The Early ON School Readiness Project: a preliminary report.
Winter, Suzanne M. ; Zurcher, Raymond ; Hernandez, Arthur 等
Abstract. The Early ON School Readiness Project is an emerging
community-based model aimed at promoting the school readiness of 3- to
5-year-old children in a metropolitan area with a predominantly Latino
(Mexican-American) population. Using an ecological approach, the
multi-component program seeks to enhance school readiness opportunities
for children at various levels. Key program components aim to improve
community awareness, parent education, professional development for
child care teachers, quality of child care environments, and transition
to school. The model evolved through a community partnership initiated
by the local government, involving nonprofit agencies and a university
partnership. Child care environmental quality was measured by
administration of the Early Childhood Environment Rating Scale-Revised
Edition (ECERS-R; Harms, Clifford, & Cryer, 1998). To measure
children's progress, the Developmental Indicators for the
Assessment of Learning-Third Edition (DIAL-& Mardell-Czudnowski
& Goldenberg, 1998) was administered in the fall and spring of two
consecutive school years. A sample of children was screened to gain
insight regarding the developmental status of children in the project
compared to a matched sample of children not involved in the project.
Preliminary findings show the progress of children in key developmental
areas during the initial and second year of program implementation in
selected child care centers. In the baseline year, standard scores of
children at the comparison sites were unchanged from fall to spring in
all of the developmental areas measured by the DIAL-3; scores at the
intervention sites, however, were higher at a statistically significant
level for two subtests and the composite score. During the second year,
all subtest scores and the composite scores were higher at the
intervention sites in the spring as compared to the fall. Although
additional research is needed, preliminary results suggest the emerging
model shows promise for increasing children's developmental skills
and abilities associated with school readiness.
**********
Major paradigm shifts in national policy and priorities, along with
increased accountability for schools, have brought the issue of school
readiness to the forefront. As a result, calls for public investment in
early education have led communities across the United States to become
more active in developing programs to enhance the school readiness
skills and abilities of all children (Shonkoff, 2004). Local
policymakers have launched efforts to meld policy and programmatic arenas into cohesive frameworks of support for families with
preschool-age children (Knitzer, 2002; Murphey & Burns, 2002; Weigel
& Martin, 2006). Social and economic motivation to improve the
quality of care and educational contexts for young children has remained
high (Brauner, Gordic, & Zigler, 2004). Accordingly, many local
governments have created policy and forged new alliances among community
agencies and early childhood programs. Initiating new relationships
among governmental entities and fostering community partnerships are
strategies purported to hold promise for better child outcomes (Kagan
& Neuman, 2003).
Technological advances in the neurosciences also have stirred
interest in early learning and school readiness. Brain imaging research
has provided insight into how the architecture of the brain develops and
has underscored the importance of stimulating brain development early in
a child's life. Technology has revealed that a child's brain
undergoes a hierarchical maturation process, first establishing lower
level skills as a foundation for the later acquisition of more complex
skills. The optimal time to influence neural development is during early
childhood, when neurons are making connections, or "synapses,"
at an amazing rate. Increasing evidence supports the theoretical
perspective that brain development is influenced by both nature and
nurture. While nature provides children with their biological and
genetic characteristics, early experience can shape the expression of
genes, thereby influencing the development of specific neural circuits.
Clearly, the architecture of the brain is influenced by early experience
and stimulation provided in children's physical and social
environments. While poor-quality environments with insufficient
stimulation can inhibit brain development, a stimulating environment
promotes neural development and early learning, even when children are
at risk for poor developmental outcomes (Bergen & Coscia, 2001;
D'Arcangelo, 2003; Knudsen, Heckman, Cameron, & Shonkoff, 2006;
Shonkoff & Phillips, 2000). Brain imaging technology has also
provided an opportunity to examine the effects of socioeconomic status,
race, and ethnicity on brain development. Although studies are limited,
preliminary results suggest that racial disparities in cognitive
development and school achievement may be mediated by socioeconomic
status. Rather than being attributable to race or ethnicity,
developmental and achievement differences appear to be associated with
the negative effects of poverty on brain development (Noble, Tottenham,
& Casey, 2005).
Quality of Early Learning Environments
Evidence substantiates that learning environments play a key role
in nurturing children's development and stimulating early learning.
Consequently, there has been much interest in identifying specific
contextual factors that might exert the greatest impact on
children's attainment of school readiness skills and abilities.
Large-scale, national studies have been instrumental in establishing a
strong association between high-quality programs and better
developmental outcomes in the cognitive and language domains, two areas
considered foundational to academic success (Belsky, 2001; Fuller,
Kagan, Loeb, & Chang, 2004; Peisner-Feinberg et al., 2000; Shonkoff
& Phillips, 2000). The Cost, Quality, and Outcomes in Child Care
Centers Longitudinal Study established the importance of high-quality
child care to ensure children's school readiness and better
long-term academic achievement (Peisner-Feinberg et al., 2000). The
National Institute of Child Health and Human Development (NICHD)
longitudinal studies have provided corroborating evidence that
high-quality child care is a strong predictor of children's
cognitive attainment, language performance, and school readiness skills
but is not predictive of social ability. Moreover, the NICHD studies
reported these predictions held true regardless of children's
personal characteristics and family heritage (National Institute of
Child Health and Human Development [NICHD] Early Child Care Research
Network, 2002, 2003, 2004, 2005). The national study of Early Head Start
programs also reported positive results for children's cognitive,
language, social, and behavioral outcomes (Love et al., 2005). In Great
Britain, the Effective Provision of Pre-school Education (EPPE) study
provided strong evidence of a positive correlation between school
readiness and preschool attendance (Sammons et al., 2004).
The enduring effects of high-quality programs regarding
children's achievement are important to school success.
Longitudinal studies indicate that children who attend high-quality
preschool programs tend to gain cognitive advantages, such as higher
achievement in math and reading, once they enter school. Moreover,
children appear to maintain a higher performance across time compared to
peers who do not attend high-quality programs (Gormley Jr., Gayer,
Phillips, & Dawson, 2005; Magnuson, Meyers, Ruhm, & Waldfogel,
2004; Ou, 2005). Recent follow-up analyses of the NICHD Study of Early
Child Care and Youth Development (SECCYD) reported a strong relationship
between high-quality preschool experience and better vocabulary in the
elementary grades. These findings further substantiate the association
of high-quality child care with positive outcomes in children's
language development (Belsky et al., 2007).
Inequities in access to high-quality care have been
well-documented. One study reported that only 10 percent of child care
centers studied were rated as excellent (NICHD Early Child Care Research
Network, 2000). These findings corroborated earlier reports that raised
concerns about accessibility of high-quality care and the general lack
of research that would provide insight regarding the effects of low to
mediocre child care (Belsky, 2001; Cost, Quality and Child Outcomes
Study Team, 1995; Magnuson et al., 2004; Vandell, 2004). Less is known
about the effects of poor to mediocre quality care or whether these
environments can properly support the growth and development of
children, especially those at risk of developmental delays (Buysse,
Wesley, Bryant, & Gardner, 1999; Wolery, Brashers, & Neitzel,
2002). Mounting evidence suggests that large numbers of children
experience substandard care that may be detrimental to their learning.
Further, when care of lesser quality is center-based, children appear to
be particularly at risk of developing persistent behavioral problems
(Belsky et al., 2007).
Attending better quality early childhood programs may be especially
critical for children who are economically disadvantaged and for those
who are at risk for developmental delays (Peisner-Feinberg et al., 2001;
Peisner-Feinberg et al., 2000; Pigott & Israel, 2005; Sammons et
al., 2004). There is evidence that quality may exert a stronger
influence on the outcomes of children in poverty compared to those in
higher income families (Burchinal et al., 2000; Hubbs-Tait et al.,
2002). Yet, low-income families are less likely to enroll their children
in center-based educational settings. One explanation is that families
in poverty often lack sufficient access to the high-quality centers that
might improve educational support for children when family resources are
meager. Inequitable access can widen the gap in school readiness
experienced by poor children, who might benefit from high-quality
educational experiences to stimulate cognitive and language development.
Children who attend high-quality preschool programs may gain lasting
advantages that improve their chances for success throughout their
schooling, such as less retention and higher math and reading
achievement (Hoff, 2003; Magnuson et al., 2004; Weigel & Martin,
2006). The link between high-quality learning environments and positive
developmental outcomes for children has led to widespread calls for
communities to take action and make investments in improving the quality
of child care as a strategy to promote school readiness (Brauner et al.,
2004; Pianta, 2007).
Learning and Latino Children
Latino children constitute the largest child minority population in
the United States. According to the U.S. Census, Latinos are the largest
and fastest growing minority group in the United States, with a growth
rate exceeding that of the nation. However, for some Latino children,
high poverty rates and other risk factors threaten their chances for
academic success (Morse, 2003; National Center for Education Statistics,
2005; United States Census Bureau, 2006). Studies have reported that
children from low-income, minority families enter school less prepared
and lacking the fundamental skills to succeed (Lee & Burkam, 2002;
McLanahan, 2005). While growing numbers of children in the United States
are enrolling in preschool programs, disparities still exist (Pianta,
2007). Children from advantaged homes are enrolled in early childhood
programs at a higher rate than children from disadvantaged homes,
resulting in a school readiness gap (Magnuson et al., 2004). Latino
children, less likely to be enrolled in early childhood programs
compared to other children, are often caught in the school readiness
gap. Recruiting Latino families into early childhood programs and
improving the quality of programs attended by Latino children are among
two strategies that could help close the gap (Collins & Ribeiro,
2004; Magnuson & Waldfogel, 2005; Morse, 2003). Unfortunately, even
if efforts to close the gap are successful, Latino children and their
families may still face potential barriers. For example, despite the
rapidly increasing Latino population in the United States, the number of
bilingual and Latino early childhood professionals in the field is
insufficient to meet the rising need. Inadequate preparation for
teaching in culturally and linguistically diverse early childhood
classrooms is also detrimental to children's success. Failure to
address differences in culture and language can hamper home-school communication, limit parental participation, and make relationships
between teachers and Latino families cumbersome (Buysse, Castro, West,
& Skinner, 2005; Collins & Ribeiro, 2004).
Considerable attention has been focused on reducing risk factors
for minority children and identifying ways to improve their chances for
success in school. Variations in child care quality and accessibility
may limit minority children's educational opportunities and result
in poor achievement once these children enter school (Pianta, 2007).
Experts have stressed that serious action is needed toward upgrading the
quality of child care systems, especially those serving racial and
ethnic minorities in the United States. It is critical to identify ways
to help parents succeed as the first teachers of their children,
especially when parents may face barriers to achieving their own
literacy (Cassidy et al., 2004).
Research suggests certain program characteristics and practices may
be advantageous in preparing Latino children for school. For instance,
providing a culturally responsive program with appropriate levels of
family support can help Latino preschoolers improve their school
readiness skills and abilities (Gormley Jr. et al., 2005; Ryan, 2005).
Relationships with Latino parents may be enhanced when early childhood
professionals receive staff development aimed at improving cultural
competence and sensitivity (Collins & Ribeiro, 2004). Programs
characterized as comprehensive, using multiple strategies and
components, appear to better prepare Latino children for success in
school, compared to programs with less comprehensive approaches (Buysse
et al., 2005; Pigott & Israel, 2005; Ryan, 2005). These strategies
are recommended to help bridge the cultures of home and school to better
ensure the school readiness of Latino children.
Purpose
The purpose of this study was exploratory in nature and intended to
examine the efforts of an emerging city-wide program aimed at improving
the school readiness of young children in a predominantly Latino
(Mexican American) population. We are reporting preliminary data
collected during the baseline and one subsequent year as the new program
emerged. Consequently, the purposes for this study were twofold: 1) to
gain insight into the overall quality of the child care center
environments experienced by children in the Early ON School Readiness
Project, and 2) to sample the developmental progress of children
enrolled in the program.
Method
Participants
The participants were children, ages 3-5, from predominantly
Mexican American families whose average income was approximately
$20,000. Acculturation questions revealed that the majority of the
families preferred to speak English at home and reported their
children's first language as English. The parental age range most
frequently reported was from 20 to 29 years, with the highest
educational attainment reported as high school. The children were
enrolled in child care centers located across San Antonio, Texas, a
large metropolitan city in the southwestern United States. The
population served by each center included children whose families
qualified for a level of subsidized care assistance.
The Program
Emergence of Program. The Early ON program was launched as part of
a community development initiative to achieve broad, systemic change
through activities designed to strengthen families and improve the
future workforce. Similar to other community-based early childhood
projects, the Early ON program can be described as emergent rather than
explicit and preplanned. Emergent programs often evolve through
community partnerships and are informed by empowered stakeholders who
know the needs and culture of the community (Buysse, Wesley, &
Skinner, 1999; Hurd, Lerner, & Barton, 1999; Kaplan & Larkin,
2004). In this case, the local government initiated a
community-university partnership consisting of nonprofit community
agencies, school districts, and the University of Texas at San Antonio.
The community agencies and one of the school districts were contracted
to facilitate implementation of the program in child care centers
selected for participation in the program. The key role of the
University of Texas at San Antonio was to conduct formative research to
examine the process of program development and summative research
studies to examine the impact of the emerging program. Monthly meetings
of the community-university partnership served as a forum for consensus
building as a common set of program strategies evolved. From the
collaborative vision and effort of the community-university partnership,
a comprehensive, multi-component model program, the Early ON School
Readiness Project, emerged.
Key Intervention Strategies. Based on an ecological approach, the
program consisted of strategies aimed at layers of contexts: community,
home, child care, and the next school setting, kindergarten. Five key
intervention strategies emerged: 1) Community Awareness--A media
campaign was launched with public service announcements to encourage
parents to be the first teachers of their children. Brochures with
school readiness guidelines for parents in both English and Spanish were
distributed. 2) Parent Education--In Year 1, monthly sessions on school
readiness topics were held at child care centers. In Year 2, the
community agencies agreed upon a common set of topics and content for
the sessions. 3) Teacher Professional Development--In Year 1, individual
community agencies made decisions about the content and number of
sessions. Teachers received professional development typical of most
community-based programs. In Year 2, the agencies decided to provide
five half-day workshops on literacy development to all teachers in the
project. 4) Child Care Quality--In Year 1, Early ON community agencies
provided technical assistance to improve the overall environment and
teaching practices at centers. In Year 2, the community agencies gave
increased emphasis on ensuring that all centers had materials to promote
literacy development. 5) Transition to School--Child care teachers met
with kindergarten teachers at the neighborhood elementary school several
times a year to become acquainted and to plan a smooth transition
process.
Study Design
The sites for the study were child care centers selected to
participate in the Early ON School Readiness Project. As is typical of
most communities, the centers varied in type from federally funded Head
Start, nonprofit, and faith-based centers to proprietary child care
facilities. During the first year of the project, the authors collected
data to establish a baseline for subsequent years. This preliminary
examination focused on two aspects of the program. First, we examined
child care center quality at the beginning and end of each school year,
using a descriptive approach. Second, we assessed the progress of
children, using a quasi-experimental pretest, posttest comparison group
design. A new cohort of children was examined each year.
Procedures
Center Quality. To measure the overall quality of the child care
environments, we administered the Early Childhood Environment Rating
Scale-Revised Edition (ECERS-R; Harms et al., 1998), widely used in
early childhood research to measure global quality of child care
centers. The ECERS-R yields a total score and seven subscale scores:
Space and Furnishings, Personal Care Routines, Language-Reasoning,
Activities, Interaction, Program Structure, and Parents and Staff.
Rating scores on the ECERS-R range from 1.0 to 7.0, with 1.0 being
considered "inadequate" and 7.0 considered to be
"excellent." Research staff and university students were
trained as raters to administer the ECERS-R. Raters received three hours
of intensive training from a research coordinator with a master's
degree in education who followed recommended training protocols,
including item-by-item discussions and practice coding with the ECERS-R
videotaped training. According to the ECERS-R authors, interrater
internal consistency coefficients range from 0.71 to 0.88 for the
subscales and 0.92 for the total environment score. Therefore, an
interrater reliability of 80 percent agreement among pairs of raters was
established before data collection began in the child care centers. The
procedure for establishing interrater reliability consisted of paired
raters administering the ECERS-R in child care classrooms,
simultaneously. Ratings that were identical were counted as an
agreement. If raters failed to achieve 80 percent agreement, they
discussed disparities and attempted to establish the required level of
interrater reliability in another set of observations.
The ECERS-R scale was administered at each center during the fall
and spring. Interview items were addressed to center personnel and two
hours of observation at the center was necessary to complete
observational items. ECERS-R was administered in the morning hours to
ensure that most of the children would be present and engaged in program
activities. However, Buell and Cassidy (2001) have urged consideration
of the Complex Dynamical Systems Theory, also known as the Chaos Theory,
when evaluating child care centers, to account for subtle changes and
fluctuations in program quality across time. Following their
recommendation, we avoided the single "snapshot" approach and
endeavored to administer the ECERS-R at least twice at each center,
averaging the scores to obtain a mean. In Year 1, there were 17 centers,
and in Year 2 the project had doubled to 34 centers in the program,
necessitating a higher number of observations for the ECERS-R
assessment.
Children's Progress. The Developmental Indicators for the
Assessment of Learning-Third Edition (DIAL-3) (Mardell-Czudnowski &
Goldenberg, 1998) was administered to children at the child care centers
in the study during the fall and spring of each school year. The DIAL-3
provides subtest scores in the development categories of Motor (gross
and fine motor development), Concepts (knowledge of such basic concepts
as counting and colors), and Language (use of receptive and expressive
language), as well as an Overall Composite score. Each of the standard
scores and the overall composite score has a mean of 100 and an SD of
15. The DIAL-3 can be team- or individually administered, and each of
the three content areas has up to 10 questions (there are basals and
cutoffs, and the test requires approximately 15 minutes to administer).
According to the publisher, the DIAL-3 has internal consistency
coefficients that range from 0.66-0.87. Results from concurrent validity studies suggest that scores from this test can be considered an adequate
measure of early childhood development across the constructs
represented.
To gain insight into the developmental progress of children, a
sample of approximately one-quarter of the children in the intervention
sites was selected for administration of a developmental assessment each
year of the study. Each year, the investigators used a judgment sample
of children thought to be representative of four geographic quadrants of
the city. Children from 10 child care centers in Year 1 and 11 child
care centers in Year 2 were assessed. Four matched comparison sites were
selected from child care centers not involved in the intervention
program. These sites were located in each of the four quadrants of the
city and matched on major characteristics of intervention centers in
those areas. The DIAL-3 was administered during the baseline year of the
project at the intervention sites and comparison sites. The test was
again administered during the second year of the project at the
intervention sites. Tests were administered during the fall and spring
of each school year, during the morning hours so as not to take place
immediately before or after nap time.
University students participated in data collection by
administering the DIAL-3. Prior to on-site data collection, students
were provided with three hours of intensive training that conformed to
training procedures recommended in the test manual. Staff members with
master's degrees administered the test and answered questions posed
by the students. Students also practiced using a DIAL-3 training video
and administered the test to one another. A team administration of the
DIAL-3 was used at the child care centers so that university students
could be supervised by research staff. As members of the team, students
had an opportunity to observe and work alongside staff members. To
ensure accuracy of administration and scoring, students could ask any
questions that might have arisen during test administration.
Analysis
Analysis was conducted using the SPSS statistical analysis package.
Analysis of the quality of the child care environments was conducted by
generating descriptive statistics, mean and standard deviations for each
subscale, and the total for the ECERS-R instrument to compare fall and
spring scores each year. To analyze children's progress,
descriptive statistics, including the mean and standard deviations, were
calculated using standard scores of the intervention group and the
comparison group of children at the fall pretest and the spring
posttest. A two-tailed hypothesis test was conducted for the
intervention group and the comparison group each year to determine
whether children made progress from fall to spring each year, as
increases are expected for typically developing children. A second
two-tailed hypothesis test was conducted to compare results for the
intervention and comparison group children each year. Sample sizes
varied slightly for each test, due to missing data. Using criteria
recommended by Cohen (1988), an estimate of effect size (partial 112)
was calculated to assess the degree to which study variables were
related.
Results
This section provides information about the quality of the child
care centers involved in the project as measured by ECERS-R,
developmental progress within the comparison and intervention sites by
year, and between the comparison and intervention sites by year.
Quality of Environments
Table 1 shows that on the seven attributes measured by the ECERS-R,
the centers on average changed very little. According to the authors of
the ECERS-R, the scores found in Table 1 show that the centers involved
in this project are "good" but not "excellent." A
recent examination of the psychometric properties of the ECERS-R
suggests that the overall score is most indicative of the quality of the
child care environment. The factor analysis investigations conducted
revealed redundancy among items, resulting in subgroups similar to the
overall score (Perlman, Zellman, & Le, 2004). Results, presented in
Table 1, corroborate the Perlman et al. findings. Specifically, the
subtest scores and the overall scores are relatively similar within
semesters.
Developmental Progress Within Sites
During the baseline year, standard scores on the DIAL-3 were not
different ([alpha] = .05) at the comparison sites from fall to spring
(see Table 2). Average scores on the Motor subtest were almost one
standard deviation above the mean (approximately 112 for both
semesters), while scores on the Concepts and Language subtests were
slightly lower than the mean. Average Motor and Language subtest scores,
as well as the average Overall Composite score, were higher in the
spring as compared to the fall at the intervention sites for the
baseline year. Again, average scores on the Motor subtest were higher
than average in the spring, with the average motor subtest score for
students at the intervention sites approximately one standard deviation
above the mean (114.8). Average Concepts and Language subtest scores, as
well as the average Overall Composite scores, increased from fall to
spring during the second year of the program. While scores on the Motor
subtest were again above the mean, no statistically significant
difference existed between the two semesters.
For year one at the intervention sites, effect sizes (partial
[[eta].sup.2]) for the Motor, Language, and Overall Composite scores
meet Cohen's criterion for a "small" effect size (.010)
(Cohen, 1988). Cohen suggested that effect sizes in naturalistic studies
are typically small, as they are affected by multiple influences.
However, the effect sizes estimated for this study are similar to those
seen in the Study of Early Child Care, a longitudinal study of over
1,000 children also conducted in a naturalistic setting (NICHD Early
Child Care Network, 2002). The effect size for the Language scores
during year two at the intervention sites also meets this criterion.
During year one, differences between the Concepts, Language, and Overall
scores between the intervention and comparison sites also resulted in a
"small" effect size.
Developmental Progress Between Comparison and Intervention Sites
Scores also were compared between the comparison sites and
intervention for the spring semesters of the baseline year and year two.
Table 3 shows that scores at the intervention sites were significantly
higher on the Language subtest than those at comparison sites during the
baseline year; however, the Motor, Concepts, and Overall Scores were not
found to be different. The effect sizes for the Language (partial
[[eta].sup.2] = 0.026) and Concepts (partial [[eta].sup.2] = 0.011)
scores can be considered "small." Year two scores at the
intervention sites were not significantly different from scores at
comparison sites.
Discussion
Consistent with the findings of the NICHD Early Child Care Research
Network (2000) and other studies of community-based child care, the
centers in this study fell into the mid-range of quality as measured by
the ECERS-R. The centers were "good" but not
"excellent" in quality. Although improving the overall quality
of the child care center sites was one of the goals of the project, the
center environments began the study in the "good" category and
were at the same level at the conclusion of the study. This finding
suggests that changing the quality of the centers may take more time,
effort, and resources than those brought to bear in the initial two
years of the project.
In examining the child outcomes measured by the DIAL-3, increases
in standard scores from fall to spring suggest that the approaches and
elements of the Early ON School Readiness intervention are promising.
Especially promising are the scores from the DIAL-3 Language subtest
(this subtest measures both expressive and receptive language skills as
well as phonemic awareness). Scores on the Language subtest were not
only higher at intervention sites than at the comparison sites at
posttest the first year of the project, but also increased from years
one and two of the project at the intervention sites. As phonemic
awareness is an important predictor of reading ability (International
Reading Association, 1998), further research on the methods and outcomes
of the Early ON intervention is warranted. These findings, however, are
particularly noteworthy, since the children in the study were
predominantly Latino from low-income families. Previous studies have
found that minority and economically disadvantaged children are at
greater risk of lacking fundamental school readiness skills upon school
entry (Lee & Burkam, 2002; McLanahan, 2005).
The fact that the scores on the Motor subtest (this subtest
measures such skills as catching, jumping, hopping, skipping, building
with blocks, cutting, copying shapes and letters, and writing) were
consistently higher than the national average would seem to indicate
that children at these centers have the growth and physical development
needed for learning. The Motor subtest scores are approximately one
standard deviation above scores on the other (Language and Concepts)
subtests. The children's environments may offer a multitude of
opportunities for them to improve their fine and gross motor skills but
lack the academic stimulation that is necessary for them to develop
cognitive skills on par with their motor skills.
A unique strength of the Early ON School Readiness Project has been
the grassroots effort of community agencies working toward the common
goals of the program. Each community agency has brought a wealth of
conventional wisdom, community experience, and specialized services to
the children and families served by the Early ON School Readiness
Project. Ideas spawned through collaboration of community partners have
been tried within clusters consisting of selected child care centers and
elementary schools. As the project progressed, stakeholders and
researchers continued to make solid progress toward a program model with
codified strategies and procedures that can be replicated in other
sites, locally and beyond. While the collaboration of community agencies
has provided many benefits, it has taken time for community partners to
move toward consensus regarding the specific program strategies to use
in promoting school readiness. Discussions between community partners
during the first two years of the program have helped to frame the
evaluation of program strategies and build consensus surrounding
programmatic strategies chosen.
Limitations of the Study
This study was intended to be exploratory and developmental in
nature. As commonly reported by developers and researchers of other
community-based approaches to early care and intervention (e.g., Buysse,
Wesley, & Skinner, 1999), this study had several limitations readers
should consider when generalizing study findings to other populations.
Children sampled for progress were not selected at random. Instead, the
sampling method did employ judgment and an attempt to represent a
roughly geographically representative sample.
The investigators faced limitations regarding the extent of
guidance they could exert to encourage consistent implementation of the
Early ON program strategies at the child care centers. As noted in the
introduction of this article, researchers worked through representatives
of the partner community agencies to encourage full implementation of
program strategies and were not able to work directly with centers.
Implementation of the program strategies was somewhat inconsistent
across sites and across the time period of the study as the program
emerged through consensus building of the community partners. Moreover,
broadly aimed community awareness strategies could not be strictly
controlled. Consequently, parents at both the intervention sites and the
comparison sites may have experienced some exposure to the media
campaign. These limitations may have exerted an impact on the extent to
which score gains can be attributed to the Early ON School Readiness
Project.
Implications and Future Research
The results of this study contribute to a better understanding of
the impact of child care typically available in community settings that
falls within the middle range of quality. Preliminary data suggest that
communities may be able to boost the development and school readiness
skills of children enrolled in "good" quality centers with an
emergent program approach. That is, strategies that emerge from
community partnerships and are aimed at key program components, such as
teacher training and parent education, might be effective. While it was
beyond the scope of this preliminary examination, future studies could
help to determine whether specific strategies in the multi-component
program were responsible for greater developmental gains by children at
the intervention sites and to what degree. Information of this kind
could help streamline program strategies for greater efficiency and
better results.
Community development approaches can be challenging to implement,
especially when the community is diverse. However, studies suggest that
such grassroots efforts might yield positive results for children.
Community agencies with a long history of working with minority
families, in collaboration with university partners who can inform the
emergent program process with evidence-based research, may be successful
in identifying program strategies that can have a positive impact on the
school readiness of minority children (Buysse, Wesley, Bryant et al.,
1999; Buysse et al., 2005).
Once the Early ON School Readiness Project is better established
and program strategies are consistently implemented, it will be possible
to conduct more rigorous types of research designs to gain greater
insight into the effectiveness and, ultimately, the efficacy of the
program. The preliminary data reported in this study suggests the
program may be beneficial to children. However, future empirical
research using experimental design and randomized trials will be needed
to fully establish the effectiveness of the new program. Future research
also can be aimed toward streamlining a model for building community
alliances and developing programs that are informed by research and
tempered with local wisdom and experience.
Acknowledgments
This study was funded, in part, by the Department of Community
Initiatives of the City of San Antonio, Texas. The authors wish to
acknowledge the contributions of Dennis Campa, Director of the
Department of Community Initiatives, for his vision and leadership in
establishing this community-university partnership.
References
Belsky, J. (2001). Emanuel Miller lecture: Developmental risks
(still) associated with early child care. Journal of Child Psychology,
42(7), 845-859.
Belsky, J., Burchinal, M., McCartney, K., Vandell, D. L.,
Clarke-Stewart, K. A., Owen, M. T., et al. (2007). Are there long-term
effects of early child care? Child Development, 78(2), 681-701.
Bergen, D., & Coscia, J. (2001). Brain research and childhood
education: Implications for educators. Olney, MD: Association for
Childhood Education International.
Brauner, J., Gordic, B., & Zigler, E. (2004). Putting the child
back into child care: Combining care and education for children ages
3-5. Social Policy Report, 18(3), 3-15.
Buell, M. J., & Cassidy, D. J. (2001). The complex and dynamic
nature of quality in early care and educational programs: A case for
chaos. Journal of Research in Childhood Education, 15(2), 209-219.
Burchinal, M. R., Roberts, J., Riggins, R., Zeisel, S., Neebe, E.,
& Bryant, D. (2000). Relating quality of center-based child care to
early cognitive and language development longitudinally. Child
Development, 71, 339-357.
Buysse, V., Castro, D. C., West, T., & Skinner, M. (2005).
Addressing the needs of Latino children: A national survey of state
administrators of early childhood programs. Early Childhood Research
Quarterly, 20, 146-163.
Buysse, V., Wesley, P. W., Bryant, D., & Gardner, D. (1999).
Quality of early childhood programs in inclusive and noninclusive
settings. Exceptional Children, 65(3), 301-314.
Buysse, V., Wesley, P., & Skinner, D. (1999). Community
development approaches for early intervention. Topics in Early Childhood
Special Education, 19(4), 236-243.
Cassidy, J., Garcia, R., Tejeda-Delgado, C., Garrett, S. D.,
Martinez-Garcia, C., & Hinojosa, R.V. (2004). A learner-centered
family literacy project for Latino parents and caregivers. Journal of
Adolescent & Adult Literacy, 47(6), 478-488.
Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral
sciences (2nd ed.). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Collins, R., & Ribeiro, R. (2004). Toward an early care and
education agenda for Hispanic children. Early Childhood Research and
Practice, 6(2). Retrieved March 15, 2007, from
http://ecrp.uiuc.edu/v6n2/collins.html.
Cost, Quality and Child Outcomes Study Team. (1995). Cost, quality,
and child outcomes in child care centers. Denver, CO: University of
Colorado at Denver, Department of Economics.
D'Arcangelo, M. (2003). On the mind of a child: A conversation
with Sally Shaywitz. Educational Leadership, 60(7), 6-10.
Fuller, B., Kagan, S., Loeb, S., & Chang, Y. (2004). Child care
quality: Centers and home settings that serve poor families. Early
Childhood Research Quarterly, 19(4), 505-527.
Gormley Jr., W. T., Gayer, T., Phillips, D., & Dawson, B.
(2005). The effects of universal pre-k on cognitive development.
Developmental Psychology, 41(6), 872-884.
Harms, T., Clifford, R. M., & Cryer, D. (1998). Early childhood
environment rating scale. New York: Teachers College Press.
Hoff, E. (2003). The specificity of environmental influence:
Socioeconomic status affects early vocabulary development via maternal
speech. Child Development, 74, 1368-1378.
Hubbs-Tait, L., Culp, A. M., Huey, E., Culp, R., Starost, H., &
Hare, C. (2002). Relation of Head Start attendance to children's
cognitive and social outcomes: Moderation by family risk. Early
Childhood Research Quarterly, 17(4), 539-558.
Hurd, T. L., Lerner, R. M., & Barton, C. E. (1999). Integrated
services: Expanding partnerships to meet the needs of today's
children and families. Young Children, 54(2), 74-80.
International Reading Association. (1998). Phonemic awareness and
the teaching of reading: A position statement summary of the
International Reading Association. Retrieved June 19, 2007, from
www.reading.org/positions/phonemic.html.
Kagan, S. L., & Neuman, M. J. (2003). Integrating early care
and education. Educational Leadership, 60(7), 58-63.
Kaplan, M., & Larkin, E. (2004). Launching intergenerational programs in early childhood settings: A comparison of explicit
intervention with an emergent approach. Early Childhood Education
Journal, 31(3), 157-163.
Knitzer, J. (2002). Children's readiness for school: Toward a
strategic policy framework. News & Issues, 12(3), 1.
Knudsen, E. I., Heckman, J. J., Cameron, J. L., & Shonkoff, J.
P. (2006). Economic, neurobiological, and behavioral perspectives on
building America's future workforce. Proceedings of the National
Academy of Sciences, 103(27), 10155-10162.
Lee, V., & Burkam, D. (2002). Inequality at the starting gate:
Social background differences in achievement as children begin school.
Washington, DC: Economic Policy Institute.
Love, J. M., Kisker, E. E., Ross, C., Constantine, J., Boller, K.,
Chazan-Cohen, R., et al. (2005). The effectiveness of early Head Start
for 3-year-old children and their parents: Lessons for policy and
programs. Developmental Psychology, 41(6), 885-901.
Magnuson, K. A., Meyers, M. K., Ruhm, C. J., & Waldfogel, J.
(2004). Inequality in preschool education and school readiness. American
Educational Research Journal, 41(1), 115-157.
Magnuson, K. A., & Waldfogel, J. (2005). Early childhood care
and education: Effects on ethnic and racial gaps in school readiness.
The Future of Children, 15(1), 169-196.
Mardell-Czudnowski, C., & Goldenberg, D. S. (1998).
Developmental indicators for the assessment of learning (3rd ed.).
Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service.
McLanahan, S. (Ed.). (2005, Spring). School readiness: Closing
racial and ethnic gaps. The Future of Children, 15(1).
Morse, A. (2003). Language access: Helping non-English speakers
navigate health and human services. Washington, DC: National Conference
of State Legislatures Children's Policy Initiative.
Murphey, D. A., & Burns, C. E. (2002). Development of a
comprehensive community assessment of school readiness. Early Childhood
Research and Practice, 4(2). Retrieved March 17, 2007, from
http://ecrp.uiuc.edu/v4n2/murphey.html.
National Center for Education Statistics. (2005). National
assessment of educational progress (NAEP), various years, 1990-2005,
reading and mathematics assessments. Retrieved April 28, 2007, from
http://nces. ed.gov/programs/coe/2007/charts/chart14. asp?popup=true.
National Institute of Child Health and Human Development Early
Child Care Research Network. (2000). Characteristics and quality of
child care for toddlers and preschoolers. Applied Developmental Science,
4(3), 116-135.
National Institute of Child Health and Human Development Early
Child Care Research Network. (2002). Early child care and
children's development prior to school entry: Results from the
NICHD study of early child care. American Educational Research Journal,
39(1), 133-164.
National Institute of Child Health and Human Development Early
Child Care Research Network. (2003). Does amount of time spent in child
care predict socioemotional adjustment during the transition to
kindergarten? Child Development, 74(4), 976-1005.
National Institute of Child Health and Human Development Early
Child Care Research Network. (2004). Type of child care and
children's development at 54 months. Early Childhood Research
Quarterly, 19, 203-230.
National Institute of Child Health and Human Development Early
Child Care Research Network. (2005). Child care and child development:
Results from the NICHD Study of Early Child Care and Youth Development.
New York: The Guilford Press.
Noble, K. G., Tottenham, N., & Casey, B. J. (2005).
Neuroscience perspectives on disparities in school readiness and
cognitive achievement. The Future of Children, 15(1), 71-89.
Ou, S. (2005). Pathways of long-term effects of an early
intervention program on educational attainment: Findings from the
Chicago longitudinal study. Applied Developmental Psychology, 26,
578-611.
Peisner-Feinberg, E., Burchinal, M., Clifford, R., Culkin, M.,
Howes, C., Kagan, S., et al. (2001). The relation of preschool
child-care quality to children's cognitive and social developmental
trajectories through second grade. Child Development, 72(5), 1543-1553.
Peisner-Feinberg, E. S., Burchinal, M. R., Clifford, R. M., Culkin,
M. L., Howes, C., Kagan, S. L., et al. (2000). The children of the Cost,
Quality, and Outcomes Study go to school: Technical report. Chapel Hill,
NC: University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill, Frank Porter Graham Child Development Center.
Perlman, M., Zellman, G. L., & Le, V. (2004). Examining the
psychometric properties of the Early Childhood Environment Rating
Scale-Revised (ECERS-R). Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 19(3),
398-412.
Pianta, R. C. (2007). Preschool is school, sometimes. Education
Next, 7(1), 44-49.
Pigott, T. D., & Israel, M. S. (2005). Head Start
children's transition to kindergarten: Evidence from the early
childhood longitudinal study. Journal of Early Childhood Research, 3(1),
77-104.
Ryan, A. M. (2005). The effectiveness of the Manchester Even Start
program in improving literacy outcomes for preschool Latino students.
Journal of Research in Childhood Education, 20(1), 15-26.
Sammons, P., Elliot, K., Sylva, K., Melhuish, E., Siraj-Blatchford,
I., & Taggart, B. (2004). The impact of pre-school on young
children's cognitive attainments at entry to reception. British
Educational Research Journal, 30(5), 691-712.
Shonkoff, J. P. (2004). Evaluating early childhood services:
What's really behind the curtain. The Evaluation Exchange, 10(2),
3-4.
Shonkoff, J. P., & Phillips, D.A. (Eds.). (2000). From neurons
to neighborhoods: The science of early childhood development.
Washington, DC: National Academy Press.
United States Census Bureau. (2006). Nation's population
one-third minority [Press Release]. Washington, DC: Author.
Vandell, D. L. (2004). Early child care: The known and the unknown.
Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 50(3), 387-414.
Weigel, D. J., & Martin, S. S. (2006). Identifying key early
literacy and school readiness issues: Exploring a strategy for assessing
community needs. Early Childhood Research and Practice, 8(2). Retrieved
March 13, 2007, from http://ecrp.uiuc.edu/v8n2/weigel.html.
Wolery, M., Brashers, M. S., & Neitzel, J. C. (2002).
Ecological congruence assessment for classroom activities and routines:
Identifying goals and intervention practices in childcare. Topics in
Early Childhood Special Education, 22(3), 131-142.
Suzanne M. Winter
Raymond Zurcher
Arthur Hernandez
Zenong Yin
The University of Texas at San Antonio
Table 1
Mean ECERS-R Scores at Intervention Sites
Year 1
Fall Spring
Section Scores Mean (SD) Mean (SD)
Space/furnishings 4.9 (1.3) 4.7 (1.3)
Personal care 4.9 (1.8) 5.3 (1.4)
Language/reasoning 4.7 (1.3) 5.0 (1.5)
Activities 4.5 (1.3) 4.4 (1.2)
Interaction 5.3 (1.7) 5.7 (1.7)
Program structure 4.9 (1.9) 5.3 (1.6)
Parents and staff 5.3 (1.3) 5.2 (1.1)
Total 4.9 (1.3) 5.0 (1.1)
No. of observations 40 64
Year 2
Fall Spring
Section Scores Mean (SD) Mean (SD)
Space/furnishings 5.1 (1.1) 5.3 (1.1)
Personal care 5.0 (1.6) 5.5 (1.5)
Language/reasoning 5.0 (1.8) 5.2 (1.6)
Activities 4.5 (1.5) 4.9 (1.4)
Interaction 5.3 (1.8) 5.8 (1.5)
Program structure 5.2 (1.7) 5.7 (1.6)
Parents and staff 4.9 (1.5) 5.3 (1.2)
Total 4.9 (1.3) 5.3 (1.2)
No. of observations 55 81
Note: Scores on the ECERS-R range from 1.0 (inadequate) to 7.0
(excellent).
Table 2
DIAL-3 Scores at Comparison Site and Intervention Sites
Fall Spring
Comparison Sites, Year 1
Mean (SD) Mean (SD)
Motor 112.6 (13.2) 112.2 (11.6)
Concepts 96.7 (13.0) 95.8 (13.7)
Language 95.1 (13.8) 95.4 (14.0)
Overall Composite 101.6 (13.5) 101.6 (12.7)
Fall Spring
Intervention Sites, Year 1
Mean (SD) Mean (SD)
Motor 109.2 (14.9) 114.8 (16.0)
Concepts 97.0 (13.9) 99.4 (13.5)
Language 96.3 (14.1) 101.7 (16.0)
Overall Composite 100.6 (14.0) 106.0 (16.1)
Fall Spring
Intervention Sites, Year 2
Mean (SD) Mean (SD)
Motor 109.0 (15.9) 111.0 (16.4)
Concepts 95.8 (13.2) 98.4 (14.1)
Language 94.6 (14.7) 98.6 (14.2)
Overall Composite 100.2 (15.1) 103.1 (15.5)
Comparison Sites, Year 1
t Partial [[eta].sup.2]
Motor 0.15 .000
Concepts 0.31 .001
Language -0.08 .000
Overall Composite -0.03 .000
Intervention Sites, Year 1
t Partial [[eta].sup.2]
Motor -3.04 * .031
Concepts -1.43 .007
Language -3.03 * .031
Overall Composite -3.03 * .031
Intervention Sites, Year 2
t Partial [[eta].sup.2]
Motor -1.36 .004
Concepts -2.05 * .009
Language -2.96 * .019
Overall Composite -2.04 * .009
* p > .05
Note: The DIAL-3 standardization sample had a Mean of 100 and an
SD of 15.
Table 3
Posttest DIAL-3 Scores of Comparison and Intervention Sites
Comparison Intervention
Year 1 Sites Sites
Mean (SD) Mean (SD)
Motor 112.2 (11.6) 114.8 (16.0)
Concepts 95.8 (13.7) 99.4 (13.5)
Language 95.4 (14.0) 101.7 (16.0)
Overall composite 101.6 (12.7) 106.0 (16.1)
Sample size 32 127
Comparison Intervention
Year 2 Sites Sites
Mean (SD) Mean (SD)
Motor 112.2 (11.6) 111.0 (16.4)
Concepts 95.8 (13.7) 98.4 (14.1)
Language 95.4 (14.0) 98.6 (14.2)
Overall composite 101.6 (12.7) 103.1 (15.5)
Sample size 32 207
Year 1
t Partial [[eta].sup.2]
Motor -0.869 .005
Concepts -1.330 .011
Language -2.029 * .026
Overall composite -1.412 .013
Sample size
Year 2
t Partial [[eta].sup.2]
Motor 0.373 .000
Concepts -0.955 .004
Language -1.134 .006
Overall composite -0.502 .001
Sample size
* p < .05