Self-presentation, individual differences, and gendered evaluations of nonverbal greeting behaviors among close male friends.
Bowman, Jonathan M. ; Compton, Benjamin L.
When looking at the development of friendships, it is important to
look at the origins of such relationships. Evolutionary processes caused
something resembling "friendship" to have evolved over time in
animals, but nowhere is it so well-developed as in human beings
(Seyfarth & Cheney, 2012). However, such a well-developed social
construct leads scholars to find many ways to define a friendship. Many
separate studies have attempted to dissect factors that can become
reified as the defining characteristics of friendship. Regardless of
definition, most scholars agree about the importance of ritualized
greeting behaviors among most--if not all--human friendships. Indeed, a
greeting serves to begin or end a conversation between two more people
(Gander, 2007) and most every conversation is initiated by some sort of
greeting.
Specific types of ritualized greetings vary depending upon a wide
range of factors, but particular importance is attached to greetings
according to the level of friendship between greeting partners. Over
time, different ritualized greetings have become more sociably
acceptable, such that in modern times things like the handshake have
near-universal acceptance in Western society. Regardless of the
relationship, most greetings involve haptic interactions; the simple act
of touching is enough to communicate partner emotions (Thompson &
Hampton, 2011). Scholars have oft-studied the hug, for example, and have
found it to be increasingly widespread over the past 50 years, much more
so than in previous generations (Winters, 2011). The use of touch in
ritualized greeting behaviors are also dependent upon the age of the
participants, given that societal expectations for appropriate behavior
within relationships have changed (McDougall & Hymel, 2007).
The level, or depth, of friendship often predetermines the specific
action that will be performed during the greeting. It would be widely
assumed that the greater relational closeness between two or more
individuals, the greater the influence of the greeting on the experience
of that relationship (Weinstein, Laverghetta, Alexander, & Stewart,
2009). Indeed, Weinstein and colleagues found that greetings between
university professors and university students often correlated with
higher test scores. Scholars may argue that perhaps greetings create a
positive emotional experience in the relationship, leading to
increasingly positive perceptions and outcomes for the individuals
involved. Because friendships have various depths and levels that
characterize the relationship, the emotional intimacy in a relationship
often correlates with the amount of nonverbal intimacy expressed during
physical contact such as those found in ritualized greetings (Heslin
& Boss, 1980). Relatedly, higher levels of perceived affection (or
even romance) in a relationship often led to greater expectations for
expressions of nonverbal intimacy (Fuhrman, Flannagan, & Matamoros,
2009). This suggests that two friends who experience greater emotional
closeness would greet each other differently than friends with less
emotional intimacy. For the purpose of this study, we are particularly
interested in the two categories of Heslin's (1974) five major
categories of touch that are most associated with greetings among close
friends: the friendship-warmth function of touch (which involves the
negotiation of touch between two individuals) and the love-intimacy
function of touch (which involves nonsexual intimate touches, such as
hand-holding or prolonged hugging).
GENDER AND RITUALIZED GREETINGS
A major factor that determines how specific greetings are performed
within relationships includes the genders of the participants involved.
Gender is believed to be the most important individual factor
influencing nonverbal immediacy behavior (Astrom, 1994). There are
significant differences between men and women in both approach behaviors
as well as which areas of the body are appropriate for touching (Hewitt
& Feltham, 1982; Nguyen, Heslin, & Nguyen, 1975). For example,
women generally move more proximally close to the other person during a
greeting than would men in a similar situation. Independent of the
perceived closeness between two individuals, it was found that men were
also less comfortable with more intimate styles of greetings (i.e.,
kissing, etc.) than were women (Felmlee, Sweet, & Sinclair, 2012).
Indeed, this male discomfort with perceived intimacy plays itself out in
the very manner that men tend to express intimacy with one another;
same-gender friendships among women are more likely to participate in
conversation (emotional intimacy), while same-gender male friendships
that focus more on physical activity (Baumgarte & Nelson, 2009).
While many potential reasons abound, some scholars have suggested that a
strong motivation has to do with male sexuality: the fear of being
considered homosexual has played into reasons why women are typically
more comfortable with same-sex touching than are men (Floyd, 2000).
Same-Sex Friendships
When looking specifically at male same-sex relationships, it should
be noted that males also participate in same-sex non-romantic intimate
relationships, despite cultural beliefs to the contrary (Bowman, 2008).
Still, men are less likely to display physical intimacy with other males
than they are with their female friends (Derlega, Lewis, Harrison, &
Winstead, 1989). Regardless of the perceived level of intimacy, there
has been some direct evidence suggesting that there can be diminished
emotional intimacy between men based on issues such as homophobia or
perceptions of homophobia (Devlin & Cowan, 1985). Even among those
male friends who are emotionally intimate, intimate touch in nonverbal
greeting rituals is decidedly less so than among same-sex female
friends. Perhaps this can be attributed to greetings between males being
seen as interactions or enactments of masculine performances
(Migliaccio, 2009). These performances are based on both
individuals' expectations of the perceived friendship.
Although male same-sex heterosexual friendships include varying
levels of intimacy, there are some significant factors that may change
the expectations of friendship partners for both heterosexual and
homosexual friendship participants. Indeed, the more interactions and
close relationships that a heterosexual man has with homosexuals, the
less negatively valenced the attitudes they experience as a result of
that increased contact (Hodson, Harry, & Mitchell, 2009). The more
discomfort that a friendship partner perceives as a result of these
differences in sexual orientation, the more likely each participant is
involved in less-intimate levels of friendships with out-group
friendship partners. Despite sexual orientation, men expressed more
behavior inhibition in interaction with people whose sexual orientations
differed (Cook, Calcagno, Arrow, & Malle, 2012), which may
eventually influence behaviors like ritualized greetings or even the
desire to participate in such greetings. Overall, heterosexuals are more
likely to find intimate male-to-male contact inappropriate compared to
homosexuals viewing the same situation (Derlega, Catanzaro, & Lewis,
2001).
Perception in Third-Party Observers
The greeting between two people is oftentimes a private
performance, but bringing in a third party observer adds another
dimension and additional perceptions to manage. There can be three
different perspectives during a social interaction: the encoder of the
greeting behavior, the decoder (or receiver) of the greeting behavior,
and third-party observers of that greeting (Floyd & Erbert, 2003).
The majority of the times, all three roles have their own
interpretations of the meanings ascribed to each behavior, yet the
third-party perspective oftentimes has the least accurate interpretation
and/or recollection of the interaction. For example, the perception of
male-to-male reciprocal touching is deemed less normative than the
reciprocal touching of a cross-sex friendship, such that the simple act
of hugging between two men could suggest a sexual relationship in a
third-party observer where none exists (Derlega, Lewis, Harrison, &
Winstead, 1989).
A person's perceptions, including how a person perceives
something like a ritualized greeting, are often influenced by factors
associated with their social experiences. A study conducted in Belgium
found that males who have an extensive network of friends are more
likely to have a negative view of homosexuals, while females with an
extensive network of friends were more likely to have a positive view of
homosexuals (Hooghe, 2011). This difference in gender views was
explained by traditional roles of masculinity in peer pressure groups of
predominantly male-oriented networks. Although some scholars have argued
that men's friendships with other men potentially could create
greater social equality, the Belgium study argues that it is indeed
masculinity-driven male friendships that promote homophobic group-think
(Armengol-Carrera, 2009). Part of the way male-friends may contribute to
homophobic beliefs is by expressing that heterosexuality is not an act
or performative, but that it is a naturally occurring state (Kehler,
2007). Especially during both high school and college where identities
are not yet solidified, men may display their perceptions of
heterosexual masculinity, oftentimes performed through ritualized
practices that avoid any perception of femininity or homosexuality.
Information Processing in Third-Party Observers
The way one perceives another's ritualized greeting behavior
is often affected by their general ability to process relational
information (Knobloch & Solomon, 2005). Individuals who have
experienced less relational uncertainty in their own lives are more
likely to misinterpret others' actions due to a lack of knowledge
about relational cues. Also, if the observer is not a member of the
relationship they are observing, then their perception of the
relationship can be influenced by the perceived personality
characteristics (Chen, Bond, & Fung, 2006). If the two individuals
in a dyadic relationship are seen as having very upbeat, friendly
personalities, the observer is less likely to believe there is anything
unique in their greeting patterns. Indeed, specific background features
can play into the personal bias of a person's observations
(Cunningham & Melton, 2012).
Of course, a person's perceptions after observation of
others' behaviors may also be based on their ideas of traditional
gender roles, which are in some way influenced by their cultural ideas
of personal attributes and behaviors (Adana, Arslantas, Ergin, Bicer,
Kiransal, & Sahin, 2011). It should be noted that gendered
expressions often induce different expectations depending upon a range
of other factors (Scott-Carter, Corra, & Carter, 2009). At the core
of gender roles, however, the female gender role is seen as nurturing,
while the male gender role is often focused more on protection or
aggression (Gross, 2003). The idea of the male gender role is directly
linked with the masculine identity, related to such qualities as success
and self-reliance, which could actually have negative effects on men
seeking counsel, psychotherapy, or romantic relationships (Deering &
Gannon, 2005; McCarthy & Holliday, 2004; Morr Serewicz & Gale,
2008).
It isn't just the traditional gender roles of men and women,
but also the "gay stereotype" held by these men and women that
can alter perception and individual processing of relationship behaviors
like greetings. The "gay stereotype" is applied more often
when being used by a person who has little to no historical encounters
with the LGBTQ population throughout their daily lives (Sakalli, 2002).
These stereotypes are prevalent among heterosexual men; indeed, men are
more likely to use stereotypic descriptions of homosexual men than are
women. These stereotypes aren't just limited to homosexual men,
however. It was found that homosexual men are often compared to
heterosexual women, while homosexual women are seen as similar to
heterosexual men (Kite & Deaux, 1987). Interestingly, there is a
larger perceived stereotype difference between a heterosexual and
homosexual male than between a heterosexual and homosexual female. This
has been reified within our culture: straight actors playing homosexual
male characters on television are more likely to speak at a high pitch
when playing those characters (Cartei & Reby, 2012), suggesting yet
another example of the existence of the effeminate stereotype of male
homosexuals.
A discussion of sexual orientation and touch necessarily must
include the increasing sexual themes that men tend to place upon most
any interaction. With ritualized greetings varying in the amounts of
intimacy, it is important to look at the difference between men and
women in their sexual cognitions of said greetings. While some scholars
may disagree that there is not enough empirical evidence to conclude a
difference (Fisher, Moore, & Pittenger, 2012), most scholars agree
the traditional sex roles continue to dominate heterosexual
relationships (Sanchez, Fetterolf, & Rudman, 2012) and that men tend
to sexualize non-sexual interactions.
HYPOTHESES
Based on the above-mentioned previous research in the area of
self-presentation and individual differences' impacts upon
evaluation of friendship behavior, we offer the following hypotheses and
research questions for test in the current study.
Heterosexual Self-Presentation
First, individuals who report a higher degree of fear of being
perceived as homosexual are more likely to report intimate touching
behaviors as less normative on a variety of measures.
H1a-1c. In highly intimate forms of touch, heterosexual
self-presentation will be negatively correlated with ratings of a) how
typical the touch was, b) how often male friends engage in similar forms
of touch, and c) how appropriate the touch was. In nonintimate (routine)
forms of touch, no significant correlations will emerge.
Also, those individuals who report a higher degree of fear of being
perceived as homosexual are also more likely to rate these intimate
touching behaviors as indicators of closer relationships.
H2. In highly intimate forms of touch, heterosexual
self-presentation will be positively correlated with ratings of the
intimacy of touch. In nonintimate (routine) forms of touch, no
significant correlations will emerge.
Relational Closeness
Relatedly, it is expected that all participants are making
evaluations of the participants and the nature of their relationship. As
such, it is expected that there will be significant differences in the
evaluation of the relationship closeness based upon whether the touch is
highly intimate or nonintimate (routine).
H3. Third-party observers will be significantly more likely to
attribute relational closeness in highly intimate forms of touch than in
nonintimate (routine) forms of touch.
Because of this perceived relational closeness associated with the
intimacy of touch, people will assume that intimate touchers are close
enough that they don't care about what others think about their
relationship and are comfortable with the intimate form of touch
displayed.
H4a-4b. Third-party observers will be a) significantly more likely
to attribute actors' comfort with touch and b) significantly less
likely to attribute actors' fears regarding public
self-presentation when they are engaging in highly intimate forms of
touch than in nonintimate (routine) forms of touch.
Masculinity and Femininity
Also, gendered norms in our culture will likely have an impact on
the evaluation of male same-sex touching behavior, such that people who
have internalized a stronger sense of femininity are more likely to have
also developed the nurturance associated with traditional sex roles,
leading to greater belief in the acceptability of highly intimate touch.
H5a-5c: In highly intimate forms of touch, femininity will be
positively correlated with ratings of a) how typical the touch was, b)
how often male friends engage in similar forms of touch, and c) how
appropriate the touch was.
However, it is unclear whether masculinity will be more likely have
1) a negative correlation or 2) no significant correlation, given that
femininity is the clear traditional sex role associated with displays of
emotional intimacy.
RQ1. Is there a relationship between masculinity and ratings of a)
how typical the touch was, b) how often male friends engage in similar
forms of touch, and c) how appropriate the touch was?
Perception and Cognitive Load
When observing the greeting as a third party person, it is
interesting to look at the process of cognitive load to track the
concentration of thought and attention focus. Performing a cognitive
demand, such as an observation of something that is more personally
distressing, can alter a person's memory and therefore cause
attention to be affected (Sylva, Rieger, Linsenmeier, & Bailey,
2010). The focus of attention positively correlates with short-term
memory (Lewis-Peacock, Drysdale, Oberauer, & Postle, 2012). This
ability of short-term memory can be remembered after a brief delay, and
it has been found that things outside of the attention of focus are not
as likely to be recalled later. If participants are more likely to be
distracted by actors engaging in intimate touch because of the salience
of self-presentational goals, then those participants should be less
likely to notice details about the video that don't relate to those
self-presentational goals.
H6. In highly intimate forms of touch, a negative correlation
between heterosexual self-presentation will be positively associated
with participant's ability to remember a specific detail from the
video (which actor, if any, was carrying a specific artifact.) In
nonintimate (routine) forms of touch, no significant association will
emerge.
METHODS
Detailed Methodology
For this study, participants were students at a medium-sized
private institution in the southwestern United States. Participants
volunteered for a 20-minute survey that also included the viewing and
evaluation of a short video. Participants were randomly assigned to one
of the two combinations of touch type, with the constraint of
maintaining equal sample sizes.
Participants. The participants in the study were 151 undergraduate
students who participated in exchange for class credit. Students ranged
in age from 18 to 26, with an average age of just over 20 (m = 20.34).
Participants represented both sexes, although the campus has
significantly more women than men; as such, the sample was skewed
towards women, with just over two-thirds (73%) of participants being
female. While all students were encouraged to participate, because of
the research question only those students that self-identified as
heterosexual on the anonymous questionnaire were kept in the sample for
final data analysis, with 5 students being removed from the sample for a
final IV of 146.
Procedure. Participants volunteered for a study consisting of
measures of four separate sections, administered online through the
Qualtrics[C] survey software. Students were recruited in a wide range of
courses and disciplines across campus. Upon agreeing to participate,
students were sent a link to the Qualtrics[C] website, with the
constraint that students take the survey on a computer that can play
embedded YouTube[C] videos (i.e., not on iPhones or iPads.) Students
were then given a 7-day period to take the survey, and then indicated
their consent to participate in the research project.
Section one: Student profile. After consenting, participants
received a series of questions which attempted to ascertain basic
sociodemographic characteristics (e.g., age, sex, year, sexual
orientation, major, state of origin, etc.), as well as a variety of
filler questions about general attitudes towards various areas of life.
Following the methodology of Bowman (2008), few embedded questions
within the larger survey were the main items of interest for this
section, specifically three items drawn from Floyd's (2000) scale
of homophobia. These questions were developed to focus directly on the
fear of being seen as homosexual. The three items used in this survey,
rated on a likert-type scale from 1 (strongly disagree) to 9 (strongly
agree), include "I would be very upset if someone else thought I
was gay," "If a homosexual person began talking to me in
public, I would be concerned about what other people might think,"
and "I am careful not to do things that might make others think I
am homosexual." These three items were buried in a questionnaire of
26 other items which assessed students' perceptions of various
campus, personal, and social issues. Because these three items were
significantly correlated (r = .637), a composite measure was created by
summing the three items on the scale.
Section two: Video and video responses. At this point, participants
began the second portion of the study, watching a short clip of three
friends meeting up. These were filmed in a natural setting on campus,
using a "hidden-video" style format. Before watching the
video, participants read a scenario referencing several well-known
locations on campus, with scenario content framed as follows:
"Brandon, who lives off campus, was meeting up with Kyle and Ian,
who live in the sophomore residential halls, to go to Saturday brunch.
They went to eat at the cafeteria and discussed their weekend. (Brandon
is wearing the plaid shirt, Kyle is wearing the white shirt, and Ian is
wearing the black shirt.) "In the video, two of the friends
(Brandon and Ian) engaged in a ritualized greeting. In the nonintimate
video, the two friends said hello and each gave the other a friendly pat
on the back. In the intimate video, the two friends said hello and then
engaged in a criss-cross hug with full body contact. (It must be noted
that 48 separate videos were filmed using varying levels of intimacy,
and these two videos were selected based upon responses to a pilot test
conducted with a representative sample.) After watching the video,
students privately indicated their responses on a 9-point scale to seven
questions that attempted to ascertain one's belief in the
appropriateness of touch, one's concerns about how the touch may
affect one's image, and likely attributions one's friend would
make upon occurrence of the touch.
Section three: Recall responses. In an attempt to measure whether
participants were able to recall a specific detail of the short video
clip, a question about one thing that was visible in the video was
included. Students were asked to report on whether they noticed a
particular object held by an actor in the video (e.g., a folder, etc.)
and if so, who was holding it.
Section four: Gender identity. Finally, twenty questions drawn from
a modified version of Bern's Sex Role Inventory (Wheeless &
Dierks-Stewart, 198) were included to determine the gender identity of
participants. After responding to each of the questions, participants
submitted their names to receive course credit for their participation
in the extra credit opportunity.
RESULTS
Hypotheses 1a-1c
The first set of hypothesis was designed to test the impact of
one's desire to present oneself as heterosexual (heterosexual
self-presentation) upon the perceived typicality of intimate vs.
non-intimate touch, appropriateness of intimate vs. non-intimate touch,
and frequencies of expression associated with intimate vs. non-intimate
touch.
Hypothesis 1a. As predicted, a strong negative correlation emerged
between heterosexual self-presentation and the perceived typicality of
highly intimate touch, r (81) = -.30, p = .008. Also as predicted, no
significant correlation emerged between the two factors for non-intimate
touch, r (65) = -.095, p = .45.
Hypothesis lb. As predicted, a strong negative correlation emerged
between heterosexual self-presentation and the perception of how often
men engage in highly intimate touch, r (79) = -.30,p = .008. Also as
predicted, no significant correlation emerged between the two factors
for non-intimate touch, r (65) = -.064,p = .62.
Hypothesis 1c. As predicted, a strong negative correlation emerged
between heterosexual self-presentation and the perceived appropriateness
of highly intimate touch, r (81) = .32,p = .004. Also as predicted, no
significant correlation emerged between the two factors for non-intimate
touch, r (65) = -.121,p = .32.
Clearly, one's desire for heterosexual self-presentation is a
strong factor in the evaluation of the nonverbal behaviors of others,
with those individuals who have a higher desire to present themselves as
not homosexual being more likely to negatively evaluate the more
intimate forms of ritualized greetings among same-sex friends.
Hypothesis 2
The second hypothesis was designed to test the impact of one's
desire to present oneself as heterosexual (heterosexual
self-presentation) upon the ascribed closeness of relationships based
upon intimate vs. non-intimate touch.
As predicted, a positive correlation emerged between heterosexual
self-presentation and the perceived intimacy of highly intimate touch, r
(81) = .213,p = .057. Also as predicted, no significant correlation
emerged between the two factors for non-intimate touch, r (65) = .126, p
= .32.
In more physically intimate interactions, individuals with a
relatively higher desire to present themselves as heterosexual are also
more likely to evaluate that physical touch as intimate than are those
individuals who do not have this concern with heterosexual
self-presentation.
Hypothesis 3
The third hypothesis was designed to test whether, in general,
participants observing actors engaging in highly intimate greetings are
significantly more likely to assume that they are closer than are actors
engaging in nonintimate forms greetings.
As predicted, a one-way ANOVA demonstrated a significantly higher
rating of perceived relational closeness between actors engaged in
highly intimate touch (m = 5.91, sd = .99) than actors engaged in
nonintimate, routine forms of touch (m = 5.43, sd = 1.14), F = 7.46, p =
.007.
Participants clearly believe that people who engage in more
intimate greeting behaviors have a closer relationship.
Hypotheses 4a-4b
The fourth hypotheses was designed to test the impact of these
evaluations of relational closeness: whether, in general, participants
observing actors engaging in highly intimate greetings are significantly
more likely to assume that they a) don't care about what others
think about their relationship and b) are comfortable with the intimate
form of touch displayed.
Hypotheses 4a-4b. As predicted, a one-way ANOVA demonstrated a
significantly higher rating of perceived comfort with the greeting
between actors engaged in highly intimate touch (m = 5.26, SD = 1.13)
than actors engaged in nonintimate, routine forms of touch (m = 4.69, SD
= 1.13), F = 9.10,p = .003. Also confirming our predictions, a one-way
ANOVA demonstrated a significantly lower rating of perceived fears of
making a bad impression on other people for actors engaged in highly
intimate touch (m = 2.88, SD = 1.46) than for actors engaged in
nonintimate, routine forms of touch (m = 3.58, SD = 1.24), F = 9.70,p =
.002.
Third party observers seem to assume that people in close
friendships don't worry as much about the impact of their nonverbal
behaviors because that relationship is already close; people in
less-close friendships may have greater issues of self-presentation,
explaining the differences in comfort and fear of a bad impression.
Hypotheses 5a-5c and Research Question 1
The fifth set of hypothesis and the research question were designed
to test the impact of femininity and masculinity upon one's
evaluation of the perceived typicality, the appropriateness, and how
often men engage in with highly intimate touch.
Hypothesis 5a. As predicted, a strong positive correlation emerged
between femininity and the perceived typicality of highly intimate
touch, r(81) = .27,p = .017.
Hypothesis 5b. As predicted, a strong positive correlation emerged
between femininity and the perception of how often men engage in highly
intimate touch, r(79) = .23, p = .043.
Hypothesis 5c. As predicted, a strong positive correlation emerged
between femininity and the perceived appropriateness of highly intimate
touch, r(81) = .24,p = .035.
Research Question 1. No correlations emerged between masculinity
and the perceived typicality (r(81) = .10, p = .391), frequency (r(79) =
.10, p = .386), or appropriateness of highly intimate touch (r (81) =
.12, p= .270).
Clearly, femininity influences impressions of the likelihood of
engaging in a highly-intimate nonverbal greeting ritual among same-sex
friends.
Hypothesis 6. The sixth hypothesis was designed to test whether, in
general, participants with higher levels of heterosexual
self-presentation are more likely to be distracted by actors engaging in
highly intimate touch, while there is no difference in distraction when
touch is nonintimate.
As predicted, a negative association emerged between heterosexual
self-presentation and the ability to remember details of a video that
included a greeting with highly intimate touch, r (81) = -.312,p = .005.
Also as predicted, no significant association emerged between the two
factors for non-intimate touch, r(65) = -.049, p = .70.
As expected - but no less surprising--performing an additional
cognitive demand (like observing a greeting that includes a salient,
personally-distressing highly intimate touch) can distract individuals
because of their high need for heterosexual self-presentation.
DISCUSSION
The present study looked at the influence of personality
characteristics and self-presentational goals on third-party perceptions
of ritualized nonverbal greetings in natural settings. Additionally, the
study looked at cognitive processing and its effect upon information
retrieval and cognitive load, as impacted by participants' own
self-presentational goals as third-party observers.
Self-Presentation
The impact of self-presentation on perceptions of various levels of
intimate physical contact was based on the notion that males have less
intimate interactions due to homophobia (Devlin & Cowan, 1985). With
this, it was found that men and women who cared deeply about
successfully presenting themselves as heterosexual were more likely to
believe that male-to-male intimate greetings were less typical, less
frequent, and less appropriate. On the other hand, those who had a
lesser desire for heterosexual self-presentation did not find the more
intimate same-sex greetings to be irregular. Clearly, during the process
of self-presentation, individuals portraying themselves as heterosexual
seem more comfortable with others who also engage in similarly
heteronormative behaviors that they themselves would be likely to show.
Additionally, it was found that those men and women who had high
desire for a heterosexual self-presentation not only saw intimate
greetings as irregular, but also assumed a closer relationship between
the two males observed in the videos. Not only would they believe there
to be greater relational closeness in the relationship, but also they
seemed willing to ascribe characteristics typically associated with
intimacy (or even romance) to the interaction partners. Thus, if a pair
of males were to shake hands, it would be first be perceived that they
were friends. Alternately, if a pair of males were to hug in a
particularly intimate manner, it would be perceived that they may have a
romantic relationship.
Interestingly (and significantly) this perception changes for those
individuals who do not purport to show a desire to be perceived
themselves as heterosexual. Regardless of self presentation,
participants were more likely to assume that both male participants did
not care about what others thought of their relationship; males in the
video were perceived to be relatively comfortable with their actions
during the greeting. Also, partners expressing more-intimate greetings
were not perceived to fear making a bad impression on other people by
third-party observers. This clearly demonstrates that third-party
observers may assume that people involved in close relationships do not
worry about their self-presentational needs in ritualized greeting
behaviors because the relationship is already close. Perhaps people in
less-close relationships have more concern with their self-presentation
during greetings because those relationships are not already firmly
established and comfortable.
While sex was not seen as a significant predictor of third-party
observations, the third individual difference tested was the impact of
individual gender associations on the perceptions. Overall, third-party
observers' femininity had a stronger connection between seeing
highly intimate touching and typicality, frequency, and appropriateness.
Masculinity, however, was found to have no correlation with perceived
typicality, frequency, or appropriateness ascribed to these more
intimate nonverbal greeting behaviors.
The final area of inquiry for the current study looked at the
cognitive load associated with both self-presentation and third-party
perceptions. People with higher concerns for heterosexual
self-presentation were less likely to remember specific details
surrounding the intimate greetings. However, when it came to
non-intimate greetings, respondents were able to remember the details.
This suggests that when people observe something irregular and
personally salient, such as an intimate same-sex greeting, that the
third-party observer will only think of that irregular behavior as
opposed to other possible observations.
Future Directions
Potential extensions of this study are numerous; such findings are
heuristically provocative. As the study focused on men's and
women's evaluations of same-sex male nonverbal greeting behaviors,
a natural follow-up would include the general appropriateness associated
with nonverbal intimacy between female friends. While research has
typically designated females as more accepting of intimate physical
behaviors (Floyd, 2000), a research question would help clarify the
cultural norms that then may or may not be influenced by
self-presentation. Indeed, a comprehensive look at gender roles and
self-presentation may prove useful.
Additionally, our statistically significant hypotheses associated
with cognitive load and memory builds upon a traditional understanding
of ritualized greeting behaviors. With people seeing more mediated
and/or public acts of intimacy among both heterosexual couples and
homosexual couples, self-concept and its impact upon third-party
observations may prove fruitful in understanding peoples' memories
and perceptions associated with nonverbal displays. Increased scrutiny
may clarify how intimacy and affection displays affect our short-term
memory and cognitive processing for both participants and observers.
Indeed, even perceptions of situational appropriateness may alter the
likelihood of recollection and perception.
Finally, this study was able to demonstrate that a wide variety of
differences and processes can influence third-party observations.
Increasing the representative nature of the sampling
process--particularly with regard to the inclusion of a large sample of
non-heterosexual or questioning participants--may influence the
understandings of in-group and out-group cognitive experiences of
intimacy within close relationships of many types. If our cognitive load
research is to generate a much broader discussion, there must be greater
attention to the wide contexts in which observation and experience may
occur. Limitations notwithstanding, however, this study sheds light on
the influence of gender orientation, self-presentation, and perceptions
of nonverbal norms upon both cognitive processing of third-party
observations and also one's subsequent attitude towards relational
behaviors; future research in this area will likely prove fruitful and
may dramatically impact our understanding and experience of the
sense-making process associated with ritualized greetings.
References
Adana, F., Arslantas, H., Ergin, F., Bicer, N., Kiransal, N., &
Sahin, S. (2011). Views of male university students about social gender
roles: An example from east of Turkey. Journal of Family Violence,
26(7), 519-526. doi: 10.1007/s10896-011-9385-1
Armengol-Carrera, J.M. (2009). Of friendship: Revisiting
friendships between men in American literature. Journal of Men's
Studies, 17(3), 193-209. doi: 10.3149/jms.1703.193
Astrom, J. (1994). Introductory greeting behavior: A laboratory
investigation of approaching and closing salutation. Perceptual &
Motor Skills, 79(2), 863. doi: 10.2466/pms. 1994.79.2.863
Baumgarte, R., & Nelson, D. (2009). Preference for same- versus
cross-sex friendships. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 39(A),
901-917. doi: 10.1111/j.1559-1816.2009.00465.x
Bowman, J.M. (2008). Gender role orientation and relational
closeness: Self-disclosive behavior in same-sex male friendships.
Journal of Men's Studies, 76(3), 316-330. doi: 10.3149/jms.
1603.316
Cartei, V., & Reby, D. (2012). Acting gay: Male actors shift
the frequency components of their voices towards female values when
playing homosexual characters. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 26(1),
79-93. doi: 10.1007/s10919-011-0123-4
Chen, S., Bond, M., & Fung, I. (2006). Perceiving communion in
the dyadic relationship of others. Asian Journal of Social Psychology,
9(2), 103-111. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-839X.2006.00187. x
Cook, J.E., Calcagno, J.E., Arrow, H., & Malle, B.F. (2012).
Friendship trumps ethnicity (but not sexual orientation): Comfort and
discomfort in inter-group interactions. British Journal of Social
Psychology, 51(2), 273-289. doi: 10.1111/j.2044-8309.2011.02051.x
Cunningham, G.B., & Melton, N. (2012). Prejudice against
lesbian, gay, and bisexual coaches: The influence of race, religious
fundamentalism, modem sexism, and contact with sexual minorities.
Sociology of Sport Journal, 29(3), 283-305.
Deering, C., & Gannon, E.J. (2005). Gender and psychotherapy
with traditional men. American Journal of Psychotherapy, 59(4), 351-360.
Derlega, V.J., Catanzaro, D., & Lewis, R.J. (2001). Perceptions
about tactile intimacy in same-sex and opposite-sex pairs based on
research participants' sexual orientation. Psychology of Men &
Masculinity, 2(2), 124-132. doi: 10.1037/1524-9220.2.2.124
Derlega, V.J., Lewis, R.J., Harrison, S., & Winstead, B.A.
(1989). Gender differences in the initiation and attribution of tactile
intimacy. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 13(2), 83-96. doi:
10.1007/BF00990792
Devlin, P.K., & Cowan, G.A. (1985). Homophobia, perceived
fathering, and male intimate relationships. Journal of Personality
Assessment, 49(5), 467.
Felmlee, D., Sweet, E., & Sinclair, H.H. (2012). Gender rules:
Same- and cross-gender friendships norms. Sex Roles, 66(7/8), 518-529.
doi: 10.1007/s11199-011-0109-z
Fisher, T.D., Moore, Z.T., & Pittenger, M. (2012). Sex on the
brain?: An examination of frequency of sexual cognitions as a function
of gender, erotophilia, and social desirability. Journal of Sex
Research, 49(1), 69-11. doi: 10.1080/00224499.2011.565429
Floyd, K. (2000). Affectionate same-sex touch: The influence of
homophobia on observers' perceptions. Journal of Social Psychology,
140(6), 774-788. doi: 10.1080/00224540009600516
Floyd, K., & Erbert, L.A. (2003). Relational message
interpretations of nonverbal matching behavior: An application of the
social meaning model. Journal of Social Psychology, 143(5), 581-597.
doi: 10.1080/00224540309598465
Fuhrman, R.W., Flannagan, D., & Matamoros, M. (2009). Behavior
expectations in cross-sex friendships, same-sex friendships, and
romantic relationships. Personal Relationships, 16(4), 575-596.
doi:10.1111/j. 1475-6811.2009.01240.x
Gander, H. (2007). On attention: From a phenomenological analysis
towards an ethical understanding of social attention. Research in
Phenomenology, 37(3), 287-302. doi: 10.1163/ 156916407X227857
Gross, R.M. (2003). What went wrong? Feminism and freedom from the
prison of gender roles. Cross Currents, 53(1), 8.
Heslin, R. (1974). Steps toward a taxomony of touching. Paper
presented to the annual meeting of the Midwestern Psychological
Association, Chicago, IL.
Heslin, R., & Boss, D. (1980). Nonverbal intimacy in airport
arrival and departure. Personality & Social Psychology Bulletin,
6(2), 248-252. doi: 10.1177/014616728062010
Hewitt, J., & Feltham, D. (1982). Differential reaction to
touch by men and women. Perceptual & Motor Skills, 55(3), 1291-1294.
doi: 10.2466/pms.1982.55.3f.1291
Hodson, G., Harry, H., & Mitchell, A. (2009). Independent
benefits of contact and friendship on attitudes toward homosexuals among
authoritarians and highly identified heterosexuals. European Journal of
Social Psychology, 39(4), 509-525. doi: 10.1002/ejsp.558
Hooghe, M. (2011). The impact of gendered friendship patterns on
the prevalence of homophobia among Belgian late adolescents. Archives of
Sexual Behavior, 40(3), 543-550. doi: 10.1007/s10508-010-9635-y
Kehler, M.D. (2007). Hallway fears and high school friendships: The
complications of young men (re)negotiating heterosexualized identities.
Discourse: Studies in the Cultural Politics of Education, 28(2),
259-277. doi: 10.1080/01596300701289375
Kite, M.E., & Deaux, K. (1987). Gender belief systems:
Homosexuality and the implicit inversion theory. Psychology of Women
Quarterly, 11(1), 83. doi: 10.1111/j.1471-6402.1987. tb00776.x
Knobloch, L.K., & Solomon, D. (2005). Relational uncertainty
and relational information processing: Questions without answers?
Communication Research, 32(3), 349-388. doi: 10.1177/0093650205275334
Lewis-Peacock, J.A., Drysdale, A.T., Oberauer, K., & Postle,
B.R. (2012). Neural evidence for a distinction between short-term memory
and the focus of attention. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 24(1),
61-79. doi: 10.1162/jocn_a_00140
McCarthy, J., & Holliday, E.L. (2004). Help-seeking and
counseling within a traditional male gender role: An examination from a
multicultural perspective. Journal of Counseling & Development,
82(1), 25-30. doi: 10.1002/j.11556-6678.2004.tb00282.x
McDougall, P., & Hymel, S. (2007). Same-gender versus
cross-gender friendship conceptions. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 53(3),
347-380. doi: 10.1353/mpq.2007.0018
Migliaccio, T. (2009). Men's friendships: Performances of
masculinity. Journal of Men's Studies, 17(3), 226-241.
doi:10.3149/jms.1703.226
Morr Serewicz, M., & Gale, E. (2008). First-date scripts:
Gender roles, context, and relationship. Sex Roles, 53(3/4), 149-164.
doi: 10.1007/s11199-007-9283-4
Nguyen, T., Heslin, R., & Nguyen, M.L. (1975). The meanings of
touch: Sex differences. Journal of Communication, 25(3), 92-103. doi:
10.1111/j.1460-2466.1975.tb00610.x
Sakalli, N. (2002). Pictures of male homosexuals in the heads of
Turkish college students: The effects of sex difference and social
contact on stereotyping. Journal of Homosexuality, 43(2), 111. doi:
10.1300/J082v43n02_07
Sanchez, D.T., Fetterolf, J.C., & Rudman, L.A. (2012).
Eroticizing inequality in the United States: The consequences and
determinants of traditional gender role adherence in intimate
relationships. Journal of Sex Research, 49(2/3), 168-183. doi:
10.1080/00224499.2011.653699
Scott-Carter, J.J., Corra, M., & Carter, S.K. (2009). The
interaction of race and gender: Changing gender-role attitudes,
1974-2006. Social Science Quarterly (Blackwell Publishing Limited),
90(1), 196-211. doi: 10.1111/j.1540-6237.2009.00611.x
Seyfarth, R.M., & Cheney, D.L. (2012). The evolutionary origins
of friendship. Annual Review of Psychology, 63(1), 153-177. doi:
10.1146/annurev-psych-120710-100337
Sylva, D., Rieger, G., Linsenmeier, J.W., & Bailey, J. (2010).
Concealment of sexual orientation. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 39(1),
141-152. doi: 10.1007/s10508-008-9466-2
Thompson, E.H., & Hampton, J.A. (2011). The effect of
relationship status on communicating emotions through touch. Cognition
& Emotion, 25(2), 295-306. doi: 10.1080/02699931. 2010.492957
Weinstein, L., Laverghetta, A., Alexander, R., & Stewart, M.
(2009). Teacher greetings increase college students' test scores.
College Student Journal, 43(2), 452-453.
Wheeless, V.E., & Dierks-Stewart, K. (1981). The psychometric
properties of the Bern sex-role inventory: Questions concerning
reliability and validity. Communication Quarterly, 28, 173-186.
Winters, W.L. (2011, June). The metaphoric hug: A secret medicinal.
Methodist DeBakey Cardiovascular Journal, 2.
JONATHAN M. BOWMAN * AND BENJAMIN L. COMPTON **
* University of San Diego.
** University of Kansas.
A version of this article was presented at the 2013 Annual
Conference of the National Communication Association in Washington, DC.
Correspondence regarding this article should be addressed to
Jonathan M. Bowman, University of San Diego Department of Communication
Studies, 5998 Alcala Park, San Diego, CA 92110. Email: bowman@
sandiego.edu
DOI: 10.3149/jms.2203.207