Estimation and impact of gender differences in risk tolerance.
Neelakantan, Urvi
I. INTRODUCTION
Are women systematically less risk-tolerant than men? The answer
has critical implications for the financial well-being of women
(Schubert et al., 1999). For example, corporations may pass women over
for promotion if it is believed that they are unable to make risky
financial decisions (Johnson and Powell, 1994). Financial advisers,
believing that women are less risk tolerant, may recommend conservative
portfolios that have lower returns (Wang, 1994).
This paper uses a simple model of individual portfolio choice to
estimate the distributions of risk tolerance for women and men. Moments
of the model are matched to data on Individual Retirement Accounts
(IRAs) from the Health and Retirement Study (HRS) to obtain estimates of
the distributions' parameters. Under the assumption of constant
relative risk aversion (CRRA) utility, results show that the mean risk
tolerance is 0.25 for women and 0.28 for men. Economists generally
assume that risk aversion, the reciprocal of risk tolerance, falls in
the 0-10 range (Jagannathan and Kocherlakota, 1996). The estimates
obtained in this paper are consistent with this assumption.
The impact of the results is gauged in the context of gender
differences in wealth accumulation. Less risk-tolerant individuals are
more conservative investors. Conservative investments can lead to low
levels of wealth accumulation and may account for the gender gap in
wealth (Lyons et al., 2008). Data on older Americans from the HRS show
that men have 103% more wealth in their IRAs than do women. (1) The
results from this paper show that gender differences in risk tolerance
alone would lead to men accumulating 10% more wealth than women. Gender
differences in risk tolerance can thus account for nearly 10% of the gap
in wealth accumulation. By comparison, gender differences in earnings
can account for 51% of the wealth gap.
The results have implications for recent economic and policy
changes that are making individuals increasingly responsible for their
own financial security. Financial security depends greatly on the
ability to accumulate adequate retirement wealth (Wolff, 1998). The
results suggest that while the difference in risk tolerance does
contribute to the gender gap in wealth accumulation, the role of the
gender gap in earnings remains a primary concern.
II. LITERATURE REVIEW
Gender differences in risk tolerance have been observed in
responses to survey questions and risk-tolerance instruments. The
National Longitudinal Survey of Youth 1979 (NLSY79) and the HRS both
include a series of questions about choosing between two jobs, one that
pays respondents their current income with certainty and the other that
has a 50-50 chance of doubling their income or reducing it by a certain
fraction. Analyzing the HRS question, Barsky et al. (1997) found that
men tended to be somewhat more risk-tolerant than women. Spivey (2008)
corroborated this finding using the NLSY79. Grable and Lytton (1998) and
Sung and Hanna (1996) studied the responses to a subjective
risk-tolerance question in the Survey of Consumer Finances (SCF) and
concluded that women were significantly less risk-tolerant than men.
Grable (2000) used a used a 20-item instrument to assess risk tolerance
among faculty and staff at a university and also found that women were
less risk-tolerant than men.
Researchers have also made inferences about women's risk
tolerance based on observed wealth accumulation and investment choices.
Using SCF data, Jianakoplos and Bernasek (1998) found that as
individuals' wealth increased, the proportion of wealth held in
risky assets increased for both men and women, but the effect was
significantly smaller for single women. They concluded that single women
were less risk-tolerant than single men. Dwyer et al. (2002) found that
women took less risk than men in their mutual fund investment decisions.
However, the impact of gender on risk-taking was reduced significantly
when controlling for the investor's financial knowledge.
A related literature has studied gender differences in portfolio
allocation within retirement plans without necessarily attributing the
difference to risk tolerance. Bernasek and Shwiff (2001) collected data
from faculty members employed at five universities in Colorado and found
that women invested a significantly lower percentage of their defined
contribution plan in stocks. Using data from federal workers in a
government-sponsored Thrift Savings Plan, Hinz et al. (1997) found that,
compared to men, a smaller fraction of women participated in an equity
fund and invested a smaller share of their assets in that fund. Sunden
and Surette (1998) used SCF data to show that single women were less
likely than single men to invest their defined contribution plan mostly
in stocks. In contrast to these papers, Papke (1988) found no effect of
gender on investment choices in the National Longitudinal Survey (NLS)
of mature women. Rather, people who were able to choose their
investments put 14 percentage points more in stocks than people with no
choice.
The approach of this paper is to use observed gender differences in
portfolio allocation to estimate the distributions of risk tolerance for
men and women. The next section describes the theoretical model that is
used to derive the estimates.
III. THEORETICAL FRAMEWORK
The theoretical framework that is used to estimate the distribution
of risk tolerance follows the exposition of Jagannathan and Kocherlakota
(1996) of the classic Merton-Samuelson framework (Merton, 1969;
Samuelson, 1969).
In each time period t, individual i has wealth [w.sup.i.sub.t].
Individuals must decide how much of their wealth they wish to allocate
to a risk-free asset, [b.sup.i.sub.t], that earns a certain return,
[r.sup.m], and a risky asset, [s.sup.i.sub.t], that earns a stochastic return, [[??].sup.s.sub.t] ([theta]), where [theta] denotes the state of
nature. The objective of the individual is to maximize expected utility,
u([w.sup.i.sub.T]), where [w.sup.i.sub.T] is the wealth accumulated by
period T. The objective function can be written in this way--ignoring
the individual's consumption-savings decision--because it is
assumed that u(*) is a CRRA utility function, that returns
[[??].sup.s.sub.t] ([theta]) are independent and identically distributed
over time, and that there are no short-sale constraints. Under these
assumptions, the portfolio allocation decision is independent of the
consumption-savings decision (Samuelson, 1969).
The individual's problem in period t, given initial period
wealth, [w.sup.i.sub.0], and interest rates, [[??].sup.s.sub.t] and
[r.sup.m], is to choose savings in the risk-tree asset, [b.sup.i.sub.t],
and savings in the risky asset, [s.sup.i.sub.t], to solve
[MATHEMATICAL EXPRESSION NOT REPRODUCIBLE IN ASCII],
where [[gamma].sup.i] is the individual's risk aversion (i.e.,
1/[[gamma].sup.i] is risk tolerance).
[FIGURE 1 OMITTED]
Let [[rho].sup.i.sub.t] = [s.sup.i.sub.t]/[w.sup.i.sub.t] be the
share of the individual's wealth invested in the risky asset. It
can be shown that the individual's problem is the same every period
t and that the share of wealth he or she invests in the risky asset is a
constant [[rho].sup.i*], that solves (2)
(1) [MATHEMATICAL EXPRESSION NOT REPRODUCIBLE IN ASCII].
Though it cannot be solved analytically, Equation (1) shows that
[[rho].sup.i*] does not depend on wealth. In other words, given interest
rates, the individual's investment in the risky asset depends only
on his or her individual risk tolerance. The relationship is illustrated
in Figure 1 for values of risk aversion, [[gamma].sup.i], ranging from 1
to 10. It shows that as risk aversion increases, the individual chooses
to invest a smaller share of his or her wealth in the risky asset. (3)
Equation (1) can thus be used to calculate the unique optimal value of
[[rho].sup.i*] for a given [[gamma].sup.i].
IV. DATA
The data used in the estimation come from the 2006 wave of the HRS.
The HRS is a longitudinal study that has surveyed older Americans every
other year since 1992. (4) Using data on older Americans enables us to
look at realized wealth accumulation around retirement age.
The HRS has detailed information on financial assets, though most
of this information is collected at the household level. It reports the
amount of money in the household's three largest IRAs and also
reports to which household member these accounts belong. This paper
focuses on IRAs because they are the only component of wealth that can
be attributed to an individual rather than to a household, which may
consist of more than one person. In 2006 the HRS reported for the first
time the percentage of the IRA invested in stocks. In this paper, stocks
in IRAs are treated as the risky asset.
The HRS had 18,469 respondents in 2006, of whom 5,265 had an IRA.
Those who were missing information about the amount of money in their
IRA account or the percentage invested in stocks were dropped, yielding
a sample of 3,156 observations. Of these 1,553 were men and 1,603 were
women. Table 1 describes the sample. (5)
Table 2 shows the mean and standard deviation of the share of
stocks in IRA accounts for men and women. Men had an average of
$193,367.30 in their IRAs while women had an average of $95,037.01.
Observe that men hold a larger share of their IRA assets in stocks
compared to women. The estimation described in the next section will
attempt to match these key facts.
V. ESTIMATION AND RESULTS
The aim of this paper is to estimate the distribution of risk
tolerance separately for men and women. Let [[gamma].sup.i.sub.m], and
[[gamma].sup.i.sub.f] denote the risk aversion of an individual male and
female, respectively. Assume that risk tolerance (the reciprocal of risk
aversion) follows a lognormal distribution:
log 1/[[gamma].sup.i.sub.m] ~ N([[mu].sub.m],
[[sigma].sup.2.sub.m])
log 1/[[gamma].sup.i.sub.f] ~ N([[mu].sub.f],
[[sigma].sup.2.sub.f]).
Previous literature has shown that it is reasonable to assume that
risk tolerance is lognormally distributed because it restricts risk
tolerance to be non-negative and fits the data well (Kimball et al.,
2008).
The parameters of the lognormal distributions of
1/[[gamma].sup.i.sub.m] and 1/[[gamma].sup.i.sub.f] are derived using
the method of moments simulation. Specifically, the simulation is used
to find the values of [[mu].sub.m], [[sigma].sub.m], [[mu].sub.f], and
[[sigma].sub.f], to match the data in Table 2. The procedure used to
calculate the parameter values for females is described in detail below.
The procedure for males is identical.
The simulation begins by drawing values of [[gamma].sup.i.sub.f]
from a lognormal distribution with parameters [[mu].sub.f] and
[[sigma].sub.f] set to an arbitrary initial value. The value of
[[rho].sup.i*]. is calculated for each [[gamma].sup.i.sub.f] by
numerically solving Equation (1). (6) The mean and standard deviation of
the calculated values of [[rho].sup.i*]] are computed. The process is
repeated for a wide variety of values of [[mu].sub.f] and
[[sigma].sub.f] until the mean and variance of [[rho].sup.i*] in the
simulation match the data in the second row of Table 2.
The results are reported in Table 3. For men, the mean and standard
deviation of the underlying normal distribution were found to be
[[mu].sub.f] = -1.4661 and [[sigma].sub.f] = 0.5991. For women, the
corresponding values were [[mu].sub.f] = -1.5879 and [[sigma].sub.f] =
0.6603. The mean and median of risk tolerance for men and women are also
reported. The mean risk tolerance of women is lower than the mean risk
tolerance of men.
The importance of these results can be illustrated by estimating
their contribution to the gender gap in wealth accumulation. As reported
earlier, the average man in the HRS has over twice the amount of wealth
in his IRA compared to the average woman. How much of this gap can be
attributed to the gender difference in risk tolerance? This question can
be answered using sample simulations. First assume that both men and
women started out with the same amount of initial wealth,
[w.sup.i.sub.0]. Initial wealth can be thought of as the present
discounted value of future income at time 0. From Equation (1) we can
calculate that the average male, with a risk tolerance of 0.28, would
invest 64% of his wealth in stocks each period. We consider 10,000
different paths of stock returns and calculate the amount of wealth
accumulated over 38 years for each path. (7) Over a period of 38 years
(the average number of years that males in the HRS sample worked), men
would accumulate wealth equal to 6.5278 [w.sup.i.sub.0]. (8)
The same exercise is repeated for the average female with a risk
tolerance of 0.25, who would invest 59% of her wealth in stocks. Over a
period of 38 years, she would accumulate 5.9122 [w.sup.i.sub.0]. Men
thus accumulate 10% more wealth than women. Since the actual difference
in IRA wealth accumulation is 103%, the difference in risk tolerance
accounts for about 10% (10%/103 %) of the gap in wealth accumulation.
To get a sense of the relative magnitude of the role of risk
tolerance, the results are compared to the role played by the gender gap
in income. In the sample, the average earnings of men is approximately
53% more than the average earnings of women. (9) Now assume that both
men and women invest 64% of their initial wealth in stocks but
men's initial wealth, [w.sup.i.sub.0], is 53% more than
women's wealth, 0.655 [w.sup.i.sub.0]. This is appropriate if we
think of initial wealth as the present discounted value of future
earnings. From the simulation, men would accumulate 6.5278
[w.sup.i.sub.0] as before, while women would accumulate 4.2705
[w.sup.i.sub.0]. The gap persists, which means men end up with 53% more
wealth than women. The gender gap in income thus accounts for 51%
(53%/103%) of the gender gap in wealth.
Combining both the gender gap in income and in risk tolerance, we
find that women would accumulate 3.8678 [w.sup.i.sub.0]. The exercise
thus accounts for two-thirds of the actual gender gap in wealth. Other
factors such as differences in the amount of time worked, which are
outside the scope of the model, could account for the rest.
VI. CONCLUSION
This paper used a simple theoretical model to infer
individuals' risk tolerance from observed investment choices.
Parameters of the distribution of risk tolerance for men and women were
estimated by matching moments of the model to data on asset allocation in IRAs. On average, women were found to be less risk-tolerant than men.
The estimates were used to measure the impact of risk tolerance on the
gender gap in wealth accumulation. The gender difference in risk
tolerance accounted for 10% of the gap in accumulated wealth. The gender
difference in earnings was found to have a more substantial impact,
accounting for 51% of the gap in accumulated wealth.
The findings have implications for policies that are making
individuals increasingly responsible for managing their own retirement
savings. The results suggest that women might optimally choose to invest
a smaller fraction of their wealth in risky assets, which might slightly
exacerbate the gender gap in wealth. However, the effect of the gender
difference in earnings on the gender gap in wealth is likely to remain a
primary concern.
It is important to acknowledge a couple of limitations of this
study. First, the sample is restricted to older Americans and previous
research has shown that risk preferences may change with age. For
example, Dohmen et al. (2005) find that risk tolerance decreases with
age. Thus, it may not be possible to generalize the estimates from this
study to the U.S. population as a whole. Second, the role of factors
such as knowledge and education are outside the scope of the theoretical
framework used in this paper. As a result, estimates of the preference
parameter may be capturing not just gender differences in risk aversion
but, more generally, differences in the taste for stocks. A more
comprehensive look at the factors that affect earnings, risk aversion,
and hence wealth accumulation is left for future research.
ABBREVIATIONS
CRRA: Constant Relative Risk Aversion
HRS: Health and Retirement Study
IRAs: Individual Retirement Accounts
NLS: National Longitudinal Survey
NLSY79: National Longitudinal Survey of Youth 1979
SCF: Survey of Consumer Finances
doi: 10.1111/j.1465-7295.2009.00251.x
REFERENCES
Barsky, R. B., F. T. Juster, M. S. Kimball, and M. D. Shapiro.
"Preference Parameters and Behavioral Heterogeneity: An
Experimental Approach in The Health and Retirement Study."
Quarterly Journal of Economics, 112(2), 1997, 537-79.
Bernasek, A., and S. Shwiff. "Gender, Risk, and
Retirement." Journal of Economic Issues, 35(2), 2001, 345-56.
Dohmen, T., A. Falk, D. Huffman U. Sunde, J. Schupp, and W. Wagner.
"Individual Risk Attitudes: New Evidence From a Large,
Representative, Experimentally-Validated Survey." IZA Discussion
Paper No. 1730.
Dwyer, P. D., J. H. Gilkenson, and J. A. List. "Gender
differences in Revealed Risk Taking, Evidence from Mutual Fund
Investors." Economics Letters, 76(2), 2002, 151-58.
Grable, J. E. (2000). "Financial Risk Tolerance and Additional
Factors That Affect Risk Taking in Everyday Money Matters." Journal
of Business and Psychology, 14(4), 2000, 625-30.
Grable, J. E., and R. H. Lytton. "Investor Risk Tolerance:
Testing the Efficacy of Demographics as Differentiating and Classifying
Factors." Financial Counseling and Planning, 9(1), 1998, 61-74.
Hinz, R. P., D. D. McCarthy, and J. A. Turner. "Are Women
Conservative Investors? Gender Differences in Participant-Directed
Pension Investments," in Positioning Pensions for the Twenty-first
Century, edited by M. S. Gordon, O. S. Mitchell, and M. M. Twinney.
Philadelphia: University of Pennsylvania Press, 1997, 91-103.
Jagannathan, R., and N. R. Kocherlakota. "Why Should Older
People Invest Less in Stocks than Younger People?" Federal Reserve
Bank of Minneapolis Quarterly Review, 20(3), 1996, 11-23.
Jianakoplos, N. A., and A. Bernasek. "Are Women More Risk
Averse?" Economic Inquiry, 36(4), 1998, 620-30.
Johnson, J. E. V., and P. L. Powell. "Decision Making, Risk
and Gender: Are Managers Different?" British Journal of Management,
5(2), 1994, 123-38.
Kimball, M. S., C. R. Sahm, and M. D. Shapiro. "Imputing Risk
Tolerance from Survey Responses." Journal of the American
Statistical Association, 103(483), 2008, 1028-38.
Lyons, A., U. Neelakantan, and E. Scherpf. "Gender and Marital
Differences in Wealth and Investment Decisions." Journal of
Personal Finance, 6(4), 2008, 57-75.
Merton, R. C. "Lifetime Portfolio Selection under Uncertainty:
The Continuous-Time Case." Review of Economics and Statistics,
51(3), 1969, 247-57.
Papke, L. "How are Participants Investing Their Accounts in
Participant-Directed Individual Account Pension Plans?" American
Economic Review, 88(2), 1988, 212-16.
Samuelson, P. A. "Lifetime Portfolio Selection by Dynamic
Stochastic Programming." Review of Economics and Statistics, 51(3),
1969, 239-46.
Schubert, R., M. Brown, M. Gysler, and H. W. Brachinger.
"Financial Decision-making: Are Women Really More Risk Averse? The
American Economic Review Papers and Proceedings, 89(2), 1999, 381-85.
Spivey, C. "Desperation or Desire? The Role of Risk Aversion
in Marriage." Economic Inquiry, forthcoming,
http://www3.interscience.wiley.com/journal/
121506373/abstract?CRETRY=1&SRETRY=0.
Sunden, A. E., and B. J. Surette. "Gender Differences in the
Allocation Of Assets in Retirement Savings Plans." American
Economic Review, 88(2), 1998, 207-11.
Sung, J., and S. Hanna. "Factors Related to Risk
Tolerance." Financial Counseling and Planning, 7, 1996, 11-20.
Wang, P. "Brokers Still Treat Men Better Than Women."
Money, 23(6), 1994, 108-10.
Wolff, E. N. "Recent Trends in the Size Distribution of
Household Wealth. Journal of Economic Perspectives, 12(3), 1998, 131-50.
(1.) This is in line with previous estimates. For example, Johnson
and Powell (1994) found that even among full-time workers approaching
retirement age with pension coverage, men had 76% greater pension wealth
than had women.
(2.) See Jagannathan and Kocherlakota (1996).
(3.) Note that it is possible for the share of the risky asset in
the portfolio to exceed 100% because the risk-free asset can be used to
borrow money to finance this in the first period.
(4.) The HRS is sponsored by the National Institute of Aging (grant
number NIA U01AG009740) and is conducted by the University of Michigan.
(5.) It would have been ideal to analyze the portfolio allocation
of never-married persons alone to ensure that allocations were not
influenced by the spouse's preferences. However, as Table 1 shows,
there are insufficient cases in the data to be able to do this.
(6.) For computational simplicity, a stylized distribution of
returns is chosen in the solution to Equation (1). It is assumed that
the return on the risk-free asset, [r.sup.b] is fixed 1% and the return
on risky assets, [r.sup.s.sub.t], is either 27.03, 13, or -15.25% with
equal probability. This yields a mean return of 8.26% with a standard
deviation of 17.58%, which corresponds to the S&P 500 for 1871-2004.
The return data are taken from
http://www.econ.yale.edu/shiller/data.htm.
(7.) As before, it is assumed that the realized stock return each
period is 27.03, 13, or -15.25% with equal probability.
(8.) This, and the results reported hereafter, is the median of the
10,000 simulated paths.
(9.) The ratio of men's to women's average total income
is also the same.
Neelakantan: Department of Agricultural and Consumer Economics,
University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign, Urbana, IL 61801. Phone
1-217-333-0479, Fax 1-217-333-5538, E-mail urvi@illinois.edu
TABLE 1
Descriptive Statistics
Men Women
(N = 1553) (N = 1603)
Age 67.2 65.3
Marital Status (%)
Married/Partnered 85.1 66.4
Divorced/Separated 7.1 11.5
Widowed 5.3 19.0
Never Married/Unknown 2.5 3.1
Race (%n)
White 94.4 93.1
Black 3.0 3.8
Other 2.6 3.1
Education (%)
< 12 years 7.6 2.0
12 years 25.8 12.8
13-15 years 20.5 15.9
16 years 20.3 20.4
> 16 years 25.2 21.8
Unknown 0.6 27.1
TABLE 2 Share of Stocks in IRAs
Mean Standard Deviation
Men 64.4% 41.8%
Women 59.5% 44.0%
TABLE 3
Estimated Parameter Values
Risk Tolerance
[mu] [sigma] Mean Median
Men -1.4661 0.5991 0.2749 0.2308
Women -1.5879 0.6603 0.2541 0.2044