Birth order and risky adolescent behavior.
Argys, Laura M. ; Rees, Daniel I. ; Averett, Susan L. 等
I. INTRODUCTION
There is a widespread belief that birth order is an important
determinant of personality, intelligence, and economic success. This
belief is supported by a number of recently published popular books,
each with its own approach to the topic. Leman (2001, 16-18), for
instance, argues that firstborns "are more highly motivated to
achieve than later borns" and as a consequence "often fill
positions of high authority or achievement." In contrast, Wallace (1999, 18), worries that firstborns "go through life feeling like
they cannot measure up to the high standards their parents expected ...
lacking in confidence they might drop out and refuse to compete
altogether." (1)
Although there is an intuitive appeal to some of these theories,
the weight of evidence suggests that birth-order effects of this nature
either do not exist or are difficult to measure using the standard
approaches of social scientists. A large number of empirical studies have examined the effect of birth order on test scores, education, and
earnings--see Olneck and Bills (1979), Blake (1981), Hauser and Sewell (1983), Behrman and Taubman (1986), Kessler (1991), and Hanushek (1992).
Taken together their results suggest that, after properly controlling
for family size, birth order explains little variation in these
conventional measures of success.
Another, more recent strand of research in this area examines
whether the sex composition of an individual's siblings affects
academic achievement as measured by years of education. In a well-known
article, Butcher and Case (1994) present evidence that females raised
with brothers go on to receive more education, on average, than that of
females raised with at least one sister. However, even this relatively
modest, albeit unexpected finding has been called into question by
subsequent studies; note Kaestner (1997) and Hauser and Kuo (1998).
Using data on adolescents aged 12 through 17, we investigate the
relationship between birth order and a number of behaviors that have
been, for the most part, ignored in this literature. These behaviors
include the probability that an individual smoked cigarettes, used
substances such as marijuana or alcohol, engaged in sexual activity, or
committed any one of a series of crimes. In contrast to what most
empirical studies have found, we find strong evidence that birth order
is related to many of the outcomes under study. We also show that at
least one birth-order difference can last into early adulthood, if not
beyond. Although it is difficult to isolate the precise routes through
which birth order affects the behavior of adolescents, our results are
consistent with the idea that peer influences play an important role.
II. WHY BIRTH-ORDER EFFECTS MIGHT EXIST
Researchers working in this area have long speculated that birth
order might be related to child outcomes through parental investments in
their offspring. Becker and Tomes (1976) posit a model in which parents
devote more financial resources to children with lower innate ability.
(2) If, as suggested by Behrman and Taubman (1986) and Kessler (1991),
genetic endowments favor earlier-born children, this model would predict
offsetting financial investments in later-borns. Under the assumption of
"imperfect capital markets," Birdsall (1991) predicts greater
expenditures on later-born children as family income rises and older
siblings become financially independent, but the author also notes that
firstborns do not initially have to compete for their parents' time
and attention. In a frequently cited piece in the psychology literature,
Zajonc (1976) likewise emphasizes the early period in a firstborn's
cognitive development when parental attention is undivided. (3) In fact,
most researchers working on birth-order issues have assumed that
children are particularly sensitive to parental time investments and the
home environment at young ages. (4) However, certain nonmonetary
parental inputs, such as monitoring and supervision, may become
increasingly important as a child matures, especially in the
determination of risky or delinquent behaviors.
Sibling interactions represent another possible route through which
birth order might be related to child behavior and subsequent
achievement. For instance, older siblings could act as positive role
models, their achievements adopted as goals and their failures serving
as cautionary examples. (5) Older siblings might also act as caregivers
or authority figures, especially when one of the parents is absent from
the family unit. Alternatively, older siblings may serve as negative
role models or even purposely introduce their younger brothers and
sisters to certain behaviors earlier than would otherwise be the case.
(6) In addition, having an older sibling may provide more opportunities
to interact with, and perhaps copy the behavior of, a different set of
friends. This could be especially important when the sex of the sibling
is different or when there is a substantial age gap between siblings
and, as a consequence, the behavior of the older sibling's peers is
markedly different from the behavior of the younger sibling's
peers; on this point, see Rodgers et al. (1992). (7)
These possibilities suggest that researchers searching for
birth-order differences might fruitfully focus attention on adolescent risky behavior as opposed to earnings, education, or test scores. If
birth order is indeed related to such behavior, it could have important
implications for the health and well-being of adults as well as teens.
For instance, if having an older sibling increases the likelihood that
teens become sexually active, then birth order could be associated with
marriage match quality. Or, if older siblings expose younger family
members to the use of alcohol, marijuana, or tobacco, then birth order
could lead to long-term health and work-related problems, especially if
the substance use persists into adulthood.
III. PAST EMPIRICAL STUDIES
Early empirical studies in this area often confused the effects of
family size and birth order. (8) These effects are interrelated because,
to use an example offered by Kessler (1991), children from larger
families are more likely to be middle borns as opposed to firstborns or
last borns. One of the first studies to explicitly address this
confusion was that of Olneck and Bills (1979). They examined the effect
of birth order and family size on tests scores, education, occupation,
and earnings. They found that, after controlling for family size, birth
order was no longer a statistically significant predictor of these
measures of achievement. However, Olneck and Bills (1979) were unable to
include siblingless children in their analysis and entered family size
linearly, a functional form assumption that has been criticized as being
overly restrictive--see Kessler (1991).
Subsequent researchers corrected these flaws but, by and large,
produced similar results. For example, neither Blake (1981), Hauser and
Sewell (1983), nor Hanushek (1992) found evidence that birth order was
related to educational achievement. Behrman and Taubman (1986) found
that, controlling for family size, birth order was unrelated to
earnings, although their results did suggest a negative relationship
between birth order and years of education for women (but not for men).
Kessler (1991) found no effect of birth order on either the level or
growth rate of earnings.
More recently, researchers have turned their attention to examining
the effects of sibling sex composition on measures of achievement.
Perhaps the best-known study in this vein is by Butcher and Case (1994).
Using a variety of data sources, these authors show that women raised
with only brothers went on to receive significantly more education than
women raised with at least one sister. This finding, however, is not
easily explained. In fact, some economic models suggest that females
raised with brothers should receive less education than those raised
with sisters. Moreover, the reliability of Butcher and Case's
empirical work has been called into question by subsequent authors.
Kaestner (1997), replicated their analysis using data from a more recent
cohort but could only find a similar effect for black women. Hauser and
Kuo (1998) argued that Butcher and Case's findings were "no
more than suggestive" and found little evidence that sex
composition affects educational outcomes in an analysis of three
nationally representative data sets.
A handful of studies by psychologists have examined the effect of
birth order on outcomes similar to those used in this research. Using
data from the National Longitudinal Survey of Youth--1979 (NLSY79),
Rodgers et al. (1992) examined the effect of birth order and sibling sex
composition on age at first intercourse. They found that younger
siblings tended to have first intercourse earlier than older siblings,
but the authors excluded siblingless children from their calculations as
well as individuals who became sexually active after the age of 18. In
addition, although Rodgers et al. controlled for family size, they did
not include other family background measures in their analysis.
Using data on male college students, Brook et al. (1991) found that
the characteristics and actions of older brothers were correlated with
the probability that their younger brothers experimented with drugs.
This result, however, is subject to at least two interpretations. It is
possible that the behavior of older brothers directly influences their
younger brothers' drug use, but it is also possible that this
correlation can be attributed to difficult-to-measure environmental
factors that exert similar influences on the behavior of both siblings.
Such factors could include parental attitudes toward substance use or
the general level of parental supervision within the family. (9)
IV. DATA AND EMPIRICAL MODEL
Our primary data source is the National Longitudinal Survey of
Youth-1997 Cohort (NLSY97). The NLSY97 was launched to enable
researchers to define "the transition that today's youths make
from school to the labor market and into adulthood." In addition to
including schooling and labor market information, the NLSY97 contains
detailed questions on family background, personal characteristics, and a
variety of behaviors that can be considered risky or delinquent.
Respondents to the NLSY97 have been surveyed on an annual basis
beginning in 1997. The initial wave of the NLSY97 consisted of a
representative sample of the U.S. population aged 12-16 years on
December 31, 1996 (n = 6,748), coupled with a supplemental oversample of
black and Hispanic adolescents (n = 2,236). The current study uses data
from respondents between the ages of 12 and 17 who were interviewed at
least once during the first three waves of the NSLY97. (10) Respondents
could contribute up to three observations to the analysis. (11)
Table 1 presents unweighted means for our outcome variables by
gender and the presence of an older sibling. Information on tobacco,
marijuana, and alcohol consumption; sexual activity; birth control; and
criminal/delinquent behaviors was collected using ACASI (Audio Computer
Assisted Self-interview) technology. This technology allowed
participants to read sensitive questions on a laptop screen or listen to
them on headphones and then indicate their responses electronically. It
was adopted by the NLSY97 with the goal of minimizing the potential
influence of family members and interviewers.
To examine the relationship between birth order and the outcomes
listed, we estimate the following baseline model, which focuses on the
role of older siblings:
(1) [R.sup.*.sub.i] = [alpha][S.sub.i] [gamma]'[F.sub.i] +
[beta]'[X.sub.i]+ [[epsilon].sub.i],
where [R.sup.*.sub.i] is an adolescent's propensity to engage
in a particular risky activity, for example, substance use; [S.sub.i] is
a dichotomous variable equal to 1 if the adolescent has an older
sibling; [F.sub.i] is a vector of controls for family size; and
[X.sub.i] represents controls such as age, race, and measures of
socioeconomic status. The variable [R.sup.*.sub.i] is latent, but when
[R.sup.*.sub.i] > 0, an indicator variable, [R.sub.i], can be seen to
equal 1 so that Prob ([R.sub.i] = 1) = Prob([alpha][S.sub.i] +
[gamma]'[F.sub.i] + [[beta]'[X.sub.i] + [[epsilon].sub.i] >
0). If the error term of Equation 1 is normally distributed, then the
result is a standard univariate probit model.
Our principal interest is in the coefficient of [S.sub.i]. An
estimate of [alpha] greater than zero would indicate that children with
older siblings--in other words, middle borns and last borns--are more
likely to engage in the risky behavior under study than are firstborns,
holding family size, age, and the other factors in [X.sub.i] constant.
An estimated coefficient less than zero would suggest that middle borns
and last borns are less likely to engage in this risky behavior than
firstborns.
The presence of an older sibling was ascertained using the NLSY97
household and non-household family rosters. These rosters also provide
information on the age and gender of a respondent's siblings.
Although the baseline model does not take advantage of this additional
information, we incorporate more detailed sibling characteristics when
we extend the baseline model below.
The vector [F.sub.i] consists of four dichotomous variables,
[F.sub.1] through [F.sub.4], where [F.sub.1] is equal to 1 for
siblingless children, [F.sub.2] is equal to 1 for two-child families,
and so forth. The omitted category is families with five or more
children. Notice that in this specification firstborn and siblingless
children are treated as separate categories. Notice too that there are
no interactions between birth order and family size in this
specification, although we relax this restriction later in our analyses.
(12)
The vector [X.sub.i] contains a variety of individual
characteristics, such as the respondent's age, race, and ethnicity,
as well as socioeconomic indicators (the education level of the
youth's most educated parent and a classification of family income
relative to the poverty level). The unweighted means of these variables
and a continuous measure of family size are reported by gender in Table
2 for the full sample, the subsample consisting of observations
contributed by respondents with an older sibling, and the subsample
consisting of observation contributed by firstborns and siblingless
children.
As expected, we observe clear differences in family characteristics
when we compare the subsamples. For example, family size tends to be
larger in the older-sibling sampies, whereas parental education tends to
be smaller. Because these and other factors may affect the probability
that an adolescent engages in risky or delinquent behaviors, it is
important that they be included in the vector [X.sub.i] if one is
interested in obtaining accurate estimates of the effects of birth
order.
V. RESULTS
Determinants of Substance Use
Table 3 reports estimated marginal probabilities and robust
standard errors for the baseline probit model in which three outcomes
are considered: the probability of adolescents' ever having smoked
cigarettes, the probability of their ever having consumed alcohol, and
the probability of their ever having used marijuana. (13) Results for
males and females are presented separately to allow for gender
differences in the relationship between birth order and substance use.
In keeping with published studies by the National Center on
Addiction and Substance Abuse (2002) and Pacula et al. (2001), we find
strong evidence of race and ethnicity effects. Being black is associated
with a lower probability of substance use across all six estimations
presented in Table 3, and the marginal effect of the Hispanic indicator
is always negative but fails to reach conventional levels of statistical
significance in two cases. In addition, we find that adolescents who
lived with both of their parents are much less likely to have used
tobacco, alcohol, or marijuana, whereas household income seems to have
an effect on female, but not male, substance use. Growing up in an urban
area is associated with an increase in the probability of having smoked
marijuana and, for females only, an increase in the probability of
having drunk alcohol.
As noted, the baseline model includes only a single measure of
birth order: the dichotomous variable [S.sub.i]. However, this simple
specification produces striking results. We find that children with
older siblings are, on average, more likely to have used tobacco,
alcohol, and marijuana than their firstborn counterparts controlling for
family size, age, and the other factors in the model. For instance,
females with older siblings are 8 percentage points more likely to have
smoked cigarettes than are their firstborn counterparts. Males with
older siblings are 6.2 percentage points more likely to have drunk
alcohol and are 5.1 percentage points more likely to have tried
marijuana than their firstborn counterparts.
A positive relationship between older siblings and substance use is
not consistent with the argument that experienced parents provide better
guidance for their younger children nor with the notion that older
siblings serve as positive role models. This result is more in keeping
with the argument that adolescents with older siblings are
"prematurely" exposed to behaviors initiated by their older
siblings at later ages, although it could reflect differences in
parental monitoring and supervision or even a beneficial effect of
having younger siblings. (14)
In general, family size, as measured by the number of siblings in
the household, seems to have little influence on the outcomes examined.
The estimated family-size effects in Table 3 are never significant for
males, nor is family size a good predictor of smoking or marijuana use
among females. (15) However, there is evidence that family size is
negatively related to the probability of female alcohol use. (16)
Table 4 presents estimates of Equation 1 in which an alternative
set of substance use measures are employed. Specifically, it examines
the relationship between birth order and substance use in the past 30
days. Although not shown, the full set of regressors are included.
Again, the results strongly suggest that children with older siblings
are more likely than their firstborn counterparts to have used tobacco,
alcohol, and marijuana.
Determinants of Sexual Behavior
Table 5 presents estimates of the relationship between birth order
and adolescent sexual behavior. Two dependent variables are considered:
the first is an indicator of adolescents ever having had intercourse,
whereas the second is an indicator of whether they used contraception at
first intercourse and it applies only to the subsample of respondents
who indicated that they had had sexual intercourse.
The results presented in Table 5 suggest that black males were more
likely to have had sex than their white or Hispanic counterparts the
opposite is true for Hispanic females. Parental characteristics and
socioeconomic status are also important determinants of sexual behavior.
Having an older mother is associated with a decrease in the probability
of having had intercourse, and living with both parents (as opposed to
living in a nontraditional family) sharply reduces this same probability
for both sexes. Higher levels of parental education are associated with
a reduction in the probability of having had intercourse: an additional
year of education is associated with a 0.9 to 1.7 percentage point
reduction in this probability.
For both males and females, we see substantial (7.4 and 4.0
percentage points, respectively) increases associated with the presence
of an older sibling in the probability of having had intercourse. This
result might indicate that middle borns and last borns are at higher
risk of experiencing unwanted pregnancies and contracting sexually
transmitted diseases. However, there exists the possibility that older
siblings could mitigate these negative consequences by introducing their
younger siblings to responsible methods of birth control. Using a
subsample of respondents who had had intercourse, the second column in
Table 5 presents estimates of the effect of birth order on the
probability that contraception was used at first intercourse. They
provide little evidence that birth order is related to this probability.
In Table 6 we assess the impact of birth order on current sexual
behavior, using two alternative outcome measures: whether the respondent was sexually active during the past year and, for sexually active
respondents, the probability of using birth control during the past
year. Although not shown, the full set of regressors are employed. The
results of this exercise are similar to those presented in Table 5 in
that they suggest that younger brothers and sisters are more likely to
be sexually active than their firstborn counterparts. The results also
indicate that having an older sibling decreases the probability that
sexually active females used birth control in the past year. There is no
evidence of a corresponding effect for male adolescents.
Determinants of Criminal and Delinquent Activities
In Table 7 we examine the relationship between older siblings and
the probability that an adolescent ever participated in a variety of
illegal or delinquent activities, including carrying a handgun, being a
member of a gang, stealing, destroying property, and assault.
Holding other factors constant, blacks and Hispanics are often less
likely to report having engaged in these activities than whites,
although blacks are more likely to have committed assault than their
white counterparts, and black males are more likely to have been a
member of a gang. Being raised by two parents is associated with a lower
probability of engaging in a number of criminal/delinquent activities,
whereas living in an urban area is associated with an increase in the
probability of committing assault and increases in the probability that
males belonged to a gang, destroyed property, and stole. (17)
Table 7 contains evidence that birth order is related to delinquent
activities, but the estimated marginal probabilities are not always
significant at conventional levels. We find that males with older
siblings are more likely to have stolen and carried a handgun as
compared to their firstborn counterparts. Females with older siblings
are also more likely to have carried a handgun but are actually less
likely to have stolen.
Table 8 examines the determinants of criminal and delinquent
behavior in the past year. The results are similar to those reported in
Table 7: once again we find that males with older siblings are more
likely to have stolen and carried a handgun as compared to their
firstborn counterparts and that females with older siblings are more
likely to have carried a handgun. The negative relationship between
stealing and having an older sibling for females disappears in Table 8,
replaced with a positive relationship between destroying property and
having an older sibling.
Extensions of the Model
The baseline empirical model only captures differences in behavior
between firstborns and younger siblings and does not allow for the
possibility, as some theories suggest, that sibling gender composition
and age may matter. In this section we address these issues and others
by extending the baseline model in a number of ways.
The first extension, labeled Model 1 in Tables 9 and 10, examines
the role of older sibling gender. Specifically, we allow the
oldersibling effect to differ according to whether the respondent had an
older sister or brother by estimating,
(2) [R.sup.*.sub.i] = [[alpha].sub.1][OB.sub.i] +
[[alpha].sub.2][0S.sub.i] + [[gamma]'[F.sub.i] +
[[beta]'[X.sub.i] + [[epsilon].sub.i],
where [OB.sub.i] is a dichotomous variable equal to 1 if respondent
i had an older brother and [OS.sub.i] is a dichotomous variable equal to
1 if respondent i had an older sister. (18) The estimated effect of
having an older sister is often larger than that of having an older
brother, but the marginal probabilities associated with [OB.sub.i] and
[OS.sub.i] are statistically different at the .10 level in only 3 out of
18 estimations, suggesting that sibling sex composition is less
important than simple birth order. (19)
The second extension, labeled Model 2 in Tables 9 and 10, tests if
the relationship between older siblings and risky/delinquent behavior
depends on birth spacing. Specifically, we estimate,
(3) [R.sup.*.sub.i] = [[alpha].sub.1][S3.sub.i] +
[[alpha].sub.2][S4.sub.i] + [[gamma]'[F.sub.i] +
[[beta]'[X.sub.i] + [[epsilon].sub.i],
where [S3.sub.i] is a dichotomous variable equal to 1 if respondent
i had a sibling zero to three years older and [S4.sub.i] is a
dichotomous variable equal to 1 if respondent i had a sibling four or
more years older. (20) The results indicate that an age gap of four or
more years can lead to an increased risk of substance use and sexual
activity. In fact, per Table 9 the estimated effect associated with an
age gap of four or more years is always significantly greater than that
associated with a gap of zero to three years. Some evidence suggests
that birth spacing may be important in the determination of female
delinquent/criminal behavior, but the marginal probabilities are not
significantly different for most of the outcomes examined in Table 10.
(21)
In Model 3 we expand our search for birth-order effects by adopting
a more flexible specification in which the older-sibling variable is
replaced by a set of dichotomous variables indicating if the respondent
was second-, third-, fourth-, or fifth-born. (22) The results of this
exercise reveal that later-born children are especially prone to
engaging in risky and delinquent behaviors. For instance, having been
born second is associated with a .09 increase in the probability that
males ever smoked, but having been born fifth is associated with a .180
increase in this probability. To take another example, having been born
second is associated with a statistically insignificant increase of .025
in the probability that females had sexual intercourse, but having been
born fifth is associated with a .133 increase in this probability. (23)
It is tempting to view these results as evidence that additional older
siblings increase an adolescent's exposure to risky and delinquent
behaviors. However, it is possible that early-borns benefit from extra
parental supervision when there is a last-born child in the household.
(24)
In Model 4 we investigate if the effect of birth order varies
systematically according to family size. Specifically, we show the
results of running separate regressions by family size, using the
birth-order measures from Model 3. The estimated effects from these
regressions, although less precise, indicate that middle-born and
later-born children are especially prone to engaging in risky and
delinquent behaviors across the range of family sizes examined. However,
the data provide only limited evidence that birth order and family size
interact in a significant fashion. We cannot reject the hypothesis that
the effect of having been born second is equal across two-, three-, and
four-child families in 14 out of 18 cases. Nor can we reject the
hypothesis that the effect of having been born third is equal across
three- and four-child families in 12 out of 18 cases. (25)
VI. BIRTH-ORDER DIFFERENCES IN AN EARLIER COHORT: EVIDENCE FROM THE
NLSY79
In this section we investigate the relationship between birth order
and risky behavior using an alternative data set, the NLSY79. (26)
Examining an earlier cohort serves two purposes: first, we can confirm
if birth-order differences such as those documented in Table 3 existed
for adolescents growing up in the 1970s; second, and perhaps more
important, we can explore if these effects typically lasted past
adolescence. Evidence of effects on adult behavior would suggest that
birth order can have long-run health consequences and point to
heretofore overlooked economic consequences.
The upper panel of Table 11 presents estimates of the effects of
birth order on substance use and sexual activity for individuals between
the ages of 14 and 21 in 1979. Although the results are not as strong
those that we found using the NLSY97, it is nevertheless clear that
birth order was an important correlate of risky behavior for this
earlier cohort. According to the probit estimates presented in Table 11,
males with older siblings were 5.6 percentage points more likely to
smoke than their firstborn counterparts, and females with older siblings
were 4.5 percentage points more likely to smoke. Likewise, the estimated
coefficient of the older-sibling variable is significant and positive in
the male marijuana and sexual-activity equations as well as in the
female alcohol equation. (27)
The bottom panel of Table 11 presents estimates of the relationship
between birth order and tobacco, marijuana, and alcohol use in 1992,
when the NLSY79 respondents were between the ages of 27 and 34. The data
suggest that birth order is not related to the drinking behavior of
young adults. However, there is strong evidence that having an older
sibling is positively related to the probability that young adults
smoke, and some evidence to suggest that males with older sibling were
more likely to use marijuana as young adults, although this result is
not quite significant at conventional levels.
It is not surprising that birth order seems to have a
longer-lasting influence on smoking as compared to that on drinking or
marijuana use. Cigarettes are highly addictive, and, as Gruber and
Zinman (2001) have shown, most adult smokers begin their habit as
adolescents. The relationship between having an older sibling and
smoking as an adult suggests that birth order should be related to
easily measurable long-run health outcomes such as longevity.
VII. CONCLUSION
A general belief holds that birth order is a key determinant of an
individual's personality and attainments. However, academic
researchers have typically been unable to link birth order to
quantifiable measures of success such as educational attainment and
earnings. Here we attempt to measure the relationship between birth
order and substance use, sexual behavior, and criminal/delinquent
activities during adolescence. Our results provide the strongest
empirical evidence to date that birth order is related to behavior.
Our primary data source is the first round of the NLSY97, which
provides detailed information on a large sample of adolescents between
the ages of 12 and 17. Estimating standard univariate probit models, and
controlling for family size and other factors such as parental education
and income, we conclude that adolescents with older siblings are more
likely to have used tobacco, alcohol, and marijuana, and are more likely
to have had sexual intercourse than their firstborn counterparts. In
addition, the evidence suggests that male adolescents with older
siblings are more likely to steal as compared to firstborns, and female
adolescents with older siblings are more likely to destroy property.
Having an older sibling is also associated with carrying a gun for both
males and females.
A number of plausible explanations exist for these results. It may
be that older siblings purposely introduce their younger brothers and
sisters to behaviors that they otherwise would not have initiated or
that younger siblings are simply mimicking the behavior of their older
siblings. Our findings are also consistent with the idea that parents
invest less time and energy into supervising their younger offspring,
perhaps because of diminishing returns to parenting or because they have
less energy to spend on their younger offspring. (28) However, our
results are not consistent with the hypothesis that older siblings serve
as positive role models or that experience leads to better parenting
skills.
To further explore processes through which having an older sibling
might be related to behavior, we extend our model to take into account
more detailed sibling characteristics. Although we find only limited
evidence of sibling sex composition effects, the evidence suggests that
birth spacing is important. Having a sibling who was four or more years
older is associated with a larger impact on risky behavior than having a
sibling who was zero to three years older.
In addition, we find that later-born children are especially prone
to engaging in risky and delinquent behaviors as compared to second-born
children. For instance, we find that being born second is associated
with a .09 increase in the probability that males ever smoked, but being
born fifth or higher is associated with a .180 increase.
Finally, we examine the relationship between birth order and risky
behavior using an alternative data set, the NLSY79. We find that for
individuals who reached adolescence in the 1970s, having an older
sibling is associated with an increase in the probability of engaging in
risky behavior. Moreover, we find that individuals with older siblings
are more likely to smoke as young adults, a result that suggests that
birth order can have important healthrelated consequences in the long
run.
ABBREVIATIONS
NLSY79: National Longitudinal Survey of Youth-1979
NLSY97: National Longitudinal Survey of Youth-1997
REFERENCES
Argys, L. M., and D. I. Rees. "Searching for Peer Group
Effects: A Test of the Contagion Hypothesis." Paper presented at
the annual meetings of the Population Association of America,
Philadelphia, March 2005.
Becker, G. S., and H. G. Lewis. "On the Interaction between
the Quantity and Quality of Children." Journal of Political
Economy, 81(2), 1973, S279-88.
Becker, G. S., and N. Tomes. "Child Endowments and Quantity
and Quality of Children." Journal of Political Economy, 84(4),
1976, S143-62.
Behrman, J. R., and P. Taubman. "Birth Order, Schooling, and
Earnings." Journal of Labor Economics, 4(3), 1986, S121-50.
Birdsall, N. "Birth Order Effects and Time Allocation."
Research in Population Economics, 7, 1991, 191-213.
Blake, J. "Family Size and the Quality of the Children."
Demography, 18(4), 1981, 421-42.
Brook, J. S., M. Whiteman, D. W. Brook, and A. S. Gordon.
"Sibling Influences on Adolescent Drug Use: Older Brothers on
Younger Brothers." Journal of the American Academy of Child and
Adolescent Psychology, 30(6), 1991, 958-66.
Butcher, K. F., and A. Case. "The Effect of Sibling Sex
Composition on Women's Education and Earnings." Quarterly
Journal of Economics, 109(3), 1994, 531-63.
Datcher, L. "Effects of Community and Family Background on
Achievement." Review of Economics and Statistics, 64(1), 1982,
32-41.
Gruber, J., and J. Zinman. "Youth Smoking in the United
States: Evidence and Implications," in Risky Behavior among Youths:
An Economic Analysis, edited by J. Gruber. Chicago: University of
Chicago Press, 2001.
Hanushek, E. A. "The Trade-Off between Child Quantity and
Quality." Journal of Political Economy, 100(1), 1992, 84-117.
Haurin, R. J., and F. L. Mott. "Adolescent Sexual Activity in
the Family Context: The Impact of Older Siblings." Demography,
27(4), 1990, 537-57.
Hauser, R., and H-H. D. Kuo. "Does the Gender Composition of
Sibships Affect Women's Educational Attainment?" Journal of
Human Resources, 33(3), 1998, 644-57.
Hauser, R. M., and W. H. Sewell. "Birth Order and Education
Attainments in Full Sibships." Working Paper No. 83-31. Madison:
University of Wisconsin, Center for Demography and Ecology, 1983.
Haveman, R., and B. Wolfe. "The Determinants of
Children's Attainments: A Review of Methods and Findings."
Journal of Economic Literature, 33(4), 1995, 1829-78.
Hill, M., and G. J. Duncan. "Parental Family Income and the
Socioeconomic Attainment of Children." Social Science Research,
16(1), 1987, 39-73.
Isaacson, C., and K. Radish. The Birth Order Effect: How to Better
Understand Yourself and Others. Avon, MA: Adams Media, 2002.
Kaestner, R. "Are Brothers Really Better? Sibling Sex
Composition and Educational Achievement Revisited." Journal of
Human Resources, 32(2),1997, 250-84.
Kessler, D. "Birth Order, Family Size, and Achievement: Family
Structure, and Wage Determination." Journal of Labor Economics,
9(4), 1991, 413-26.
Leman, K. The Birth Order Book: Why You Are the Way You Are. Grand
Rapids, MI: Baker Book House, 2001.
Mocan, N. H., B. Scafidi, and E. Tekin. "Catholic Schools and
Bad Behavior." National Bureau of Economic Research Working Paper
No. 9172, Cambridge MA, 2002.
National Center on Addiction and Substance Abuse. "Teen
Tippers: America's Underage Drinking Epidemic." National
Center on Addiction and Substance Abuse, Columbia University, 2002.
Oettinger, G. S. "Sibling Similarity in High School Graduation Outcomes: Causal Interdependency or Unobserved Heterogeneity?"
Southern Economic Journal 66(3), 2000, 631-48.
Olneck, M. R., and D. B. Bills. "Family Configuration and
Achievement: Effects of Birth Order and Family Size in a Sample of
Brothers." Social Psychology Quarterly, 42(2), 1979, 135-48.
Ouyang, L. "Sibling Effects on Teen Risky Behaviors."
Working Paper, Duke University, Department of Economics, 2004.
Pacula, R. L., M. Grossman, F. J. Chaloupka, P. M. O'Malley,
L. D. Johnston, and M. C. Farrelly. "Marijuana and Youth," in
Risky Behavior among Youths: An Economic Analysis, edited by Jonathan Gruber. Chicago: University of Chicago Press, 2001.
Rodgers, J. L., D. C. Rowe, and D. F. Harris. "Sibling
Differences in Adolescent Sexual Behavior: Inferring Process Models from
Family Composition Patterns." Journal of the Marriage and the
Family, 54, 1992, 142-52.
Slomkowski, C., R. Rende, S. Novak, and E. Lloyd-Richardson.
"Sibling Effects on Smoking in Adolescence: Evidence for Social
Influence from Genetically Informative Design." Addiction, 100(4),
2005, 430-38.
Sulloway, F. J. Born to Rebel. Birth Order, Family Dynamics and
Creative Live. New York: Vintage, 1997.
Tyas, S. L., and L. L. Pederson. "Psychosocial Factors Related
to Adolescent Smoking: A Critical Review of the Literature."
Tobacco Control 7, 1998, 409-20.
Wallace, M. Birth Order Blues: How Parents Can Help Their Children
Meet the Challenges of Birth Order. New York: Henry Holt, 1999.
Wang, M. Q., E. C. Fitzhugh, R. C. Westerfield, and J. M. Eddy.
"Family and Peer Influences on Smoking Behavior among American
Adolescents: An Age Trend." Journal of Adolescent Health, 16(3),
1995, 200-203.
Widmer, E. D. "Influence of Older Siblings on Initiation of
Sexual Intercourse." Journal of Marriage and the Family, 59(4),
1997, 928-38.
Zajonc, R. B. "Family Configuration and Intelligence."
Science, 16, 1976, 227-36.
Zax, J., and D. I. Rees. "IQ, Academic Performance,
Environment and Earnings." Review of Economics and Statistics,
84(4), 2002, 600-616.
Zick, C. D., and W. K. Bryant. "A New Look at Parents'
Time Spent in Child Care: Primary and Secondary Time Use." Social
Science Research, 25(3), 1996, 260-80.
(1.) Other popular books in this area include those by Isaacson and
Radish (2002) and Sulloway (1997). Sulloway takes a more historical
approach and offers evidence that birth order affects political power
and scientific innovation, but the author does not investigate the
effect of birth order on wages or education.
(2.) The Becker and Tomes (1976) model was an extension of the
well-known child quantity-quality tradeoff model proposed by Becker and
Lewis (1973), who assumed an equal level of financial investment in each
child within a family.
(3.) Zajonc (1976) proposed thinking of the "intellectual
environment" of the family, in which a child is raised as the
average of the "absolute intellectual levels of [all of] its
members" (227). When a new child is born, this average falls (the
absolute intellectual level of a newborn can be thought of as zero), and
the new child's intellectual environment is therefore less
stimulating than that enjoyed by the older siblings. According to this
argument, older siblings will usually be more intelligent and score
higher on standard tests of achievement such as the SAT than will their
younger brothers and sisters.
(4.) Behrman and Taubman (1986) write that "the oldest child
has some periods, particularly during presumably critical early years,
when he or she has less competition for mother's time" (S124,
italics added). When discussing the impact of the home environment,
Zajonc (1976) explicitly notes that "the later these influences
occur in an individual's life, the smaller is their effect"
(228).
(5.) Sociologists and developmental psychologists such as Haveman
and Wolfe (1995) and Rodgers et al. (1992) have emphasized the
importance of role models, including older siblings, in the
determination of aspirations and behavioral norms of children and
adolescents.
(6.) Data from the NLSY97 show a strong positive link between risky
behaviors and age among adolescents living in the United States in the
late 1990s. For instance, the probability of marijuana use increases, on
average, by .034 for every year of adolescence. To take another example,
the probability that an adolescent is sexually active increases, on
average, by. 123 per year, according to Argys and Rees (2005). This
pattern of results suggests that older siblings may
"prematurely" expose their younger brothers and sisters to
certain risky behaviors.
(7.) If a substantial age gap exists, older siblings may be more
likely to inherit monitoring responsibilities from their parents. This
could serve to magnify the effect of having an older sibling, whether
positive or negative.
(8.) Haveman and Wolfe (1995) list a number of articles that find a
negative relationship between educational outcomes and family size. The
evidence with regard to earnings is less strong. See, for instance,
Datcher (1982), Hill and Duncan (1987), and Zax and Rees (2002).
(9.) A number of studies have in fact examined if the behavior or
achievements of an older sibling influence the younger sibling's
behavior--see Haurin and Mott (1990), Wang et al. (1995), Widmer (1997),
and Slomkowski et al. (2005)--but, to our knowledge, only Oettinger
(2000) attempted to address what he terms the "unobserved
heterogeneity" problem: "Of course, since the behavior of the
family unit simultaneously influences the achievement of all of the
children in the family and since many family factors are unobservable
[to the researcher], a sibling's achievement should be treated as
an endogenous explanatory variable" (632). Oettinger found that,
even controlling for unobserved heterogeneity, having an older sibling
who graduated from high school increased the probability that the
younger sibling would go on to graduate. Oettinger argues that
successful older siblings could act as positive role models "by
revealing information about a youth's own potential ... or through
other mechanisms" (632). He also suggests that "the
achievement of siblings might have spillover effects if learning is to
some extent a public good within the family unit" (632).
(10.) The NLSY97 questions pertaining to sexual activity and
contraceptive use were asked only of respondents ages 14 and older.
(11.) The initial three waves of the NSLY97 took place in 1997,
1998, and 1999. By the fourth wave, almost 60% of the NSLY97 respondents
were at least 18 years old. Because we restrict our sample to
participants between the ages of 12 and 17, and as a result of natural
attrition from the NLSY97, many respondents contributed fewer than three
observations to the analysis. For instance, a total of 4,317 respondents
contributed an average of 2.28 observations to the male marijuana
estimation presented in Table 3. About half of these (50.4%) were
observed in each of the three waves of the NLSY97 analyzed. A total of
4,082 respondents contributed an average of 2.28 observations to the
female marijuana estimation presented in Table 3. Of these, 50.6% were
observed in each of the three waves.
(12.) Kessler (1991), proposes an empirical model that allowed for
interactions between family size and birth order.
(13.) These and all other results in the article are based on
unweighted data.
(14.) It is also possible that this pattern of results reflects
differences in the reporting of behavior as opposed to differences in
actual behavior. However, as noted, the NLSY97 collected sensitive
information using ACASI (Audio Computer Assisted Self-interview)
technology in order to minimize measurement error.
(15.) A number of other studies confirm that family size is not a
good predictor of adolescent smoking or substance use--see, for
instance, Tyas and Pederson (1998), Mocan et al. (2002), and Ouyang
(2004).
(16.) For instance, being raised as an only child (as compared to
being raised in a family of five or more children) is associated with a
15.1 percentage-point increase in the probability that a female
respondent reported ever drinking, whereas being raised in a four-child
family is associated with a 7.6 percentage-point increase in this same
probability.
(17.) Living in an urban area is also associated with a decrease in
the probability that male respondents carried a handgun and in the
probability that female respondents destroyed property or stole.
(18.) Of middle-born and last-born respondents, 25% had both an
older brother and an older sister; 36% had an older sister but no older
brother; 39% had an older brother but no older sister.
(19.) Older sisters have a significantly larger impact than that of
older brothers on the probability that male adolescents ever smoked,
drank alcohol, or belonged to a gang.
(20.) Of middle-born and last-born respondents, 20% had a sibling
in both of these age categories; 42% had a sibling in the first category
but not in the second; 38% had a sibling in the second but not in the
first.
(21.) In Table 10, we find evidence that birth spacing matters in
the female assault and gang equations. A number of plausible
explanations are available for why the sibling age gap might be related
to risk-taking behavior; however, it is difficult to imagine why it
might be related to a willingness to report such behavior. Thus, we view
the results from Model 2 as evidence against the hypothesis that the
birth-order effects documented in Tables 3 to 6 simply reflect
differences in reporting.
(22.) This last category includes respondents who had five or more
older siblings.
(23.) In Table 9, the effect of having been fourth or fifth is
significantly greater than the effect of having been born second in 6
out of 8 cases. In Table 10, the effect of having been born fourth or
fifth is significantly greater than the effect of having been born
second in only 4 out of 10 cases.
(24.) Studies of two-parent, two-child households have shown that
the age of the youngest child is an important determinant of the time
that parents devote to child care in general--see, for instance, Zick
and Bryant (1996).
(25.) Although we did not show them here, we also estimated models
in which the dependent variable was transformed in order to reflect
whether the respondent engaged in a particular behavior by a certain
age. We found that having an older sibling was associated with large
increases in the probability of smoking, drinking, using marijuana, and
engagin in sexual activity by the age of 15. However, when we examined
the relationship between birth order and engaging in risky behavior by
the age of 17, the results were less strong. Having an older sibling was
associated with an increase in the probability that males used marijuana
and were sexual active by the age of 17, and was associated with an
increase in the probability of smoking for both males and females. The
other estimated marginal effects were not significant at conventional
levels (although they were always positive).
(26.) The NLSY79 is a precursor to the primary data source used in
this investigation. It provides information on more than 12,000
individuals who were aged 14 to 21 in 1979, including an oversampling of
blacks, Hispanics, and economically disadvantaged whites.
(27.) We also investigated the effect of birth order on criminal
activities such as assault and robbery. Although not shown, our results
suggest that there was no effect of birth order on criminal activity for
this cohort.
(28.) Another possibility is that our findings reflect an
interaction between sibling and parental influences. For example,
parents could determine the optimal level of supervision for their
children based on the assumption that sibling behavior is contagious. If
this assumption is incorrect, then parents run the risk of setting
overly restrictive rules (e.g., an 8:00 PM curfew) for their
earlier-born offspring in a misguided attempt to shield their later-born
offspring. On the other hand, if sibling behavior is in fact contagious,
then it would make sense for parents to concentrate their supervision
efforts on earlier-born offspring in order to take advantage of positive
behavioral spillovers.
LAURA M. ARGYS, DANIEL I. REES, SUSAN L. AVERETT, and BENJAMA
WITOONCHART, We would like to acknowledge support from the National
Institute of Child Health and Human Development, contract number
HD047661. The views expressed in this article are those of the authors
and do not necessarily reflect the views of the National Institute of
Child Health and Human Development.
Argys: Associate Professor, University of Colorado at Denver,
Department of Economics, Campus Box 181, Denver, CO 80217-3364. Phone
1-303-556-3949, Fax 1-303-556-3547, E-mail Laura.Argys@cudenver.edu
Rees: Associate Professor, University of Colorado at Denver,
Department of Economics, Campus Box 181, Denver, CO 80217-3364. Phone
1-303-556-3348, Fax 1-303-556-3547, E-mail drees@carbon.cudenver.edu
Averett: Professor, Lafayette College, Department of Economics and
Business, Easton, PA 18042-1776. Phone 1-617-330-5307, Fax
1-610-330-5715, E-mail averetts@lafvax.lafayette.edu
Witoonchart: PhD Candidate, University of Colorado at Boulder,
Department of Economics, 256 UCB, Boulder, Colorado 80309-0256. E-mail
benjama. witoonchart@colorado.edu
TABLE 1
Unweighted Means of Dependent Variables by Gender and Presence of an
Older Sibling
Males
Has an Has no
Full Older Older
Sample Sibling Sibling
Substance Use
Ever smoked cigarettes 0.447 0.471 0.415
Smoked cigarettes in past 30 days 0.242 0.251 0.231
Ever drank alcohol 0.530 0.539 0.519
Drank alcohol in past 30 days 0.293 0.300 0.283
Ever smoked marijuana 0.267 0.271 0.261
Smoked marijuana in past 30 days 0.129 0.133 0.125
Sexual Activity
Ever had sexual intercourse 0.400 0.415 0.381
Sexually active in the past year 0.328 0.341 0.313
Used contraception at first intercourse 0.808 0.801 0.818
(if ever had intercourse)
Used contraception within the past year 0.868 0.867 0.869
(if sexually active)
Delinquent Activities
Ever carried a handgun 0.195 0.198 0.190
Carried a handgun in the past year 0.092 0.096 0.086
Ever assaulted anyone 0.281 0.281 0.281
Assaulted someone in the past year 0.075 0.073 0.078
Ever belonged to a gang 0.091 0.092 0.089
Was a gang member in the past year 0.036 0.036 0.036
Ever destroyed property 0.398 0.392 0.405
Destroyed property in the past year 0.198 0.194 0.203
Ever stole anything 0.419 0.422 0.416
Stole items worth more than $50 in 0.086 0.093 0.078
the past year
Females
Has an Has no
Full Older Older
Sample Sibling Sibling
Substance Use
Ever smoked cigarettes 0.435 0.443 0.425
Smoked cigarettes in past 30 days 0.227 0.234 0.218
Ever drank alcohol 0.516 0.510 0.524
Drank alcohol in past 30 days 0.280 0.282 0.277
Ever smoked marijuana 0.230 0.229 0.232
Smoked marijuana in past 30 days 0.098 0.098 0.100
Sexual Activity
Ever had sexual intercourse 0.336 0.326 0.349
Sexually active in the past year 0.281 0.273 0.291
Used contraception at first intercourse 0.786 0.796 0.774
(if ever had intercourse)
Used contraception within the past year 0.888 0.869 0.910
(if sexually active)
Delinquent Activities
Ever carried a handgun 0.043 0.048 0.036
Carried a handgun in the past year 0.018 0.020 0.015
Ever assaulted anyone 0.162 0.165 0.159
Assaulted someone in the past year 0.045 0.045 0.045
Ever belonged to a gang 0.044 0.046 0.041
Was a gang member in the past year 0.013 0.012 0.013
Ever destroyed property 0.220 0.228 0.209
Destroyed property in the past year 0.095 0.099 0.090
Ever stole anything 0.312 0.310 0.315
Stole items worth more than $50 in 0.035 0.035 0.035
the past year
TABLE 2
Unweighted Means of the Explanatory Variables by Gender and
Presence of an Older Sibling
Males
Full Has an Has no
Sample Older Sibling Older Sibling
Has an Older Sibling 0.559 1 0
Black 0.260 0.260 0.260
Other race 0.137 0.149 0.122
Hispanic 0.199 0.219 0.174
Age 15.10 15.04 15.18
(1.43) (1.44) (1.41)
Lives with both parents 0.542 0.585 0.488
Mother's age at respondent's 25.62 26.97 23.91
birth (5.24) (5.07) (4.95)
Highest parent's education 13.20 12.95 13.52
(3.01) (3.16) (2.77)
Income below 125% of the 0.106 0.104 0.109
poverty level
Income above 400% of the 0.076 0.069 0.084
poverty level
Number of siblings 2.211 2.695 1.597
(1.60) (1.64) (1.31)
Number of older siblings 0.97 1.74 0
(1.20) (1.12)
Lives in urban area 0.717 0.716 0.718
Sample size 9859 5514 4345
Females
Full Has an Has no
Sample Older Sibling Older Sibling
Has an Older Sibling 0.563 1 0
Black 0.266 0.273 0.257
Other race 0.140 0.157 0.117
Hispanic 0.205 0.228 0.175
Age 15.11 15.04 15.19
(1.43) (1.44) (1.42)
Lives with both parents 0.517 0.569 0.451
Mother's age at respondent's 25.49 26.89 23.69
birth (5.41) (5.32) (4.99)
Highest parent's education 13.12 12.88 13.43
(3.02) (3.11) (2.87)
Income below 125% of the 0.117 0.115 0.120
poverty level
Income above 400% of the 0.077 0.068 0.088
poverty level
Number of siblings 2.235 2.758 1.563
(1.73) (1.83) (1.31)
Number of older siblings 0.97 1.72 0
(1.23) (1.17)
Lives in urban area 0.729 0.733 0.724
Sample size 9328 5247 4081
Note: Standard deviations of continuous variables in parentheses.
TABLE 3
Determinants of Substance Use
Males
Ever Smoked Ever Drank Ever Smoked
Cigarettes Alcohol Marijuana
Black -0.172 ** -0.202 ** -0.066 **
(0.019) (0.019) (0.016)
Other race 0.002 -0.026 0.038
(0.028) (0.028) (0.024)
Hispanic -0.081 ** -0.040 -0.035
(0.025) (0.026) (0.020)
Age 12 -0.057 * -0.092 ** -0.078 **
(0.028) (0.029) (0.026)
Age 14 0.126 ** 0.116 ** 0.163 **
(0.021) (0.021) (0.025)
Age 15 0.210 ** 0.228 ** 0.254 **
(0.019) (0.018) (0.023)
Age 16 0.287 ** 0.321 ** 0.350 **
(0.021) (0.019) (0.025)
Age 17 0.360 ** 0.384 ** 0.426 **
(0.024) (0.020) (0.028)
Lives with both parents -0.127 ** -0.094 ** -0.101 **
(0.017) (0.017) (0.014)
Mother's age at birth of -0.003 -0.002 -0.002
respondent (0.002) (0.002) (0.001)
Parents' highest education -0.009 ** 0.001 -0.0004
level (0.003) (0.003) (0.003)
Income < 125% of poverty 0.014 -0.005 0.013
(0.022) (0.021) (0.019)
Income < 400% of poverty 0.022 0.035 -0.001
(0.024) (0.023) (0.021)
Lives in an urban area -0.022 0.027 0.033 *
(0.019) (0.018) (0.015)
Only child 0.042 0.003 0.032
(0.035) (0.034) (0.031)
Two-child family -0.001 0.038 0.017
(0.025) (0.024) (0.021)
Three-child family -0.019 0.018 -0.00001
(0.024) (0.023) (0.020)
Four-child family -0.005 -0.006 -0.002
(0.026) (0.025) (0.021)
Has an older sibling 0.105 ** 0.062 ** 0.051 **
(0.017) (0.017) (0.014)
Sample size 9859 9852 9843
Females
Ever Smoked Ever Drank Ever Smoked
Cigarettes Alcohol Marijuana
Black -0.247 ** -0.193 ** -0.145 **
(0.019) (0.020) (0.013)
Other race -0.019 -0.026 -0.016
(0.028) (0.029) (0.022)
Hispanic -0.119 ** -0.039 -0.038
(0.025) (0.026) (0.019)
Age 12 -0.110 ** -0.137 ** 0.107 **
(0.029) (0.032) (0.042)
Age 14 0.119 ** 0.164 ** 0.259 **
(0.022) (0.021) (0.038)
Age 15 0.199 ** 0.297 ** 0.372 **
(0.020) (0.019) (0.038)
Age 16 0.264 ** 0.375 ** 0.427 **
(0.022) (0.019) (0.040)
Age 17 0.306 ** 0.428 ** 0.498 **
(0.026) (0.019) (0.041)
Lives with both parents -0.150 ** -0.130 ** -0.126 **
(0.018) (0.017) (0.013)
Mother's age at birth of -0.002 0.001 -0.001
respondent (0.002) (0.002) (0.001)
Parents' highest education -0.0003 0.0053 0.0017
level (0.003) (0.003) (0.002)
Income < 125% of poverty -0.010 -0.050 * 0.001
(0.022) (0.023) (0.019)
Income < 400% of poverty -0.085 ** -0.059 * -0.041 *
(0.023) (0.024) (0.018)
Lives in an urban area 0.003 0.043 * 0.052 **
(0.020) (0.019) (0.014)
Only child 0.063 0.151 ** 0.035
(0.037) (0.033) (0.029)
Two-child family 0.040 0.125 ** 0.029
(0.027) (0.026) (0.021)
Three-child family 0.026 0.101 ** 0.031
(0.026) (0.025) (0.021)
Four-child family 0.015 0.076 ** -0.006
(0.028) (0.027) (0.021)
Has an older sibling 0.080 ** 0.055 ** 0.041 **
(0.018) (0.017) (0.013)
Sample size 9328 9323 9319
Note: Marginal probabilities from a univariate probit model are
reported. Standard errors corrected for clustering at the family level
are in parentheses. Specifications include controls for survey year and
missing race/ethnicity, parental education, interview year, and family
income.
** p < .01; * p < .05; (^) p < .10.
TABLE 4
The Effect of an Older Sibling on Substance Use within the Past 30 Days
Males
Smoked Drank Smoked
Cigarettes in Alcohol in Marijuana in
Past 30 Days Past 30 Days Past 30 Days
Has an Older Sibling 0.055 ** 0.056 ** 0.029 **
(0.013) (0.013) (0.009)
Sample size 9031 9051 9340
Females
Smoked Smoked
Cigarettes in Drank Alcohol Marijuana in
Past 30 Days in Past 30 Days Past 30 Days
Has an Older Sibling 0.063 ** 0.033 * 0.018 *
(0.013) (0.013) (0.008)
Sample size 8595 8728 8913
Note: Marginal probabilities from a univariate probit model are
reported. Standard errors corrected for clustering at the family level
are in parentheses. In addition to the independent variables used in
Table 3, specifications include controls for survey year and missing
race/ethnicity, parental education, interview year, and family income.
** p < .01; * p < .05.
TABLE 5
Determinants of Sexual Behavior
Males
Ever had Sexual Used Birth Control
Intercourse at First Intercourse
Black 0.241 ** (0.020) 0.041 (^) (0.022)
Other race 0.042 (0.028) 0.010 (0.035)
Hispanic 0.005 (0.027) 0.017 (0.031)
Age 15 0.104 ** (0.027) -0.024 (0.040)
Age 16 0.269 ** (0.024) -0.034 (0.036)
Age 17 0.405 ** (0.025) -0.045 (0.039)
Lives with both parents -0.136 ** (0.017) -0.004 (0.021)
Mother's age at birth of
respondent -0.005 ** (0.002) 0.003 (0.002)
Parents' highest education -0.017 ** (0.003) 0.007 (0.004)
Income < 125% of poverty 0.044 (^) (0.027) -0.016 (0.030)
Income < 400% of poverty -0.014 (0.030) -0.005 (0.040)
Lives in an urban area 0.044 * (0.018) 0.007 (0.024)
Only child 0.025 (0.037) -0.005 (0.046)
Two-child family 0.001 (0.025) 0.014 (0.028)
Three-child family -0.011 (0.024) -0.01 (0.028)
Four-child family -0.026 (0.025) 0.027 (0.028)
Has an older sibling 0.074 ** (0.018) -0.026 (0.022)
Sample size 7291 2899
Females
Ever had Sexual Used Birth Control
Intercourse at First Intercourse
Black 0.034 (^) (0.020) 0.098 ** (0.025)
Other race 0.010 (0.027) -0.063 (0.047)
Hispanic -0.070 ** (0.024) -0.028 (0.040)
Age 15 0.130 ** (0.027) -0.010 (0.020)
Age 16 0.277 ** (0.025) 0.001 (0.021)
Age 17 0.400 ** (0.027) 0.00001 (0.026)
Lives with both parents -0.148 ** (0.016) -0.002 (0.002)
Mother's age at birth of
respondent -0.006 ** (0.002) -0.204 ** (0.067)
Parents' highest education -0.009 ** (0.003) -0.042 (0.035)
Income < 125% of poverty 0.040 (0.026) 0.047 (0.041)
Income < 400% of poverty -0.057 * (0.027) 0.034 (0.029)
Lives in an urban area -0.010 (0.018) -0.033 (0.059)
Only child 0.088 * (0.036) 0.053 (0.033)
Two-child family 0.038 (0.025) 0.039 (0.033)
Three-child family 0.022 (0.024) -0.008 (0.036)
Four-child family 0.015 (0.026) 0.043 (0.026)
Has an older sibling 0.040 * (0.017) 0.011 (0.023)
Sample size 6920 2313
Note: Marginal probabilities from a univariate probit model are
reported. Standard errors corrected for clustering at the family level
are in parentheses. Specifications include controls for survey year and
missing race/ethnicity, parental education, interview year, and family
income.
** p < .01; * p < .05; (^) p < .10.
TABLE 6
The Effect of an Older Sibling on Sexual Behavior in the Past Year
Males
Used Birth
Had Sexual Control Within
Intercourse the Past Year
in Past Year (if Sexually Active)
Has an older sibling 0.071 ** 0.005
(0.017) (0.018)
Sample size 6566 2193
Females
Used Birth
Had Sexual Control Within
Intercourse the Past Year
in Past Year (if Sexually Active)
Has an older sibling 0.042 ** -0.031 (^)
(0.016) (0.017)
Sample size 6423 1857
Note: Marginal probabilities from a univariate probit model are
reported. Standard errors corrected for clustering at the family level
are in parentheses. In addition to the independent variables used in
Table 3, specifications include controls for survey year and missing
race/ethnicity, parental education, interview year, and family income.
** p < .01; (^) p < .10.
TABLE 7
The Determinants of Criminal and Delinquent Activities
Males
Ever Ever Ever
Engaged Belonged Carried a
in Assault to a Gang Handgun
Black 0.033 (^) 0.028 ** -0.071 **
(0.018) (0.011) (0.014)
Other Race -0.013 0.012 -0.032
(0.024) (0.014) (0.019)
Hispanic -0.041 (^) 0.020 -0.028
(0.021) (0.014) (0.018)
Age 12 0.034 -0.020 0.012
(0.027) (0.014) (0.023)
Age 14 0.070 ** 0.021 0.040 *
(0.022) (0.014) (0.019)
Age 15 0.086 ** 0.041 ** 0.069 **
(0.024) (0.014) (0.017)
Age 16 0.093 ** 0.064 ** 0.084 **
(0.028) (0.016) (0.020)
Age 17 0.103 ** 0.062 ** 0.123 **
(0.031) (0.020) (0.026)
Lives with both parents -0.092 ** -0.041 ** -0.028 *
(0.015) (0.009) (0.013)
Mother's age at birth -0.002 -0.001 -0.002
of respondent (0.002) (0.001) (0.001)
Parents' highest -0.010 ** -0.004 ** -0.004
education level (0.003) (0.001) (0.002)
Income < 125% of poverty -0.004 0.013 0.033 *
(0.018) (0.011) (0.017)
Income < 400% of poverty -0.018 0.005 -0.019
(0.020) (0.013) (0.017)
Lives in an urban area 0.031 * 0.019 * -0.045 **
(0.016) (0.009) (0.015)
Only child -0.032 -0.032 * -0.002
(0.029) (0.012) (0.027)
Two-child family -0.014 -0.028 * -0.008
(0.021) (0.011) (0.019)
Three-child family -0.014 -0.006 -0.0108
(0.021) (0.011) (0.018)
Four-child family -0.031 -0.029 * -0.026
(0.021) (0.010) (0.018)
Has an older sibling 0.012 0.001 0.023
(0.016) (0.009) (0.013)
Sample size 9876 9850 9849
Males
Ever Ever
Destroyed Stolen
Property Anything
Black -0.101 ** -0.105 **
(0.019) (0.020)
Other Race 0.008 -0.003
(0.027) (0.028)
Hispanic -0.080 ** -0.049 (^)
(0.024) (0.025)
Age 12 0.032 -0.030
(0.027) (0.026)
Age 14 0.063 ** 0.079 **
(0.022) (0.021)
Age 15 0.081 ** 0.100 **
(0.024) (0.019)
Age 16 0.103 ** 0.145 **
(0.028) (0.022)
Age 17 0.107 ** 0.165 **
(0.031) (0.027)
Lives with both parents -0.066 ** -0.102 **
(0.017) (0.017)
Mother's age at birth -0.005 ** -0.001
of respondent (0.002) (0.002)
Parents' highest 0.003 0.001
education level (0.003) (0.003)
Income < 125% of poverty -0.013 -0.011
(0.021) (0.021)
Income < 400% of poverty 0.001 0.010
(0.022) (0.023)
Lives in an urban area 0.050 ** 0.068 **
(0.018) (0.018)
Only child -0.024 -0.022
(0.034) (0.034)
Two-child family -0.038 -0.031
(0.025) (0.025)
Three-child family -0.022 -0.019
(0.024) (0.024)
Four-child family -0.030 -0.039
(0.025) (0.025)
Has an older sibling 0.011 0.030
(0.017) (0.017)
Sample size 9877 9878
Females
Ever Ever Ever
Engaged Belonged Carried a
in Assault to a Gang Handgun
Black 0.041 ** -0.007 -0.013 *
(0.016) (0.007) (0.006)
Other Race 0.040 (^) 0.020 * -0.003
(0.022) (0.012) (0.008)
Hispanic -0.028 0.006 -0.002
(0.017) (0.009) (0.008)
Age 12 0.012 0.020 0.021
(0.024) (0.018) (0.020)
Age 14 0.034 (^) 0.029 * 0.050 **
(0.019) (0.016) (0.021)
Age 15 0.067 ** 0.032 * 0.057 **
(0.022) (0.016) (0.021)
Age 16 0.060 * 0.030 (^) 0.059 **
(0.025) (0.018) (0.023)
Age 17 0.062 * 0.031 (^) 0.061 **
(0.027) (0.020) (0.026)
Lives with both parents -0.079 ** -0.034 ** -0.025 **
(0.012) (0.007) (0.006)
Mother's age at birth -0.002 (^) -0.001 (^) 0.0005
of respondent (0.001) (0.001) (0.001)
Parents' highest -0.004 * -0.002 * -0.001
education level (0.002) (0.001) (0.001)
Income < 125% of poverty 0.013 -0.001 0.002
(0.016) (0.008) (0.007)
Income < 400% of poverty -0.020 -0.004 -0.016
(0.017) (0.009) (0.007)
Lives in an urban area 0.026 * -0.003 0.007
(0.013) (0.007) (0.006)
Only child -0.034 -0.011 0.023
(0.021) (0.010) (0.016)
Two-child family -0.025 0.001 0.010
(0.017) (0.009) (0.009)
Three-child family -0.0007 0.012 0.003
(0.016) (0.010) (0.008)
Four-child family -0.021 0.0003 0.014
(0.017) (0.009) (0.010)
Has an older sibling 0.013 0.007 0.016 *
(0.012) (0.006) (0.006)
Sample size 9337 9325 9328
Females
Ever Ever
Destroyed Stolen
Property Anything
Black -0.057 ** -0.069 **
(0.016) (0.019)
Other Race 0.010 0.064 *
(0.024) (0.028)
Hispanic -0.051 * -0.038
(0.020) (0.024)
Age 12 0.443 0.347
(0.284) (0.281)
Age 14 0.054 * -0.016
(0.026) (0.027)
Age 15 0.033 (^) 0.058 **
(0.020) (0.021)
Age 16 0.042 (^) 0.092 **
(0.023) (0.019)
Age 17 0.024 0.118 **
(0.026) (0.022)
Lives with both parents 0.012 0.129 **
(0.028) (0.028)
Mother's age at birth -0.088 ** -0.092 **
of respondent (0.015) (0.016)
Parents' highest 0.086 * 0.012
education level (0.039) (0.041)
Income < 125% of poverty 0.004 0.008 **
(0.003) (0.003)
Income < 400% of poverty -0.018 -0.043 *
(0.017) (0.019)
Lives in an urban area -0.028 -0.042 *
(0.015) (0.017)
Only child 0.024 0.005
(0.035) (0.039)
Two-child family -0.021 -0.006
(0.028) (0.033)
Three-child family -0.010 -0.001
(0.021) (0.024)
Four-child family -0.003 -0.017
(0.021) (0.023)
Has an older sibling -0.029 -0.051 *
(0.021) (0.024)
Sample size 9341 9342
Note: Marginal probabilities from a univariate probit model are
reported. Standard errors corrected for clustering at the family level
are in parentheses. Specifications include controls for survey year
and missing race/ethnicity, parental education, interview year, and
family income.
** p < .01; * p < .05; (^) p <.10.
TABLE 8
The Effect of an Older Sibling on Criminal and Delinquent
Activities in the Past Year
Males
Ever Ever Ever Ever Ever
Engaged Belonged Carried a Destroyed Stolen
in Assault to a Gang Handgun Property Anything
Has an -0.004 -0.001 0.017 * 0.013 0.016 (^)
older
sibling (0.007) (0.004) (0.007) (0.012) (0.008)
Sample
Size 8085 9843 9845 8356 6484
Females
Ever Ever Ever Ever Ever
Engaged Belonged Carried a Destroyed Stolen
in Assault to a Gang Handgun Property Anything
Has an 0.005 -0.001 0.005 (^) 0.017 (^) 0.006
older
sibling (0.005) (0.002) (0.003) (0.009) (0.005)
Sample
Size 8404 9322 9326 8455 6766
Note: Marginal probabilities from a univariate probit model are
reported. Standard errors corrected for clustering at the family level
are in parentheses. In addition to the independent variables used in
Table 3, specifications include controls for survey year and missing
race/ethnicity, parental education, interview year, and family income.
* p < .05; (^) p < .10.
TABLE 9
Extensions to the Baseline Model: Substance Use and Sexual Behavior
Males
Ever Ever Ever had
Smoked Drank Ever used Sexual
Cigarettes Alcohol Marijuana Intercourse
Model 1
Has an older brother 0.052 ** 0.018 0.039 ** 0.041 *
(0.017) (0.017) (0.014) (0.018)
Has an older sister 0.097 ** 0.064 ** 0.054 ** 0.064 **
(0.018) (0.018) (0.015) (0.019)
Sample size 9859 9852 9843 7291
Model 2
Has an older sibling 0.062 ** 0.008 0.021 0.027
within 3 years (0.016) (0.016) (0.013) (0.017)
Has an older sibling 4 0.111 ** 0.096 ** 0.054 ** 0.091 **
or more years older (0.018) (0.018) (0.016) (0.019)
Sample size 9859 9852 9843 7291
Model 3
Second-born 0.090 ** 0.049 ** 0.038 * 0.062 **
(0.018) (0.018) (0.015) (0.019)
Third-born 0.148 ** 0.095 ** 0.096 ** 0.099 **
(0.026) (0.024) (0.024) (0.027)
Fourth-born 0.154 ** 0.096 ** 0.080 * 0.125 **
(0.038) (0.036) (0.036) (0.041)
Fifth-born (or higher) 0.180 ** 0.103 * 0.134 ** 0.223 **
(0.048) (0.047) (0.051) (0.052)
Sample size 9859 9852 9843 7291
Model 4
Two-child families
Second-born 0.068 * 0.048 0.035 0.072 *
(0.028) (0.028) (0.024) (0.029)
Sample size 2902 2901 2902 2169
Three-child families
Second-born 0.083 ** 0.051 0.023 0.029
(0.032) (0.032) (0.026) (0.033)
Third-born 0.216 ** 0.176 ** 0.126 ** 0.154 **
(0.040) (0.036) (0.036) (0.042)
Sample size 2826 2827 2826 2057
Four-child families
Second-born 0.091 * 0.026 0.050 0.142 **
(0.046) (0.044) (0.038) (0.048)
Third-born 0.156 ** 0.034 0.113 * 0.102
(0.053) (0.051) (0.047) (0.054)
Fourth-born 0.194 ** 0.134 * 0.154 ** 0.198 **
(0.062) (0.060) (0.059) (0.069)
Sample size 1737 1734 1730 1271
Females
Ever Ever Ever had
Smoked Drank Ever used Sexual
Cigarettes Alcohol Marijuana Intercourse
Model 1
Has an older brother 0.059 ** 0.034 (^) 0.037 * 0.056 **
(0.019) (0.018) (0.015) (0.018)
Has an older sister 0.084 ** 0.053 ** 0.039 ** 0.040 *
(0.018) (0.018) (0.014) (0.017)
Sample size 9328 9323 9319 6920
Model 2
Has an older sibling 0.029 -0.0005 0.018 -0.006
within 3 years (0.017) (0.016) (0.013) (0.016)
Has an older sibling 4 0.110 ** 0.075 ** 0.071 ** 0.090 **
or more years older (0.019) (0.019) (0.016) (0.018)
Sample size 9328 9323 9319 6920
Model 3
Second-born 0.075 ** 0.051 ** 0.036 * 0.025
(0.019) (0.018) (0.015) (0.018)
Third-born 0.093 ** 0.063 * 0.058 ** 0.070 **
(0.028) (0.027) (0.023) (0.027)
Fourth-born 0.101 * 0.081 * 0.088 * 0.162 **
(0.042) (0.040) (0.038) (0.044)
Fifth-born (or higher) 0.192 ** 0.077 0.117 ** 0.133 **
(0.050) (0.048) (0.049) (0.052)
Sample size 9328 9323 9319 6920
Model 4
Two-child families
Second-born 0.071 * 0.081 ** 0.007 0.023
(0.029) (0.029) (0.022) (0.028)
Sample size 2845 2842 2841 2098
Three-child families
Second-born 0.069 * 0.050 0.071 ** 0.030
(0.033) (0.031) (0.028) (0.031)
Third-born 0.104 * 0.098 * 0.080 * 0.097 *
(0.041) (0.039) (0.036) (0.040)
Sample size 2656 2656 2653 1973
Four-child families
Second-born 0.098 * -0.007 0.017 -0.036
(0.048) (0.045) (0.034) (0.047)
Third-born 0.077 -0.045 0.013 0.013
(0.060) (0.056) (0.040) (0.059)
Fourth-born 0.092 -0.009 0.044 0.153 *
(0.074) (0.069) (0.054) (0.075)
Sample size 1406 1406 1404 1047
Note: Marginal probabilities from a univariate probit model are
reported. Standard errors corrected for clustering at the family level
are in parentheses. Sample size in brackets. In addition to the
independent variables used in Table 3, specifications include controls
for survey year and missing race/ethnicity, parental education, and
family income.
** p < .01; * p <. 05; (^) p < .10.
TABLE 10
Extensions to the Baseline Model: Criminal and Delinquent Activities
Males
Ever Ever Ever
Engaged Belonged Carries
in Assault to a Gang a Handgun
Model 1
Has an older brother 0.001 -0.004 0.019
(0.015) (0.008) (0.013)
Has an older sister 0.033 * 0.019 * 0.009
(0.016) (0.009) (0.014)
Sample size 9876 9850 9849
Model 2
Has an older sibling 0.011 -0.002 0.017
within 3 years (0.015) (0.008) (0.013)
Has an older sibling 4 0.024 0.006 0.011
or more years older (0.017) (0.009) (0.014)
Sample size 9876 9850 9849
Model 3
Second-born 0.009 -0.002 0.030 *
(0.017) (0.009) (0.015)
Third-born 0.016 0.003 0.001
(0.024) (0.013) (0.020)
Fourth-born 0.015 0.018 0.007
(0.034) (0.021) (0.029)
Fifth-born (or higher) 0.117 * 0.025 0.065 (^)
(0.050) (0.028) (0.042)
Sample size 9876 9850 9849
Model 4
Two-child families
Second-born -0.013 -0.002 -0.018
(0.026) (0.012) (0.022)
2910 2901 2905
Three-child families
Second-born 0.004 -0.004 0.047 (^)
(0.029) (0.017) (0.026)
Third-born 0.030 0.013 0.035
(0.037) (0.022) (0.032)
Sample size 2835 2823 2820
Four-child families
Second-born 0.042 -0.007 0.048
(0.041) (0.020) (0.034)
Third-born 0.016 0.011 -0.021
(0.047) (0.026) (0.038)
Fourth-born 0.040 0.036 0.063
(0.056) (0.035) (0.050)
Sample size 1736 1734 1735
Males
Ever Ever
Destroyed Stolen
Property Anything
Model 1
Has an older brother 0.009 0.033 (^)
(0.017) (0.017)
Has an older sister 0.002 0.024
(0.018) (0.018)
Sample size 9877 9878
Model 2
Has an older sibling 0.019 0.028 (^)
within 3 years (0.016) (0.016)
Has an older sibling 4 0.012 0.049 **
or more years older (0.018) (0.019)
Sample size 9877 9878
Model 3
Second-born 0.003 0.009
(0.018) (0.018)
Third-born 0.037 0.079 **
(0.026) (0.026)
Fourth-born 0.012 0.106 **
(0.038) (0.038)
Fifth-born (or higher) 0.023 0.125 *
(0.051) (0.051)
Sample size 9877 9878
Model 4
Two-child families
Second-born 0.024 0.003
(0.029) (0.029)
2911 2911
Three-child families
Second-born -0.026 -0.026
(0.032) (0.032)
Third-born 0.090 * 0.124 **
(0.040) (0.039)
Sample size 2833 2835
Four-child families
Second-born -0.022 0.077
(0.044) (0.044)
Third-born -0.038 0.043
(0.050) (0.052)
Fourth-born -0.071 0.067
(0.059) (0.063)
Sample size 1737 1737
Females
Ever Ever Ever
Engaged Belonged Carries
in Assault to a Gang a Handgun
Model 1
Has an older brother -0.0002 0.015 * 0.018 **
(0.013) (0.007) (0.007)
Has an older sister 0.021 (^) 0.010 (^) 0.010
(0.013) (0.006) (0.007)
Sample size 9337 9325 9328
Model 2
Has an older sibling 0.0004 0.003 0.008
within 3 years (0.012) (0.006) (0.006)
Has an older sibling 4 0.046 ** 0.022 ** 0.013 *
or more years older (0.014) (0.008) (0.007)
Sample size 9337 9325 9328
Model 3
Second-born 0.011 0.006 0.014 *
(0.013) (0.006) (0.007)
Third-born 0.009 0.011 0.033 **
(0.019) (0.011) (0.014)
Fourth-born 0.057 (^) 0.022 0.020
(0.034) (0.019) (0.021)
Fifth-born (or higher) 0.071 * 0.057 * 0.047 *
(0.039) (0.032) (0.030)
Sample size 9337 9325 9328
Model 4
Two-child families
Second-born 0.020 -0.0004 0.011
(0.018) (0.007) (0.009)
2847 2843 2845
Three-child families
Second-born -0.006 0.010 0.019 (^)
(0.024) (0.013) (0.012)
Third-born 0.005 0.013 0.033 *
(0.030) (0.017) (0.018)
Sample size 2661 2658 2521
Four-child families
Second-born 0.010 0.018 0.029
(0.031) (0.015) (0.022)
Third-born -0.0001 0.015 0.045
(0.040) (0.020) (0.030)
Fourth-born 0.075 0.027 0.049
(0.058) (0.029) (0.045)
Sample size 1408 1312 1406
Females
Ever Ever
Destroyed Stolen
Property Anything
Model 1
Has an older brother 0.032 * 0.023
(0.015) (0.017)
Has an older sister 0.026 (^) -0.0003
(0.016) (0.017)
Sample size 236 276
Model 2
Has an older sibling 0.033 * 0.018
within 3 years (0.015) (0.016)
Has an older sibling 4 0.039 * 0.027
or more years older (0.016) (0.018)
Sample size 9341 9342
Model 3
Second-born 0.020 0.021
(0.016) (0.017)
Third-born 0.072 ** 0.024
(0.026) (0.026)
Fourth-born 0.064 (^) 0.020
(0.039) (0.039)
Fifth-born (or higher) 0.046 -0.039
(0.046) (0.043)
Sample size 9341 9342
Model 4
Two-child families
Second-born -0.0003 0.010
(0.024) (0.028)
2848 2847
Three-child families
Second-born 0.041 0.035
(0.029) (0.032)
Third-born 0.064 0.032
(0.037) (0.039)
Sample size 2665 2664
Four-child families
Second-born -0.0002 0.036
(0.039) (0.042)
Third-born 0.106 * 0.028
(0.054) (0.052)
Fourth-born 0.033 -0.039
(0.063) (0.059)
Sample size 1407 1408
Note: Marginal probabilities from a univariate probit model are
reported. Standard errors corrected for clustering at the family level
are in parentheses. In addition to the independent variables used in
Table 3, specifications include controls for survey year and missing
race/ethnicity, parental education, and family income.
** p < .01; * p < .05; (^) p < .10.
TABLE 11
Estimated Effects of Having an Older Sibling:
National Longitudinal Survey of
Youth--1979 Cohort
Males Females
1979 interview ages 14-21
Ever smoked cigarettes 0.056 ** 0.045 **
(0.015) (0.016)
Ever drank alcohol 0.009 0.032 *
(0.013) (0.014)
Ever smoked marijuana 0.040 * 0.024
(0.018) (0.016)
Ever had sex 0.027 * -0.011
(0.013) (0.015)
1992 interview ages 27-34
Smoked cigarettes daily 0.034 (^) 0.054 **
(0.020) (0.019)
Drank alcohol daily -0.016 -0.013
(0.015) (0.017)
Drank >5 drinks daily 0.006 0.003
(0.004) (0.003)
Smoked marijuana in 0.022 0.007
the past year (0.015) (0.011)
Smoke marijuana in 0.006 0.001
the past month (0.012) (0.008)
Note: Marginal probabilities from a univariate probit
model are reported. Standard errors corrected for clustering
at the family level are in parentheses. In addition to the
independent variables used in Table 3, specifications include
controls for survey year and missing race/ethnicity,
parental education, and family income.
** p < .01; * p < .05; (^) p < .10.