyses were computed on frequency data. To check the
strengths of any associations found, the phi coefficient was used for
simple 2 x 2 tables and Cramer's V was used for more complex tables
(see Kinnear & Gray, 2004). Pearson product-moment correlations were
used on interval data. For all statistical analyses, only significance
levels of less than 0.05 are reported.
RESULTS
On average, for how many minutes per day did the parents read for
pleasure?
For this item, the parent selected either 20 minutes or fewer per
day, between 21 and 40 minutes per day, between 41 and 60 minutes per
day, and more than 60 minutes per day. As Figure 1 shows, the majority
of parents (56.9%) reported that, on average, they read for between 21
and 40 minutes per day, with few parents (7.3%) reading for greater than
60 minutes.
Was the frequency with which the parents read for pleasure
associated with the frequency with which they read to their children?
We hypothesized that the frequency of parent reading for pleasure
would be positively associated with the frequency with which they read
to the children. A cross-tabulation showed a positive association
between these two variables ([chi square] = 228.25, n = 138, df = 2, p =
.000). Cramer's V indicated that this association was strong.
How rich were the parents' reading preferences?
This variable was defined in two ways--the types of materials they
read, cross-referenced with the frequency with which they read these
types. There were three types of reading materials: traditional,
environmental, and new technology, and for each type there were four
examples, giving a total of 12 cells to be completed by each parent.
Parents also indicated whether they read daily (a score of 2), sometimes
(a score of 1), or never (a score of zero) for this range of reading
materials. The total maximum score for this question was therefore 24.
Initially, we looked at the group percentage distribution of scores for
the three types of reading materials, and these are shown in Figure 2.
[FIGURE 1 OMITTED]
[FIGURE 2 OMITTED]
[FIGURE 3 OMITTED]
As Figure 2 shows, the overwhelming majority of the parents
reported that they read materials from the three types on a daily basis:
traditional (91.4%), environmental (76.3%), and new technology (80.6%).
Second, we calculated a score out of 24 for each respondent. This
yielded a distribution that was slightly skewed toward the upper end of
scores. The range of scores was from 3 to 22, with a mean of 15.7, a
mode of 16, a median score of 16, and a standard deviation of 3.71.
These two results suggest that almost all of the parents in this study
read broadly, but the true distinction among the parents was the mean
duration of reading for pleasure per day, rather than the type of
materials they read.
From the above sets of data three categories were established,
representing an estimate of the richness of the parents' reading
habits. These were termed high, medium, and low. The high category
included those with a score greater than 18, as these parents would have
read all three types of materials, and the majority of these would have
been on a daily basis. The low category comprised those with a score of
12 or less. These parents either read a range of materials infrequently
or read frequently a very specific type of material. Finally, the medium
category comprised those respondents with a score greater than 12 and
less than 18, and these respondents read a broad range of materials
relatively frequently. Figure 3 gives the distribution of these
percentages.
Figure 3 shows that almost one third (32.6%) of the parents were
classified as high in terms of the richness of their reading habits,
with more than half (53.6%) of the parents' habits being medium
rich.
Was richness of parents' reading habits associated with the
frequency with which they read to their children?
We hypothesized that the richness of parents' reading habits
would be positively associated with the frequency with which parents
read to their children. A cross-tabulation was conducted to determine
whether these two variables were positively associated. Cramer's V
was used to check the strength of any associations found. The analysis
showed that a statistically significant association was found between
the variables ([chi square] = 8.904, n = 138, df = 2, p = 0.06).
Cramer's V indicated a medium-strength association.
Was richness of parents' reading habits associated with the
variety of literacy activities they engaged in with their children?
Using a Pearson product-moment correlation, we examined the
relationship between the richness of parents' reading habits and
the total number of different forms of literacy activities they engaged
in with their children. This statistic showed a positive relationship
between these two variables (r = .55, p < .001).
Was richness of parents' reading habits associated with the
children's orientation to literacy?
We hypothesized that the richness of the parents' reading
habits would be positively associated with the children's
orientation to literacy. Children's orientation to literacy was
determined by using their responses to four items in the child interview
(ORIENT). These items related to their level of enjoyment of reading,
their anticipation about learning to read, the ease with which the
children considered they would learn to read, and their motivation to
read. Positive responses to these questions were given a score of 1 and
negative responses a score of 0. Children scoring 0 to 2 were
categorized as low orientation and those with scores of 3 or 4 were
categorized as high orientation. A cross-tabulation indicated no
association between the variables.
Were the literacy activities that parents engaged in associated
with those they engaged in with their children?
To investigate this, Pearson product-moment correlations were
computed for the three literacy activity types: traditional,
environmental, and techno-literacy. The results of this analysis showed
significant correlations between these variables. For traditional
literacy activities, a medium-strength correlation between the
parents' preference and what they engaged in with their child was
found (r = .470, p = .000). Similarly, for environmental literacy
activities, a medium-strength correlation between the parents'
preference and what they engaged in with their child was found (r =
.469, p = .000). For techno-literacy activities, a medium-strength
correlation was found (r = .495, p = .000).
Was the variety of literacy activities that parents shared with
their children associated with the children's own literacy
preferences?
To investigate this association, a Pearson product-moment
correlation was conducted on these two variables. No significant
correlation was found.
DISCUSSION
This study investigated the habits of parents when reading for
pleasure, the frequency and types of literacy activities they engaged in
with their children, and the children's own interests and
orientation to literacy. Results show that, overall, these parents
tended to pass on their own literacy practices to their children, and
this finding is consistent with those from other studies (see e.g.,
Auerbach, 1989; Feiler & Webster, 1998; Whitmore et al., 2004).
More than one half of the parents read for pleasure between 21 and
40 minutes per day, and it is likely that these routines provide an
exemplar for their children. Around 80% also reported that they read
traditional, environmental, and new technology materials on a daily
basis, thereby demonstrating to their children that meaning is not
confined to print on paper alone, but also can be gleaned from other
forms of presentation. This is an interesting finding, which highlights
the diversity of literacies to which many children in this study were
exposed on a daily basis. Indeed, family literacies are not just
traditional literacies that have well-researched links to later literacy
achievements (see e.g., Harris et al., 2007; Weigel et al., 2006), but
also literacies whose links to later literacy may not be as well defined
yet may support children's everyday learning.
Another noteworthy finding was that the parents' literacy
preferences were mirrored in the types of literacy activities in which
they engaged with their children. Scrutiny of the distributions of these
scores indicated that the preferences of the parents in the current
study were slightly positively skewed toward traditional literacies.
Interestingly, however, this skew was not replicated in the distribution
of traditional scores with their children, A similar pattern emerged for
techno-literacies. Although there was a normal distribution of scores
for parents' preferences for environmental print, this was
negatively skewed in relation to the activities they did with their
children. This possibly suggests that techno-literacies are gaining
ground alongside traditional literacies in terms of parents'
preferences and also in relation to how they engage their children in
literacy. Thus, it is not surprising that many children commented on
their enjoyment when working on the computer. As one child mentioned,
"Um ... I can I can be happy and if I if I am on the computer
writing sometimes or on the email. I might be really happy. I know a lot
of words." Another commented on the shared experience of working on
the computer: "I like to do it with my mum or dad or my uncle or
aunt or my grandma or grandpa on the computer."
In this study, we found that the parents in this sample did more
than simply model literacy behaviors--they gave their children
intentional support, with the group of parents with richer literacy
habits reporting two distinct behaviors. First, they read more
frequently to their children than did the other parents, and second,
they shared their favored literacy activities with their children. If we
take into account these characteristics of routine exposure, provision
of exemplars, and intentional support, it is apparent that these
children are exposed to literacy-rich home environments by parents who
share characteristics with the facilitative mothers in the Weigel et al.
(2006) study; these mothers typically provided enriching literacy
environments for their children.
If this is so, it is of interest to note the lack of associations
we found between the parent variables (the richness of parents'
personal reading activities and those they share with their children)
and the child variables (their orientation to literacy and their
literacy choices). Perhaps it is the case that young children have
inconsistent preferences. Although they may enjoy shared and independent
literacy activities, they may not be certain about the notion of a
favorite activity, or perhaps what is favorite is the immediate rather
than a past or situation-specific activity, such as story reading at
bedtime. It is also possible that the different literacy materials used
for the child interview (such as the mobile phone) may have been viewed
by the children as toys that could be used as props in pretense, rather
than as materials for engaging in literacy, thus possibly confounding
the results.
What is interesting from these analyses is the lack of relationship
between these young children's own sense of themselves as readers
and their parents' interest in reading, and their reading habits.
This group of parents could be considered as a fairly literate group.
These children's responses, in part, may be due to their young age.
During the years before school, prior to any formal literacy teaching,
young children are clearly exploring their literate environments in a
number of ways (Clay, 1998). However, they may not consider themselves
as "readers" during this time, rather appreciating this as a
shared activity they enjoy, with parents taking the lead and with little
expectation that the children are required to do anything other than to
experience the satisfaction of shared time together, enjoyed by both
parties. What will be interesting to see is when these children are
followed up into formal school with the opportunities for overt
"teaching," by teachers and by parents, whether this
influences these children's reading development.
In essence, the parents in this sample, through simple exposure or
exemplars, and intentional support, envelop their children in symbols
(print and other forms) that express meaning within their family and
wider cultures. For many of these families, this is part of their daily
lives (Auerbach, 1989), is significant, and mirrors the literacy
interest of family members (Whitmore et al., 2004). Although these
practices are unique across families, they aim to imbue within children
a unique family literacy heritage. For many of these families, this
heritage upholds the value of reading being an enjoyable daily activity
that is individual and can be shared with others. It also promotes a
view that reading is not restricted to the so-called traditional
literacies but includes a wide range of materials, such as junk mail,
Web-based information sites, and mobile phone texts, not all of which
are print based.
DOI: 10.1080/02568543.2012.712086
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
The authors wish to acknowledge the parents, children, and teachers
who participated in this research and members of the Young
Learners' Project who assisted in data entry and read proofs of
this article. Further details of the study and its researchers can be
found at www.edfac.unimelb. edu.au/younglearners.
REFERENCES
Auerbach, E. R. (1989). Toward a social-contextual approach to
family literacy. Harvard Educational Review, 59(2), 165-181.
Bus, A. G., van Ijzedndoorn, M. H., & Pellegrini, A. D. (1995).
Joint book reading makes for success in learning to read: A
meta-analysis on intergenerational transmission of literacy. Review of
Educational Research, 65(1), 1-21.
Byrnes, L. J., & Brown, P. M. (2007). ORIENT: A scale to
measure children's orientation to literacy. Unpublished interview
schedule, The University of Melbourne, Melbourne, Australia.
Cambourne, B. (1988). The whole story: Natural learning and the
acquisition of literacy in the classroom. Auckland, New Zealand: Ashton
Scholastic.
Clay, M. M. (1998). By different paths to common outcomes. York,
ME: Stenhouse.
Feiler, A., & Webster, A. (1998). Success and failure in early
literacy: Teachers' predictions and subsequent intervention.
British Journal of Special Education, 25(4), 189-195.
Gregory, E. (2001). Sisters and brothers as language and literacy
teachers: Synergy between siblings playing and working together. Journal
of Early Childhood Literacy, 1, 301-322.
Harris, K. K., Loyo, J. J., Holahan, C. K., Suzuki, R., &
Gottlieb, N. H. (2007). Cross-sectional predictors of reading to young
children among participants in the Texas WIC program. Journal of
Research in Childhood Education, 21, 254-268.
Kinnear, P. R., & Gray, C. D. (2004). SPSS 12 made simple.
Hove, England: Psychology Press.
Marsh, J. (2004). The techno-literacy practices of young children.
Journal of Early Childhood Research, 2(1), 51-66.
Marsh, J., & Thompson P. (2001). Parental involvement in
literacy development: Using media texts. Journal of Research in Reading,
24, 266-278.
Natsiopoulou, T., Souliotis, D., Kyridis, A., & Hatzisavvides,
S. (2006). Reading children's books to the preschool children in
Greek families. International Journal of Early Childhood, 38(2), 69-79.
Pirrie, A. (1999). "Supposing": Reading between the
lines: An allegorical account of contemporary debates on literacy
acquisition. British Journal of Educational Studies, 47, 348-363.
Roberts, J., Jurgens, J., & Burchinal, M. (2005). The role of
home literacy practices in preschool children's language and
emergent literacy skills. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing
Research, 48, 345-259.
Rogoff, B. (2003). The cultural nature of human development. New
York, NY: Oxford University Press.
Saracho, O. N. (2007). Hispanic families as facilitators of their
children's literacy development. Journal of Hispanic Higher
Education, 6(2), 103-117.
Senechal, M., & LeFevre, J. (2002). Parental involvement in the
development of children's reading skill: A five-year longitudinal
study. Child Development, 73, 445-460.
Vygotsky, L. (1978). Cole, M. (Ed). Mind in society: The
development of higher psychological processes. Cambridge, MA: Harvard
University Press
Weigel, D. J., Martin, S. S., & Bennett, K. K. (2006).
Mothers' literacy beliefs: Connections with the home literacy
environment and pre-school children's literacy development. Journal
of Early Childhood Literacy, 6(2), 191-211.
Weinberger, J. (1996). A longitudinal study of children's
early literacy experiences at home and later literacy development at
home and school. Journal of Research in Reading, 19(1), 14-24.
Whitmore, K. F., Martens, P., Goodman, Y. M., & Owocki, G.
(2004). Critical lessons from the transactional perspective on early
literacy research. Journal of Early Childhood Literacy, 4, 291-325.
P. Margaret Brown and Linda J. Byrnes
The University of Melbourne, Melbourne, Australia
Bridie Raban
The University of Melbourne and Australian Council for Educational
Research, Melbourne, Australia
Linda Watson
University of Birmingham, Birmingham, United Kingdom
Submitted January 17, 2011; accepted June 30, 2011.
This research was funded by the Australian Research Council
(Project No: LP0883437) in conjunction with its partner organization,
the Australian Scholarships Group.
Address correspondence to Dr. Linda J. Byrnes, Melbourne Graduate
School of Education, The University of Melbourne, Melbourne, VIC 3010,
Australia. E-mail: lhyrnes@unimelb.edu.au