Gender, earnings, and the English skill acquisition of Hispanic workers in the United States.
Mora, Marie T. ; Davila, Alberto
I. INTRODUCTION
Recently, the number of English deficient residents has escalated in
the United States. According to the 1980 and 1990 decennial censuses,
individuals aged five years and older whose reported English verbal
skills are "not well" or "not at all" rose from 4.22
million (2% of the population) to 6.67 million (3%). This percentage is
more pronounced in the public schools. According to Han et al. [1997],
over 2.12 million students (5.1%) of the 41.62 million public school
students in grades K-12 were identified as being limited-English
proficient (LEP) in the 1993-1994 school year. Other estimates, such as
those made by Henderson, Abbott, and Strang [1993], put the public
school LEP share at over 6%.
Increases in the LEP numbers mirror the growth in the immigrant
population as well as the relatively high fertility rates of language
minority Americans. The Hispanic and Asian/Pacific Islander ethnic
groups account for much of this growth, as shown by the National
Research Council [1997]. Hispanics, in fact, are projected by the U.S.
Census Bureau [1996] to become this nation's largest racial/ethnic
minority group in less than a decade. Policywise the growing LEP
population raises economic and social questions concerning, for example,
the best schooling programs for children in this group, as well as the
impact of English proficiency in labor markets. Answers to some of these
questions clearly require detailed and inclusive analyses.(1)
Indeed, many social scientists have linked the ability to communicate
in English to labor market value and occupational selection.(2) The bulk
of this research, however, primarily focuses on men. Less is known on
whether gender differently affects the English proficiency outcomes of
language minorities. Researchers in other social science fields, such as
Stevens [1985, 1986, 1992], Cameron [1992], and Ellis [1994] have
considered gender disparities with respect to the acquisition of English
skills.(3) However, these investigations have not fully implemented some
of the empirical advances made by economists to analyze English
proficiency. Moreover, this extant work tends to focus solely on
gender-related English-skill differentials at a single point in time.
In this study, we use recent empirical strategies in the economics
field to analyze labor market facets associated with the English skill
acquisition of Hispanic men and women. We also employ a two-period
analysis that allows us to observe whether gender differences in the
accumulation of English skills exist. With respect to our previous
discussion, such knowledge facilitates the design of programs aimed at
improving the English fluency of the LEP population.
In particular, we first track synthetic cohorts to investigate if the
English proficiency acquisition made by Hispanic workers during the
1980s varies with respect to gender. We then test whether poor English
skills differently affect the earnings and occupational sorting of
Hispanic men and women. The 1% 1980 and 1990 Public Use Microdata
Samples (PUMS) are utilized in these labor market analyses.
Our findings suggest that English skill procurement varies between
Hispanic men and women, depending on age and schooling location. We also
find that women face a lower English deficiency earnings penalty that
rises more rapidly with education than the penalty obtained by their
otherwise similar male peers. Our occupational selection results
parallel this penalty difference, where English skills serve as a
stronger sorting mechanism for Hispanic women than men. Policy
prescriptions from such analyses suggest that male-oriented studies
overestimate the earnings gains and understate the occupational sorting
associated with acquiring English fluency.
II. ENGLISH SKILL ACQUISITION IN THE LABOR MARKET
Our first objective tests whether women acquire English proficiency
at a different rate than their male peers. All of our empirical work
relies on the 1% PUMS of the 1980 and 1990 U.S. decennial censuses. In
addition to the usual information provided by most large data sets, PUMS
data include categorical information on how well individuals from
minority language households report speaking English; these categories
include: "n/a; only English is spoken at home," "very
well," "well," "not well," and "not at
all."(4) Unfortunately, these data do not contain information on
English reading or writing skills, such that our empirical
investigations pertain to verbal ability.
PUMS Sample Selection
Our PUMS sample contains individuals aged 16-64 years, and includes
immigrant and U.S.-native Hispanic workers who speak English or Spanish
(or both), as well as U.S.-native non-Hispanic whites who only speak
English at home. We drop from our sample individuals reporting
self-employment, work outside of the U.S., military status, work without
pay, enrollment in school, zero earnings, zero usual hours worked, or
zero weeks worked. Finally, we exclude workers with allocated (imputed)
earnings, weeks worked, and usual hours worked per week.
PUMS Sample Summary Statistics for Hispanic Workers
Table I presents selected summary statistics for the Hispanic sample
within each English skill category.(5) Note that with the exception of
the "not at all" category, the percentage of women is higher
in the more proficient categories. This finding reaffirms other social
science research showing a gender differential in English skills (see
footnote 3).
Other figures in Table I conform to previous research on English
proficiency, as in footnote 2. Notably, hourly wages increase with
English fluency (hourly wage = previous year's annual salary
divided by usual weekly hours worked times annual weeks worked); English
proficient workers have more education and less experience on average
than their less English fluent peers;(6) Mexican Americans and Cubans
tend to be overrepresented (and Puerto Ricans and Other Hispanics
underrepresented) in the less proficient categories; and the share of
immigrants inversely corresponds to English fluency.(7)
The Construction of the English Deficiency Index
We next collapse these English skill categories into an English
deficiency index to be used in our analyses:
(1) EDI = (No English is Spoken)
+ [summation of] ([r.sub.j] * [Category.sub.j]) where j = NotWell to
VeryWell;
for j = "Not Well," "Well," "Very
Well," and 0 [less than or equal to] [r.sub.j] [less than or equal
to] 1.
[r.sub.j] represents a normalized parameter ranging from zero to one,
and [Category.sub.j] denotes the jth "intermediate" English
proficiency - a binary variable that takes on the value of zero or one.
The base group (the origin) of EDI contains individuals from monolingual English households (EDI equals zero). EDI equals one for completely
English-deficient individuals (no English spoken). The coefficients on
the intermediate English skill categories may be interpreted as each
fluency level's percentage of"complete" English
deficiency.(8)
To calculate the [r.sub.j]'s, we focus on Hispanics and use
non-linear regression to estimate:
(2) ln(W) = x[[Alpha].sub.1] + (female * x)[[Alpha].sub.2]
+ (90PUMS * x) [[Alpha].sub.3]
+ (female * 90PUMS * x) [[Alpha].sub.4] + e,
where ln(W) represents the natural logarithm of hourly wages, and x
depicts a vector containing EDI, education, experience,
experience-squared, and several census-coded geographic indicator
variables.(9) The terms (female * x), (90PUMS * x), and (female * 90PUMS
* x) denote vectors of the variables in x multiplied to binary variables
indicating if the individual is: female, from the 1990 PUMS sample, and
female in the 1990 PUMS sample. The [[Alpha].sub.i]'s represent
vectors of coefficients to be estimated, and finally the error term is e
[similar to] N(0,[[Sigma].sup.2]I).
This empirical technique allows the constructed EDI to reflect
earnings differences associated with poor English skills unexplained by
education, job experience, and regional and temporal cost of living
differences. The EDI also assesses English skills as a continuum,
providing intuitive insights into the [TABULAR DATA FOR TABLE I OMITTED]
labor market role of English fluency. Table II presents the estimated
[r.sub.j]'s, and the remaining results can be obtained from the
authors.
TABLE II
English Deficiency Index of Hispanic Workers in the 1980 and 1990
PUMS
English Skill Category Coefficient Standard Error
Only speaks English at home 0.000 -
Speaks English "very well" 0.107 0.024
Speaks English "well" 0.392 0.037
Speaks English "not well" 0.856 0.062
No English is spoken 1.000 -
Notes: Only the Hispanic sample is used to estimate this index.
Additional estimations of equations (1) and (2) have been used to
test whether the coefficients on the intermediate English skill
categories significantly changed between the two censuses, which
they did not. The test for the statistical difference was conducted
using the pooled 1980 and 1990 PUMS data in non-linear regression,
and the EDI coefficients were interacted with a binary variable for
90PUMS. The statistical significance was determined by the z-ratio
on the category-90PUMS interactions; the absolute value z-statistics
for "very well," "well," and "not well" are 0.129, 0.110, and 0.387.
Moreover, an F-test reveals that these EDI coefficients as a group
are not statistically different between the censuses; the calculated
F-value is 0.116.
To explore the reliability of EDI, we present selected mean
statistics of this index in Table III. Note that with the exception of
college graduates, women appear slightly more proficient in English than
their male counterparts, maintaining the sample statistics presented
earlier. Other aspects in Table III support extant research in that
individuals educated outside of the U.S. tend to be less fluent in
English than their U.S.-schooled peers; English skills positively relate
to the level of education; and English fluency varies with respect to
age and schooling location. In short, the mean characteristics of EDI
accord with conventional wisdom; hence, EDI serves as a reliable proxy
for English deficiency.
Gender and the English Skill Acquisition of Hispanic Workers
Using EDI, we now test whether gender significantly relates to the
English skill acquisition of Hispanic workers. Even though the PUMS data
are not longitudinal, temporal changes may be approximated through the
use of synthetic cohorts. Synthetic cohort analyses have been utilized
by researchers such as Borjas [1994] in the economics of immigration domain.
To illustrate this technique, individuals between the ages of 16-54
years in 1980 can be synthetically tracked in the 1990 PUMS by those
aged 26-64 years (excluding immigrants who arrived to the U.S. after
1980). We realize that because some immigrants in the 1980 sample will
have emigrated out of the U.S. before 1990, the 1990 PUMS may not
entirely reflect "true" immigrant cohorts. We acknowledge this
potential problem by dividing the cohorts into samples of individuals
solely or partly educated in the U.S., or completely educated abroad.
Presumably, immigrants who attended U.S. schools have a lower emigration likelihood, such that the U.S.schooled synthetic cohorts should be
particularly consistent with the "true" cohorts.
Moreover, because we exclude from our sample students enrolled in
school, individuals attending college in 1980 are not in the 1980 PUMS
sample, but could be in the 1990 PUMS sample. To reduce the shortcomings
arising from this scenario, we further explore three smaller cohorts: I,
J, and K. Cohort I includes workers aged 26-36 years in 1980 or 36-46
years in 1990. Similarly, individuals in Cohort J [Cohort K] were 36-46
[46-54] years old in 1980 or 46-56 [56-64] years old in 1990. We exclude
immigrants migrating to the U.S. after 1980 in all cohorts.
To test whether the English fluency accumulated by Hispanic workers
during the 1980s varies with respect to gender, we estimate:
(3) EDI = [[Delta].sub.1]female + [[Delta].sub.2] 90PUMS
+ [[Delta].sub.3] (female * 90PUMS) + (Other)D + [Epsilon],
where the [[Delta].sub.i]'s represent the coefficients of
interest, and [Epsilon] is the normally distributed stochastic error
term. D denotes a vector of additional coefficients associated with
Other, which contains the additional explanatory variables of education;
potential experience years workers had accrued in 1980 (= experience for
workers in the 1980 PUMS, and experience - 10 for workers in the 1990
PUMS); ethnicity; immigrants' U.S.-tenure (in categories), and the
school location and potential foreign experience of immigrants. Table IV
presents the regression results from estimating equation (3) for the
synthetic cohorts.
[TABULAR DATA FOR TABLE III OMITTED]
The two most important rows in Table IV for our discussion correspond
to the female and female*90PUMS variables. While workers on average gain
English fluency (i.e., become less deficient) even after entering the
labor market, women appear to acquire English skills differently than
men depending on age and schooling location. The younger female workers
schooled in the U.S. enter the labor market with an English fluency
advantage over their otherwise similar male peers. Such an English
proficiency edge, however, appears to subside over time. As with other
human capital, the English fluency advantage seemingly hits diminishing
returns. The oldest cohort (Cohort K) suggests that U.S.-educated women
in this group do not appear to have statistically different English
skills than their male counterparts.
[TABULAR DATA FOR TABLE IV OMITTED]
Immigrant women educated abroad (particularly in the youngest cohort)
initially have an English fluency disadvantage relative to men, although
their English skills progress over time. One interpretation of this
finding has been offered by Espenshade and Fu [1997], who note that
lower English skills of immigrant women may reflect limited past
expectations of female labor market participation. The English fluency
of immigrant women surpassed that of men during the 1980s only in Cohort
K. For our purposes, the synthetic cohorts in Table IV reveal the
presence of gender differentials associated with the acquisition of
English proficiency in the labor market.
The remaining variables reaffirm other work on the correlates of
English skills. Namely, education and its location positively relate to
English skills, the lower English proficiency of immigrants increases
with U.S.tenure, and ethnicity associates with English fluency
differences.
III. ENGLISH SKILLS, GENDER, AND EARNINGS
At this point, we have provided empirical evidence suggesting that
the acquisition of English skills significantly varies between men and
women. We now examine whether this linguistic difference projects into
the labor market via earnings and occupational sorting.
English Skills and the Change in the English Deficiency Earnings
Penalty
To test whether gender influences the earnings penalty associated
with poor English skills, we use the English deficiency index from above
and estimate:
(4) ln(W) = X[[Beta].sub.1] + (female * X)[[Beta].sub.2]
+ (90PUMS * X) [[Beta].sub.3]
+ (female * 90PUMS * X) [[Beta].sub.4] + u,
where the [[Beta].sub.i]'s represent vectors of coefficients to
be estimated, and u is the stochastic error term where u[similar to]N(0,
[[Sigma].sup.2]I). X depicts a vector that includes the variables in
vector x from equation (2) as well as ethnicity, immigrants'
U.S.-tenure (in categories), and the school location and potential
foreign experience of immigrants. The remaining terms in equation (4)
have a similar interpretation to their counterparts in equation (2). The
functional form of equation (4) accounts for earnings differences
between men and women in 1979 and 1989. Table V reports selected
regression results from estimating this earnings equation, with
U.S.-born monolingual-English non-Hispanic whites comprising the base
group. For the sake of brevity, we only report the results associated
with the variables of interest (such as female and EDI); the remaining
results are available upon request from the authors.
The backdrop coefficients on EDI and its interactions support those
found in related studies (see footnote 8). That is, the combined direct
and indirect effects suggest that workers with poor English skills earn
relatively less than their English fluent peers; this earnings penalty
increases with higher levels of schooling (and also with experience in
1989).(10)
Table V also shows that the combined direct and indirect effects
(through schooling) of poor English skills on earnings statistically
differ between women and men. Specifically, Hispanic women face a
significantly different English deficiency earnings penalty than their
male peers, affirming that gender related linguistic differences ensue
in labor market earnings. Most women receive a relatively smaller
earnings penalty that rises more sharply with education. For example,
the 1979 penalty received by an English deficient woman with 10
schooling years is about 13% lower [= 56.1 - (10*0.043)] than the
penalty obtained by her otherwise similar male peers; a female high
school graduate accrues an earnings penalty approximately 4.5% smaller
[= 56.1 - (12*0.043)] than her male counterparts' penalty.
[TABULAR DATA FOR TABLE V OMITTED]
The relatively smaller penalty for women implies that overall
earnings gains associated with enhancing English skills fall short of
the gains estimated from exclusively focussing on men. Hence, studies
solely utilizing men overvalue the increase in earnings associated with
enhanced English fluency for women, particularly for the less educated.
Moreover, our findings provide additional information on the well
documented male-female earnings gap because the unrestricted sample
means underscore this gap for men and women with the same reported
proficiency.
The EDI*90PUMS interaction in Table V suggests that the earnings
penalty of English deficient workers with little education fell
significantly between 1979 and 1989. However, this penalty increased
with respect to education and experience during this time, such that
skilled workers faced a higher EDI earnings penalty than their less
skilled counterparts by 1989. Overall, this finding supports Mora [1998], and further accords with the increase in returns to other human
capital that occurred during the 1980s as discussed, for example, by
Murphy and Welch [1992, 1989]. Note also that during the 1980s, the
earnings penalty of English deficient women increased relative to men,
but its rate of change with respect to schooling fell. In other words,
male/female earnings differences associated with English proficiency
somewhat narrowed during the 1980s, but did not vanish.
Other results from estimating equation (4) are standard, and we only
summarize the highlights here. Education enhances earnings, and [TABULAR
DATA FOR TABLE VI OMITTED] each additional year of experience increases
earnings at a decreasing rate. As expected, these attributes were
compensated with higher labor market rewards in 1989 than in 1979. Also,
women obtain higher returns to schooling and lower returns to experience
than their male counterparts; similar to the EDI results, however, these
differences lessened during the 1980s. Moreover, the returns to
experience significantly decline with foreign job training, although
these lower returns dissipate for female immigrants in 1989.
IV. GENDER, ENGLISH DEFICIENCY, AND OCCUPATIONAL SORTING IN 1990
One possible explanation for the relatively smaller English
deficiency earnings penalty received by women might hinge on the role of
English skills in occupational sorting. To test this proposition, we
estimate a multinomial logit for the Hispanic sample with occupation
(composed of six major categories) as the dependent variable; the base
group of comparison consists of professional, executive, and managerial
jobs. The right-hand side variables include EDI, female, EDI*female, and
the variables in vector X from equation (4). The EDI*female interactions
should indicate whether vocational selection related to English skills
varies with gender.
Table VI reports selected results from estimating this multinomial
logit using the 1990 PUMS. We do not utilize our 1980 sample because of
potential structural changes that may have occurred in the labor market
during the 1980s. However, we replicate our results for 1980 (not
shown); the same basic results hold.
Table VI indicates that poor English fluency occupationally sorts
away from professional positions, reaffirming earlier studies like Jasso
and Rosenzweig [1989] and McManus [1990]. Moreover, this sorting
significantly favors women in all vocations except technical, sales, and
administrative support trades. English proficiency seems to filter women
beyond its effect on the careers of men, such that English fluent women
are occupationally "crowded." This crowding might diminish the
relative English skill returns of women, which is consistent with our
earnings analysis in Table V. As noted by Bergmann [1989], if
occupational segregation exists between men and women, market forces
decree two sets of wages based on gender. Then, if women are
vocationally crowded with respect to skill (say, English proficiency),
their smaller job array should yield lower skill returns owing to excess
female labor supply in these occupations. In short, our findings for men
underscore the influence of English skills on female job selection.
Other occupational findings reaffirm extant research. For example,
the employment odds of women fall short of men's odds in the blue
collar sector relative to professional positions, ceteris paribus.
Moreover, education and job experience affect career choice, where
skilled workers tend to be employed in white collar vocations. Finally,
immigration status, foreign human capital, and ethnicity relate to
occupational sorting.
V. CONCLUDING REMARKS
Although research in the economics literature on the labor market
value of English proficiency traditionally focuses on men, other social
scientists have recognized that linguistic skills vary with respect to
gender. We empirically reaffirm the importance of this claim. For
example, we find that Hispanic female workers maintain their relative
verbal command of English over time (albeit at a diminishing rate) if
they were educated in the U.S. Female immigrants educated abroad are not
as proficient as their male peers, but appear to have acquired English
fluency at a faster clip during the 1980s. These results not only show
that the acquisition of English proficiency occurs in the labor market,
but that such skill procurement varies between language minority men and
women.
Gender differentials along the English-skill dimension also surface
in earnings and occupational sorting. We find that English deficient
Hispanic women receive a lower earnings penalty that rises more rapidly
with education than the penalty obtained by their otherwise similar male
peers. This penalty difference relates to our occupational selection
results, where English skills act as a stronger sorting mechanism for
Hispanic women. The relative vocational "crowding" of English
proficient women may diminish their English proficiency returns (and
hence reduce their English deficiency earnings penalty).
One possible explanation for the gender differential in English
fluency may be that language minority women have a greater necessity to
acquire basic majority language skills to effectively compete in the
labor market. Indeed, sociolinguists such as Cameron [1992] have
suggested that many language minority women perceive a more narrow
occupational array than men because of fewer construction and other
physically demanding job opportunities; our occupational sorting results
fit with this claim. Another explanation of the apparent gender
difference in English proficiency accumulation may involve females'
expectations of household activities; for example, Stevens [1985, 1986]
observes that women transfer language skills to children more than men.
Future research should explore this issue by focussing on the
intergenerational properties of majority language skill acquisition.
In short, our findings suggest that research ignoring gender may
yield an incomplete picture of the English skill outcomes in the U.S.
Moreover, male-oriented labor market studies may overestimate the
potential earnings gains and understate the occupational sorting
associated with enhancing the English fluency of women. Given the
growing number of limited-English proficient individuals, labor market,
educational, and immigration policies targeting language issues will
affect much larger segments of the population. As such, underlying
aspects of these policies should be fully addressed to understand the
potentially far-reaching economic and social consequences.
ABBREVIATIONS
EDI: English Deficiency Index LEP: Limited-English Proficient PUMS:
Public Use Microdata Samples
1. While our study concentrates on the labor market implications of
English fluency, educational policies designed for LEP children also
have increasing importance. In fact, evidence suggests that LEP
individuals tend to be relatively young; a perusal of census data
reveals that the share of 5-year-olds [15-year-olds] in the English
skill distribution monotonically decreases [increases] with English
proficiency. Lopez and Mora [1997] and Glenn [1997] represent recent
studies that discuss some potential long-term consequences of programs
like bilingual education.
2. The study conducted by McManus, Gould, and Welch [1983] represents
a classic analysis of English proficiency in the U.S. labor market. More
recent work includes Jasso and Rosenzweig [1989]; McManus [1990, 1985];
Davila, Bohara, and Saenz [1993]; Chiswick and Miller [1995]: and Mora
[1998]. Examples of empirical investigations into socioeconomic factors
related to English fluency can be found in McManus, Gould, and Welch
[1983]; Chiswick and Miller [1995]; Espenshade and Fu [1997]; Espinosa
and Massey [1997]; and Mora [1998].
3. For example, Ellis [1994] notes that women seem to have a greater
potential for acquiring standard proficiency in a second language
because they rapidly adopt newer linguistic forms and tend to rely on
their first language skills as a cognitive aid more than men.
4. The PUMS questionnaire first asks whether an individual speaks a
non-English language at home. If not, the individual disregards the
question on English fluency. We realize some individuals may speak a
non-English language outside of the home while only speaking English at
home; however, we feel that the impact of this potential bias, if any,
on our results is quite small.
5. The 1990 PUMS data provide statistical weights to be used in
analyzing the data; we utilize these weights in all of our analyses. The
number of observations presented, however, is not weighted. The 1980
PUMS data are self-weighted.
6. The 1990 PUMS data do not provide the actual number of schooling
years; instead, they present the highest education level attained in
categories. These categories closely parallel the schooling coding
presently utilized by the Current Population Survey (CPS); however, in
February 1990, the CPS provided education both continuously and
categorically. Because the same individuals gave information for both
schooling codes, Park [1994] estimates schooling years using the
educational categorical coding; we use Park's translation to
measure completed schooling years. Experience equals age - education -
5. We realize that because of differences in the labor force
participation between men and women, the experience proxy may not
completely capture "true" labor market training.
Unfortunately, the PUMS do not provide an alternative.
7. Because the labor-market consequences of job training and
education may differ when acquired abroad, we construct foreign
experience to approximate the maximum potential years of foreign
training. Specifically, we compare the minimum number of years since
migration (using migration-interval categories consistent between both
PUMS of 0-5, 6-10, 11-20, 21-30, and 31 or more years) with total years
of potential training. If the immigrant's working years exceed the
time since migration, the difference between experience and U.S. tenure
comprises foreign work years; otherwise, we assume the individual did
not work abroad. Similarly, we compare the individual's estimated
schooling completion age with these migration intervals to approximate
the location of educational attainment. Although imprecisely measured,
such variables provide additional insight into the labor-market
experiences of Hispanic immigrants in the U.S.
8. This empirical technique was cultivated by McManus, Gould, and
Welch [1983]. See McManus [1990, 1985] and Mora [1998] for examples of
other work that utilize this method.
9. These areas include the West (base group), Mountain, West North
Central, West South Central, East North Central, East South Central,
South Atlantic, Middle Atlantic, and New England regions.
10. To illustrate, Hispanic men with ten schooling years who do not
speak English earned approximately 29% less { = [0.176 - (0.047 * 10)] *
100} than their otherwise similar peers from monolingual English
households in 1979. The EDI of individuals with "moderate"
English skills can be found by multiplying together the coefficient from
Table II and the complete EDI earnings penalty. For example, men with
ten schooling years who speak English "very well" earned about
3% less (= 0.107 * 29) in 1979 than their male counterparts from
monolingual English households.
REFERENCES
Bergmann, Barbara R. "Does the Market for Women's Labor
Need Fixing?" Journal of Economic Perspectives, Winter 1989, 43-60.
Borjas, George J. "The Economics of Immigration." Journal
of Economic Literature, December 1994, 1,667-717.
Cameron, Deborah. Feminism and Linguistic Theory, 2nd ed. New York:
St. Martin's Press, 1992.
Chiswick, Barry R. and Paul W. Miller. "The Endogeneity between
Language and Earnings: International Analyses." Journal of Labor
Economics, April 1995, 246-88.
Davila, Alberto, Alok K. Bohara and Rogelio Saenz. "Accent
Penalties and the Earnings of Mexican Americans." Social Science
Quarterly, 74(4), 1993, 902-16.
Ellis, Rod. The Study of Second Language Acquisition. Oxford: Oxford
University Press, 1994.
Espenshade, Thomas J. and Haishan Fu. "An Analysis of
English-Language Proficiency Among U.S. Immigrants." American
Sociological Review, April 1997, 288-305.
Espinosa, Kristin E. and Douglas S. Massey. "Determinants of
English Proficiency among Mexican Migrants to the United States."
International Migration Review, Spring 1997, 28-50.
Glenn, Charles L. "What Does the National Research Council Study
Tell Us About Educating Language Minority Children?" READ
Abstracts, May 1997. Amherst, Mass.: Institute for Research in English
Acquisition and Development.
Han, Mei, David Baker, Carlos Rodriguez and Peggy Quinn. A Profile of
Policies and Practices for Limited English Proficient Students:
Screening, Methods, Program Support, and Teacher Training. Washington,
D.C.: U.S. Government Printing Office, 1997.
Henderson, Allison, Catherine Abbott and William Strang. Summary of
the Bilingual Education State Educational Agency Program Survey of
States' Limited English Proficient Persons and Available
Educational Services: 1991-1992. Washington, D.C.: Office of Bilingual
Education and Minority Language Affairs, August 1993.
Jasso, Guillermina and Mark R. Rosenzweig. "Language Skill
Acquisition, Labor Markets and Locational Choice: The Foreign-Born in
the United States, 1900 and 1980," in Migration and Labor Market
Adjustment, edited by J. Van Dijk, F. Hendrik, H. W. Herzog Jr, and A.M.
Schlottman. Dordrecht, The Netherlands: Kluwer Academic Press, 1989,
217-39.
Lopez, Mark Hugo and Marie T. Mora. "Bilingual Education and
Labor Market Earnings Among Hispanics: Evidence Using High School and
Beyond." Paper presented at the 19th Annual Association for Public
Policy Analysis and Management (APPAM) research conference, Washington,
D.C., November 1997.
McManus, Walter, William Gould and Finis Welch. "Earnings of
Hispanic Men: The Role of English Language Proficiency." Journal of
Labor Economics, 1(2), 1983, 101-30.
McManus, Walter S. "Labor Market Costs of Language Disparity: An
Interpretation of Hispanic Earnings Differences." American Economic
Review, 75(4), 1985, 818-27.
-----. "Labor Market Effects of Language Enclaves: Hispanic Men
in the United States." Journal of Human Resources, 25(2), 1990,
228-52.
More, Marie T. "Did the English Deficiency Earnings Penalty
Change for Hispanic Men between 1979 and 1989?" Social Science
Quarterly, forthcoming, 1998.
Murphy, Kevin M. and Finis Welch. "Wage Premiums for College
Graduates: Recent Growth and Possible Explanations." Educational
Researcher, 18, 1989, 17-26.
-----. "The Structure of Wages." Quarterly Journal of
Economics, February 1992, 285-326.
National Research Council. The New Americans: Economic, Demographic,
and Fiscal Effects of Immigration, edited by J.P. Smith and B.
Edmonston. Washington, D.C.: National Academy Press, 1997.
Park, Jin Heum. "Estimation of Sheepskin Effects and Returns to
Schooling Using the Old and New CPS Measures of Educational
Attainment." Working Paper No. 338, Department of Economics,
Industrial Relations Section, Princeton University, August 1994.
Stevens, Gillian. "Nativity, Intermarriage, and Mother-Tongue
Shift." American Sociological Review, February 1985, 74-83.
-----. "Sex Differences in Language Shift in the United
States." Sociology and Social Research, October 1986, 31-36.
-----. "The Social and Demographic Context of Language Use in
the United States." American Sociological Review, April 1992,
171-85.
U.S. Bureau of the Census. Resident Population of the United States:
Middle Series Projections, 2001-2005, by Sex, Race, and Hispanic Origin,
with Median Age. Washington, D.C., March 1996.