Iban Art: Sexual Selection and Severed Heads--Weaving, Sculpture, Tattooing and Other Arts of the Iban of Borneo.
Wadley, Reed L.
Iban Art: Sexual Selection and Severed Heads--Weaving, Sculpture,
Tattooing and Other Arts of the Iban of Borneo, by Michael Heppell,
Limbang anak Melaka, and Enyan anak Usen (2005). Leiden/Amsterdam: C.
Zwartenkot-Art Books/KIT Publishers, pp. 180, 55.00 [euro].
In this wonderfully illustrated and colorful book, Heppell brings
together disparate strands of thought on art, war, and sexual
competition and choice to create a general argument of why art,
primarily weaving, became so elaborate in Iban society. The chapters run
through Iban culture and history, cosmology, weaving, carving, and
tattooing. Given the weight placed on weaving, the book might well have
been titled Iban Weaving and Sexual Selection, and it is unfortunate
that the cover photo is of a tattooed "warrior" wearing a
clouded leopard-skin jacket. A better cover might have showcased one of
the beautiful pua' kumbu' woven by co-author Enyan anak Usen,
but the press editors may have wanted something more visually romantic
for their largely European audience than a "mere" blanket.
The purposes behind those blankets and other weavings are many, and
illustrate the central role in Iban life that such items might have
played: (1) "captur[ing] spirits, [and thus] protecting people from
a teeming world of malevolence and misanthropy" (p. 37); serving as
a protective barrier against malevolent spirits and as a medium for men
seeking dreams from the spirit world (p. 46); "to reinforce
curses" (p. 48); "to remove disturbing natural phenomena such
as an eclipse" (p. 49); and for "mundane purposes" (p.
50). "Inciting" men to brave and risky deeds was, according to Heppell, the "most profound" of these (p. 50), and this forms
part of his general thesis regarding the link between art and sexual
competition and choice.
Heppell has obviously put a great deal of thought and energy into
this effort (based in no small part on his field experience), from
collecting the many color photos of art that greatly enhance the text to
the writing of this wide-ranging narrative, which covers description,
theory, and personal account. (I was pleased to learn that
Heppell's first view of Sarawak was similar to my own, and at about
the same time, from the deck of a ship in the early 1970s--he, on his
way to conduct doctoral research in the Batang Ai, and I, a 10-year old
boy, eyes wide from tales of White Rajahs and headhunters.) That being
said, it is hard to decide if the book is meant principally as a serious
scholarly treatise or something visually pleasing for the coffee table.
It is perhaps best described as some combination of the two, but a
scholarly thesis must hold up to much higher standards than a
coffee-table book, and I must confess being not a little frustrated as I
read through it. Heppell's technical and ritual renderings are
largely consistent with what I know of Iban weaving and the like
acquired secondarily in the field, though his reliance on South
Kalimantan cosmological models (e.g., pp. 25-26) to interpret design
among the Iban does not ring particularly true. I would, however, defer
to others to evaluate his descriptions and analyses of these things specifically as there are a number of issues that go beyond the art
itself.
Despite having been published in the Netherlands, there are no
references to primary or secondary Dutch sources in the various
historical strands Heppell traces. He relies on such scholars as King
(1993) and Pringle (1970) for references to Indonesian Borneo in
history, though their work with Dutch archival materials was limited.
This remains a severe limitation of English language scholarship on
Borneo, and a particular weakness of those working in East Malaysia and
Brunei. Although I know of no Dutch study of (what has unfortunately
come to be known as) "Ibanic" art (largely because I have
never looked), colonial officials were keen to create collections and
often wrote about them. Even a cursory survey of the library at the
KITLV in Leiden or the National Library in the Hague might have turned
up some gems. In addition, I myself viewed a collection of material
culture at the Museum Nusantara, Delft--Iban knock-offs from 1920s Nanga
Badau in West Kalimantan. This might only have added to Heppell's
burden of having so much stuff to work through, but it points to the
possibilities across the border.
This general ignorance of what lies over the border is not just
limited to historical issues; for example, Heppell refers to women who
came from "across the Sarawak/Kalimantan border on the Emperan
River" (p. 63). From his nearby location in the upper Batang Ai
(where mere tens of kilometers separate cross-border communities), it is
likely that Heppell dutifully recorded in his fieldnotes that the women
were from ai' emperan, which he then translated literally as
'Emperan River.' But there is no such river, and never was. As
I have been describing for 16 years in these pages and elsewhere,
"the Emperan" refers to the relatively flat country between
the uplands of the Empanang and Kantu' rivers on the west, the
Embaioh River on the east, the Kedang Hills to the north along the
border, and the wide expanse of the Kapuas Lakes to the south. Thus,
ai' emperan refers to the waters or region of the Emperan, not a
particular stream, just as ai' belanda and ai" sarawak
referred historically to Dutch West Borneo and the Brooke territory,
respectively.
I have numerous quibbles on various issues of Iban history, but
will only touch on two points here in illustration: First, Heppell
states that the Saribas and Skrang Iban had "virtually
annihilated" the Undup Iban before the Brookes arrived. In fact, in
the early 1800s, the Undup had fled their homeland, along with their
Kantu' allies, to seek protection from the sultan of Selimbau on
the Kapuas River, only to return after James Brooke had pacified the
Saribas and Skrang. Second, Heppell implies that the principal
motivation for heads drove Iban to attack other Iban (p. 37), though an
exploration of oral history shows that inter-Iban headhunting stemmed
overwhelmingly from disputes gone awry. Even the Iban-Kantu' wars,
according to my sources in the Nanga Badau area of West Kalimantan,
originated in adultery and the inevitable revenge. Benedict
Sandin's (1968) work also shows this quite clearly.
Along another vein, Heppell's seemingly post-modern
sensibility in including his chief informants and friends as co-authors
is countered by his truly unfortunate reliance on the ethnographic present in describing the Iban over the period between the 1850s to the
1950s. It is not just that the use of the present tense may misrepresent the Iban today, the usual post-modern concern that may lead an
unsophisticated reader to assume that is the way the Iban are now and
always have been. It is more a matter of properly historicizing the
themes Heppell deals with: What advantage does the present tense offer
that is not better met by providing a proper historical context and not
implying, even inadvertently, that the 100--year period was somehow
uniform throughout the Iban population? Certainly, he is limited by the
collections at his disposal (which, like the one in Delft, were
established during that long period), but the use of past tense to
describe events of the past serves to highlight change and continuity to
the present. It is not a trivial matter and only underscores
Heppell's statement that "[h]istory ... [has] served the Iban
poorly" (p. 165).
This is particularly telling as Heppell applies his present tense
inconsistently. For example, "the Iban kept extensive genealogies.
Though much has now been forgotten ..." (p. 25, my emphases); or it
is missing entirely: For instance, "[s]urvival was an important
concern of each Iban group" (p. 36); they "had no means of
magnification" (p. 60); [b]y the end of the Second World War,
sungkit threads were usually purchased rather than dyed" (p. 84);
and "[m]ost women enjoyed a good romp, but some were choosy"
(p. 113). He shifts entirely to past tense in describing events of the
1960s (p. 81) and in disparate paragraphs referring to his selected
period (p. 85, 91, 99, 103). Furthermore, although he is really in his
element in Chapter 5 as he describes cloth designs, their meaning, and
how meaning and design names change as women copy and transport designs
across river systems (pp. 73, 77), Heppell's rounding on Traude
Gavin's work on contemporary Iban weaving (e.g., 2003) is a bit
disconcerting, especially given the temporal differences that lie
between their research. Has his reliance on the present tense for a long
historical period led him to conflate his fictive present for
Gavin's literal present (i.e., late 20th century)? He seems, at the
outset, to be comparing apples and oranges as the reasons for weaving,
not to mention materials and designs have changed so much since the
1950s. (Women throughout the Emperan today, for instance, routinely copy
designs from pua' acquired by their traveling husbands, and
don't necessarily know or care about the design names. "Oh,
it's a design from the Batang Rejang. I don't know what
it's actually called," some have told me.)
Heppell's main scholarly thesis, and perhaps weakest part of
the book, concerns the application of Darwinian sexual selection to Iban
weaving and warfare. This was reflected in the "cyclical
trinity" (p. 32)--headhunting, rice cultivation, and human
fertility, with women being central to each. Indeed, according to
Heppell, it was women's selection of lifelong mates that was
critical as the burden fell on them "to choose men who are more
likely to ensure their and their offspring's survival and will
reinvigorate the family gene pool" (p. 32). As the nurturing,
creative force in Iban society, it was women who were central to rice
farming and, through their weaving, incited men to war.
For men, "heads enhanced survival" and served as
"fitness indicators" (p. 36). Fitness refers to the biological
condition of possessing qualities that have proved reproductively
successful in the past and thus have been naturally selected. (The term
"fitness" is occasionally used inconsistently, however; for
example, "heads were required to demonstrate a man's fitness
to marry" [p. 18], in this sense a synonym for
"suitability" [also p. 95].) Men's incentives for
successful headhunting were to enhance their status, both with young
women for courtship and marriage and with other men for influence and
authority (p. 41). But it was women's woven cloths that demanded
that the men go to war (p. 43), while at the same time displaying the
skill and intelligence of the women, qualities men would have looked for
in mates (p. 92, 121, 166). As he states in a simple equation,
"beautiful cloths = heads = primacy for sexual selection" (p.
167).
Heppell routinely asserts a link between weaving/headhunting and
sexual selection; for example, regarding war jackets, an enemy would
know the identity of the man's guardian spirit, and the quality of
his genes as only great weavers could invoke powerful motifs, and his
ancestors' success in headhunting as "great weavers marry
successful headhunters" (p. 87). But this merely repeats the
supposition with no real evidence presented. Surely the thesis is
logical intuitively, but intuition is not proof, and this is coming from
one who is sympathetic to such arguments. What would he have to show?
For starters, that great weavers consistently married and reproduced
with successful headhunters; second, that those skills were consistently
transmitted culturally to their children; and third, that all that meant
more children of higher quality regarding intelligence, etc. Much of
this, however, goes far beyond the information available to any of us
working in Borneo, especially for historical periods. The fact that the
Iban expanded their territory rapidly in the 19th and early 20th
centuries indirectly supports this notion, but only circumstantially.
(The same applies to his assertion that tattooing "fast tracks a
man to a girl's lofty boudoir" [p. 115]. Do or did tattooed
men enjoy more mating success? This may well be, but the claim is merely
asserted, not supported with even indirect evidence.) Given these
evidentiary problems, a good deal more caution in making such arguments
should have been in order; otherwise, they become merely sociobiological "just-so stories," of which we have plenty already.
Likewise, his more proximally connected statement that
"[a]rtistic flair makes both males and females desirable" (p.
95) is equally asserted. "Flair" would have been one thing for
the youth who were in the process of mate selection, but who made the
more powerful cloths? If the great weavers were older and married, their
weaving could not have signaled any sort of sexual selection. It was not
the young, unmarried indo' dara who produced the most powerful
cloths or who had "their hands tattooed after the ngar
ritual," as such work was supernaturally dangerous and required
considerable experience. Indeed, a woman's weaving became, as she
aged, paradoxically more simple in design (due to fading eyesight) and
more powerful spiritually (p. 60). Contrary to Heppell's claim,
such weavers "were [not] the very women who [would] shine in the
'sexual selection stakes'" (p. 109); they had made their
choices years before. Obviously much more than sexual selection is going
on.
That things of art are meant to attract our attention should not
imply that they are all meant to attract mates; art is a form of
communication, and as with language, we use art in multiple ways. That
Iban weaving was costly and communicative should not be in doubt, but
was sexual selection the primary force behind it? The only reference to
evolutionary forces and art that Heppell cites is Geoffrey Miller's
(2000) book, The Mating Mind, in which he argues that virtually all art
originated in and is replicated for mate competition and choice. This
reliance on single sources is unfortunate as not only are there
substantive criticisms of Miller's general thesis, but also
alternative hypotheses on the evolution of art that are thereby left
unconsidered (e.g., Coe 2003). Likewise, though less seriously, he
argues exclusively from Zahavi's (1997) "handicap
principle" concerning the energetically costly traits that
nonetheless enhance mating success (e.g., the peacock's tail or the
bower bird's elaborate bower). This ignores a growing strand of
research in evolutionary anthropology on what is now termed "costly
signaling," stemming from Zahavi's insight, which has been
applied to such disparate things as hunting and religion (e.g., Bliege
Bird et al. 2001; Hawkes and Bliege Bird 2002). (Curiously, Heppell does
not cite Darwin's seminal work [1989] on sexual selection, though
he does reference, for other purposes, The Origin of Species [1909].)
Finally, the design of the text itself makes hard what should be
smooth reading, having neither indentation to mark paragraphs nor
section breaks to identify sometimes abrupt shifts in topic (e.g., pp.
83, 85, 86, 91, 99). Generally, Dutch graphic art is unparalleled in its
creativity, but the press editors appear to have opted for form over
function in the text layout. In addition, the glossary is excessively
short and does not include a large number of the terms used in the text
(e.g., engkudu and lemba, p. 91; anak umbong, p. 93), rendering it less
than useful for scholarly work. In other places in the text, Iban
equivalents are not given (e.g., the Parishia tree, p. 131). This leaves
one wondering further as to the scholarly versus coffee-table status of
the book. Nonetheless and with all the heavy caveats above, this is a
truly beautiful volume, its chief attraction being both the full color
pictures that such a book cannot do without and the author's
extensive personal insight into the Iban world (Reed L. Wadley,
Department of Anthropology, University of Missouri-Columbia, USA).
Response to Reed Wadley's Review of Iban Art
There are two major themes in the book. First is the link between
art, headhunting and sexual selection, the evidence for which is
provided by the texts. The second theme relates to the decorating of
flat surfaces and carving figures. Women are restricted to the former
due to their "unpredictable nature." The evidence for the
second theme is also provided by texts. As Limbang said about
identifying powerful cloths: "Refer to the text! (julok)".
Given that the texts are central, it is a pity that Professor Wadley
totally ignores them in his review. They have a lot to say about
weaving, warfare and sexual selection. Instead, Professor Wadley asserts
that I rely on a single source, in this case Geoffrey Miller's The
Mating Mind, which is entirely peripheral to my argument. I reference
Miller just over one page from the end of the book, stating: "In
this regard, the Iban strongly illustrate Geoffrey Miller's thesis
in his book The Mating Mind that the basic mechanism for the evolution
of art was sexual selection." No more.
Professor Wadley asserts that I rely on South Kalimantan
cosmological models to interpret Iban design. In fact, I refer to them
only to suggest that the Iban appear to have traces of these creation
myths, which leads to an understated theme of the book, that women,
through their cloths, might have driven a major shift in Iban religious
beliefs to one in which Singalang Burong took center stage. Iban cloth
design is inspired from Panggau, which has little to do with South
Kalimantan myths. (As a matter of interest, Batang Ai myths have the
Iban starting their journey northwards from Ketapang in the south).
This is a book about Iban art with a touch about religion. Much of
Professor Wadley's review skirts around the margins of the book.
He does, however, take to task the evolutionary approach of the
book, which was always likely to be controversial, citing it as
"perhaps the weakest part of the book." Essentially, he states
that there is no evidence presented to support the thesis that weaving
and headhunting have a function of sexual selection and gives his view
of the kind of evidence required.
The evidence presented in the book is the texts, which Professor
Wadley makes no mention of in his review. The texts present an ideal
situation, but one which was followed by high ranking Iban. I had
intended to pursue the idea in a paper, as the book was intended for a
wider audience than the narrowly academic. But there is evidence
available which would not require the kind of historical census that
Professor Wadley indicates is necessary. The tusut, for example, are
evidence--particularly in the Saribas where warriors constantly were
married to great weavers. The anak umbong, after all, withdrew to her
attic to weave and there to await her warrior hero to come and claim
her. Another piece of evidence would be an inventory of bilek in any
longhouse from the center to the ulu end. That would reveal that where
there were heads, there were boxes full of weaving. In ili bilek, there
were usually no heads and many households with no weaving. The ulu bilek
were generally the most successful households, paying testimony to the
basic argument of the book.
I talked of fitness indicators. I did so in a cultural sense. With
the Iban, there are unfitness indicators which I would argue, indirectly
support my case. They refer both to intelligence and physique. There are
Iban who are very unlikely to marry due to intellectual or physical
defects and, in the past, I would speculate, unlikely to mate. In the
longhouse in which I did my fieldwork, there were a number (and at that
time this was a longhouse very much in my anthropological present).
There was an otherwise very attractive woman with a club foot living in
an ulu bilek. She was openly called agu '. Her family ensured that
no man visited her at night and she remained a virgin in her late
thirties. There was an intellectually disadvantaged but physically
strong male who certainly did want to visit indu' dara'. They
rejected him, but he persisted. He was put away by the longhouse for his
troubles. There was a blind woman and an intellectually disadvantaged
woman, both of whom never married. The former remained a virgin, though
the latter was visited by military personnel from time to time and had
three illegitimate children. There was a man in his early twenties with
a genetically damaged hip who was also never able to ngayap.
Interestingly, apart from the first mentioned woman, all the others came
from ill bilek.
While the texts call for powerful cloths, you don't have to
have woven one to demonstrate talent (and therefore intelligence).
Mozart's talent was well-known by his teens, though his great works
did not materialize until later. The bachelors, especially those from
other longhouses, would examine the kain a young woman was wearing and
would find it very easy to establish whether or not it had been woven by
her.
I think that the Iban do present a good case for the exponents of
the idea that the early function of art was as a marker of intelligence.
The Iban have ritualized the requirement for cloths, which is the usual
cultural response to something deemed very important, just as they had
done with the requirements for heads. It is part of their genius.
Finally, one aspect about knowledge and scholarship, especially in
the case of Dayak art, given how few people have written about it, is
that it advances through people sharing information and through
challenging what has been written if a person has evidence to the
contrary. Professor Wadley, for example, finds that my technical and
ritual renderings are largely consistent with what he knows of Iban
weaving without mentioning the instances where they were not consistent
so that we could all learn about inaccuracies or differentiation in the
Emperan and elsewhere. At the end, after earlier "quibbles"
have become "heavy caveats" he surprisingly finds some value
in the author's "extensive personal insight into the Iban
world." It is a pity that some indication of what that might be was
not given in the review so that the less informed reader might be guided
by someone who also has extensive insights into much the same Iban world
as me (Michael Heppell).
Some Further Comments by Your Editor
Leaving aside the debate that Reed and Michael have joined here, I
would like to take a somewhat different tack.
Reading Michael's book (and I had the pleasure to read several
parts of it in draft some years ago, as well as the final published
version more recently) raised in my mind, very vividly, the question of
how does it come about that some communities produce impressive works of
art? Why is it that at certain times and places a peoples' creative
energies seem to overflow, materializing themselves, perhaps, in music
or poetry, weaving, architecture, or metalwork? Or, possibly, in many
forms at once?
As museum collections testify, Iban society, down through the early
decades of the twentieth century, was remarkably creative, most
especially, perhaps, in ikat weaving, but also in other art forms as
well, including, not the least, although less accessible to non-Iban
audiences, epics, ritual liturgy, and other oral poetic arts. How did
this come about?
Historically, we know that the Iban, for a span of some three
hundred years, were phenomenally successful, rapidly expanding in
numbers and territorial extent across a large swath of west-central
Borneo. There would seem to have been, as Heppell suggests, an aesthetic
dimension to this success. Where Michael Heppell's book succeeds, I
think, is in depicting a plausible connection between a once vigorously
expansive society and the remarkable works of art its members produced.
As he tells us, "before 1950, a close scrutiny of any longhouse,
especially on a festive occasion, would be rewarded with decoration and
many objects of great beauty. Arrive as an eligible bachelor or spinster and private museums would be yours to enjoy as you went from apartment
to apartment enjoying the bidding of the occupants to come inside and
eat" (p. 165).
The key to this connection, Heppell asserts, was that, at some
point in the past, "the Iban ritualized their expansionist tendencies" (p. 21). This they did by making headhunting the focus
of a ritual cult, hence the "severed heads" of the book's
subtitle. The taking of enemy heads was linked by means of ritual to
agricultural and human fertility in what Heppell calls a "cyclical
trinity," involving, as its interconnected parts, head taking, rice
growing, and life renewal. Art was an integral part of ritual and hence
"inseparable from the religious ideas inspiring it" (p. 25).
At the culmination of this cult were "great festivals celebrating a
warrior's achievement" in which the gods were invoked and
became temporarily present. During these festivals, woven cloth, in
particular, played a vital role. Pieces of cloth were hung from the
gallery walls, covered offerings, and enveloped the shrines at which the
gods were received. "Textiles [thus] link[ed] the Iban to their
gods. Like a mirror catching the sun, textiles inform the Celestial
Deities of some mortal activity that requires their involvement"
(p. 41). Hence, they "were made to dazzle divine and human
eyes" (p. 44). Their designs were "invested with meaning and
energy" and were intended to capture the "power" of
whatever they represented (p. 45). But, above all, Heppell argues, their
effect was to incite men to acts of daring. At the same time, they
exalted those who succeeded. "In the competitive world of the Iban,
[art] was a sign of accomplishment" (p. 166).
Art also, Heppell maintains, brought talented men and women
together. Every ambitious man sought a talented woman in marriage. Here,
Heppell is certainly right that, even now, competence is highly
regarded, however it is achieved and in whatever the field of endeavor.
Inversely, except for the sake of humor, ineptitude is scorned. In the
Saribas where I did my own fieldwork, lengthy genealogies are preserved,
and these certainly suggest that competence, whether as a weaver, expert
farmer, public speaker, or whatever, conferred marital advantages, and
that these advantages generally accrued to entire family lines and
became matters of utmost concern whenever marriages were contemplated.
There were, of course, exceptions, for example, famous bards who were
blind and so remained unmarried. Otherwise, certainly, families
remembered and took pride in the talents and accomplishments of their
genealogical forebears. Here, weaving, again, was closely linked to
achieved status. As Heppell notes, once a woman completed a prestigious
cloth, it became an heirloom and so an object of family wealth and
spiritual power. Henceforth it was displayed on ritual occasions and its
powerful designs testified not only to the skill of the woman who
created it but to the past sponsorship by family members of important
status-confirming rituals.
The Iban, as Heppell stresses, admire multitalented people. During
my own fieldwork, my young son became a great admirer of the Tuai
Rumah's three sons, who were from 3 to 10 years older than he was
and were all seemingly capable of doing everything a young boy might
wish to do, such as capturing and making pets of wild animals. Once over
several weeks, the Tuai Rumah, who was himself an exemplar of
multi-competence, taught his three sons, with my own son looking on, how
to build a canoe. He began with a lesson in how to select and prepare
the best possible wood. This was followed by a series in how to
carefully measure, fashion, and fit together each piece of the canoe so
that the resulting perau was light in the water, but stable, a pleasure
to handle as well as to look at. The basic message was that should you
turn your hand to something, you should do it well, never indifferently,
and strive to make the object something to be proud of, that others will
admire. Artistic genius clearly lived on in the 1970s and 80s in the
upper Paku.
The coming of white colonists and missionaries, Heppell writes,
"heralded the death of Iban art." Headhunting and territorial
expansion were stopped and Iban were set against Iban.
"Western-style education and wealth were to be more important than
hard work, courage and self-made art" (p. 167). Paid labor had no
time for artistic talent. "The memory bank was quickly stripped
bare by dealers buying for collectors and museums in the west seeking
only the old" (p. 168). Young artists had no incentive to invest
time in works of art, while, in any event, with religious conversion,
"the inspiration for such works was slowly extinguished" (p.
168).
Today, the arts that Heppell describes in his book are fast
disappearing (1) as is the former cultural setting that once sustained
them. Several days before writing these comments, I received a poignant
reminder of this in an email message from a very dear Iban friend. In
his letter, the writer, Jantan Umbat, described a journey that he and
his wife had just made to Tarum Longhouse, in the Saratok District, to
pay their last respects, and that of my wife and myself, to a great Iban
bard (lemambang) and master woodcarver, Renang anak Jabing, the son,
too, of a renowned shaman, who, many years earlier, had been, for me, a
major informant. The old riverside Tarum longhouse, of which I have
still a faint memory, was years ago replaced by a severely barren
concrete structure erected immediately alongside the main road. Jantan
writes in his letter:
I entered the longhouse and was very, very surprised to see the
coffin of our hero laid down on the ruai without a sapat. Nobody
wept around him, not to say to chant the sabak. Only his poor wife
who could not recognize anybody after her minor stroke sat quietly
beside the middle post. I came in, shook hands with her, said a
prayer ... Then I went to the bilik where many friends of his sat
... I was welcomed by the son ... and expressed our sadness, my
family's and yours, over the passing away of the man.
I think there was no harm to have the sapat ... It looked weird
without [it]! Well, things change with modernization.
Indeed, they do. Until recently, grieving for the dead without a
sapat, an enclosure made of ikat textiles to surround the
deceased's body, would have been unthinkable. Today, the Tarum
Longhouse is Christian, as are virtually all other Iban longhouses in
the Betong and Saratok Districts, and, indeed, Renang's own son is
a Protestant minister. Formerly, death was an occasion on which families
brought out and displayed their beautifully woven heirloom cloths
(pua' kumbu') in the construction of the sapat. During the
nightlong vigil of rabat, a dirge singer, or tukang sabak, sat within
this cloth enclosure and sang the sabak, the lament for the dead in
which she described the journey of the dead person's soul as it
travels from the longhouse to the Afterworld. Mourners and family
members sat all around the enclosure, some, because of grief, as close
as possible, weeping, talking quietly among themselves, and listening to
the dirge singer's song. For many, the beauty of her words, and of
the cloth through which her voice passed, was a source of profound
comfort. As in other spheres of Iban life, art was once an integral part
of grieving.
While Heppell has chosen to couch his arguments in terms of
"sexual selection," history, to my mind, might well have
offered a more appropriate framework. The flowering of Iban art was,
after all, extremely short-lived in biological terms and depended upon a
number of historically-circumscribed processes. Moreover, the question
of why some communities foster creativity has long interested
historians. To return to my original question, from the perspective of
European history, a superb example of this interest may be found in
Jacob Burckhardt's The Civilization of the Renaissance in Italy,
particularly its brilliant Part One, "The State as a Work of
Art." Here Burckhardt persuasively argues that the Italian
city-states not only created conditions for a rebirth and outpouring of
artistic creativity, but that the political order itself was
objectified, made the object of reflection, both in the writings of its
political theorists (such as Machiavelli), but, equally, in its art and
architecture. The times, institutions, and art that Burckhardt deals
with are, of course, very different than those that concern Heppell. In
addition, as a European, Burckhardt is also interested in those elements
of Renaissance innovation and creativity that still persist. For this
latter perspective, we must wait, very probably, for some future Iban
historian (Clifford Sather).
References Cited
Bliege Bird, R., E. A Smith, and D. W. Bird 2001 The Hunting
Handicap: Costly Signaling in Human Foraging Strategies. Behavioral
Ecology and Sociobiology 50:9-19.
Coe, Kathryn 2003 The Ancestress Hypothesis: Visual Arts as
Adaptation. New Brunswick, NJ: Rutgers University Press.
Darwin, Charles 1909 The Origin of Species. New York: P. F.
Collier.
1989 The Descent of Man, and Selection in Relation to Sex. New
York: New York University Press.
Gavin, Traude 2003 Iban Ritual Textiles. Leiden: KITLV Press.
Hawkes, K. and R. Bliege Bird 2002 Showing Off, Handicap Signaling
and the Evolution of Men's Work. Evolutionary Anthropology
11:58-67.
King, Victor T. 1993 The Peoples of Borneo. Oxford: Blackwell.
Miller, Geoffrey 2000 The Mating Mind: How Sexual Choice Shaped the
Evolution of Human Nature. London: Heinemann.
Pringle, Robert 1970 Rajahs and Rebels: The Ibans of Sarawak under
Brooke Rule, 1841-1941. London: Macmillan.
Sandin, Benedict 1968 The Sea Dayaks of Borneo before White Rajah
Rule. East Lansing: Michigan State University Press.
Zahavi, Amotz, and Arishag Zahavi 1997 The Handicap Principle: A
Missing Piece of Darwin's Puzzle. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
(1) See Vinson Sutlive's Memorial to Datin Amar Margaret
Linggi, a woman who devoted her considerable energies to reviving an
interest in ikat weaving among Iban women in Sarawak.