Examining masculine gender-role conflict and stress in relation to religious orientation and spiritual well-being in Australian men.
Jurkovic, Dubravko ; Walker, Gordon A.
Even though an overwhelming majority of Australians find churches
unappealing and irrelevant (new statistics confirm declining attendance
at traditional churches), according to the latest figures up to 75% of
the Australian population claim to be religious in some way (Australian
Bureau of Statistic, 2001). Despite this fact, most psychotherapists
receive no formal training in the psychology of religion, and many
studies report that most psychotherapists either avoid the theme of
religion or handle it with insufficient skills, feeling uncomfortable
and ill-equipped to deal with the spiritual concerns of clients
(Schreurs, 2002). In a religiously diverse society like Australia,
mental health professionals are likely to encounter many persons
(apparently three out of four) of different religious and spiritual
beliefs. Furthermore, for reasons of intellectual integrity, psychology
cannot afford to ignore or dismiss millennia of religious and
philosophical thought about the very essence of human nature and
existence (Turbott, 1996).
According to Tacey (2000), rationality without spirituality leads
to "dryness" and a lack of meaning, a scenario reported by
many depressed and suicidal Australian men. Suicide is the greatest
cause of unnatural death for men in Australia (Biddulph, 1995). Recent
surveys across 14 industrialised nations have found Australia to be the
leader in suicide rates among 15- to 24-year-olds and that the rate is
significantly higher for males in all age groups than for women (Mann,
1997). Since the increase in suicides over the past 30 years (Australian
Bureau of Statistics, 2000) happened at the same time as a marked
upheaval in men's gender roles, some recent literature suggests
that the nature of the male role in society itself may be the cause of
the tendency for males toward suicide more often than females (Hassan,
1995; Kidd, 2002). This line of reasoning (Hassan, 1995; Kidd, 2002;
Tacey, 2000) has great appeal and encourages closer examination of the
relationship between gender-role restrictions and pressures observable
in the socialization process of men on one hand and their religious
orientation and spirituality on the other.
Since the topics of religiousness and spirituality are very broad
and are discussed in detail elsewhere (see Argyle, 2000; Argyle &
Beit-Hallahmi 1975; Loewenthal, 2000; Schreurs, 2002; Spilka, Hood,
& Gorsuch, 1985; Tacey, 2000; Vaughan, 1991; Wulff, 1997), only
certain aspects will be examined in this study--namely the measurement
of religiousness and spirituality.
The dominant paradigm for measuring religion has been the
distinction between extrinsic (E) versus intrinsic (I) religious
orientation (Allport & Ross, 1967). Extrinsic religiousness is the
religion of comfort and social convention, a self-serving instrumental
approach shaped to suit oneself, while intrinsic religiousness is
religion as a meaning-endowing framework in terms of which all life is
understood (Allport & Ross, 1967). The most widely used measure of
spirituality is The Spiritual Well-Being Scale (SWBS) (Ellison, 1983),
which purports to assess two distinct dimensions. The vertical dimension
(or religious well-being) refers to a sense of well-being in relation to
God. The horizontal dimension (or existential well-being) refers to a
sense of life purpose and life satisfaction (Slater, Hall, &
Edwards, 2001).
One of the most consistent findings in the sociology of religion is
that women tend to be more religious than men (Argyle &
Beit-Hallahmi, 1975; deVaus & McAllister, 1987; Francis &
Wilcox, 1996; Paloma & Gallup, 1991). In comparison to women, men
generally avoid religious activities and neglect attending to their
spiritual lives (Batson & Ventis, 1982). The explanations for that
finding vary from psychological theories (Argyle & Beit-Hallahmi,
1975; Suziedelis & Potvin, 1981) to the structural location of women
in society (de Vaus & McAllister, 1987). Thompson (1991) challenged
existing explanations, arguing that gender differences in religiousness
are due to the different gender-role socialization processes that men
and women experience. A body of research that suggests that male clergy
tend to reflect a feminine personality profile (Ekhardt & Goldsmith
1984; Francis, 1991; Goldsmith & Ekhardt, 1984) and the findings of
Francis and Wilcox (1996) that both men and women who possess a feminine
rather than a masculine outlook tend to be more religious add further
support to Thompson's thesis. Following the work of Erikson (1958,
1969), who argued that one needs the feminine mode to be able to have a
religious experience, Zock (1997) concluded that the feminine mode is
the basis of internalised religious experience and expression.
According to Mahalik and Lagan (2001), a more heuristic framework
for explaining men's religiousness and spiritual well-being should
incorporate a comprehensive understanding of the restrictive nature of
the masculine socialization process. Pleck (1981, 1985) argued that,
when men internalise stereotyped societal norms around male ideals (that
are often contradictory, inconsistent, and unattainable), a gender-role
strain arises. One type of gender-role strain is gender-role conflict
(O'Neil, 1981; O'Neil, Good, & Holmes, 1995). More
specifically, this psychological state in men occurs when "rigid,
sexist, or restrictive gender roles learned during socialization result
in personal restrictions, devaluation, or violation of others or
self" (O'Neil, 1990, p. 25). O'Neil theorized that
traditional male role socialization produces messages that are
contradictory and unrealistic and lead to a fear of femininity
(O'Neil, 1981, 1982). In turn, men engage in gender-role conflict
patterns (that restrict their roles and behavior to stereotypically
masculine ones) because of a fear of being or appearing feminine.
Masculine gender-role stress (MGRS) refers to the cognitive
appraisal of a specific situation as being stressful for men (Eisler
& Skidmore, 1987). MGRS is a theoretical construct that describes
the stress experienced by men when they feel they are not meeting
culturally sanctioned masculine gender-role behavior or when the
situation forces men to act in stereotypically feminine ways (Eisler,
1995). The pressure men tend to place on themselves reflects stressful
experiences around (a) physical inadequacy, (b) emotional
inexpressiveness, (c) subordination to women, (d) intellectual
inferiority, and (e) performance failure. In addition, under conditions
of accelerated modernization and women's emancipation, one might
argue that there is a strong element of negativity in contemporary
Australian culture about men, which contributes to masculine gender
stress (Hassan, 1995).
Accumulated evidence supports the view that the way men are
traditionally socialized to be masculine can have deleterious mental and
physical health consequences (Fragoso & Kashubeck, 2000). For
example, gender-role conflict has been shown to be related to men's
overall psychological distress (Good et al., 1995); lower self-esteem
and higher anxiety (Sharpe & Heppner, 1991); higher levels of anger
and substance use (Blazina, & Watkins, 1996); a higher level of
depression (Cournoyer & Mahalik, 1995); and sexual aggression
against women (Rando, Rogers, & Brittan-Powel, 1998). Research on
masculine gender-role stress shows similar results in that gender-role
stress has been reported to be associated with problematic behaviors,
negative emotions, anger, and hostility as well as elevated blood
pressure and high-risk health habits (Eisler, Skidmore, & Ward,
1988; Watkins, Eisler, Carpenter, Schechtman, & Fisher, 1991).
Social constructionists also relate gender-role conflict and stress to a
higher likelihood of men committing suicide than women (Kid, 2002;
Tacey, 2000). The line of reasoning in this approach is that men's
"gender identity" is subsumed by an overall "dominant
hegemonic masculinity" and that it results in men's being
unable to seek help (Connell, 1996). More specifically, men do not
usually talk about their problems to anyone out of fear or embarrassment
of being seen as weak (i.e., it would not be considered manly). In
addition, the relative loss of status experienced by men produces a
social, psychological, and economic climate that is conducive to an
increase in men's suicide (Hassan, 1995).
In contrast, research reports a strong relationship between
religiousness, spiritual well-being variables, and positive mental and
physical health. A large proportion of the published empirical data
suggests that religious commitment may play a beneficial role in
preventing mental and physical illness (Koenig, 1998). Individuals with
strong religious faith report higher levels of life satisfaction,
greater personal happiness, and fewer negative psychosocial consequences
of traumatic life events (Ellison, 1991). Studies also indicate that a
variety of religious practices (e.g., church attendance, prayer) tends
to be inversely associated with psychoactive substance use (Gorsuch,
1995). Several studies have shown that religion has the capacity to
moderate the effects of stress in coping with significant negative life
events (Maton, 1989; Pargament & Brant, 1998). Research also
indicates that persons who are more religious experience less anxiety
(Thorson, 1998) and less depression (Kennedy, 1998; Koenig, 1998). Also,
religious commitment may afford protection against one of the most
severe outcomes of depression: suicide (Gartner, Larson, & Allen,
1991).
Overall, following the evidence of the opposite effects of gender
conflict/stress and religiousness/spirituality on men's health, it
could be argued that masculine gender-role conflict and stress factors
would relate to less spiritual well-being. Using a U.S. sample, Mahalik
and Lagan (2001) examined masculine gender-role conflict and stress in
relation to religiousness and spiritual well-being. Because men are
socialized to avoid all things feminine (Levant & Pollack, 1998),
and because the feminine mode is the basis of internalised religious
experience and expression (Zock, 1997), Mahalik and Lagan (2001) argued
that "men do not internalise religiousness or experience spiritual
well-being to the extent that they constrain themselves from activities
that may be considered feminine" (p. 24). Specifically, men who
experience greater gender-role conflict and stress are likely to
restrict themselves in terms of developing intrinsically oriented
religious behavior, values, and attitudes because of fear of being or
appearing feminine as well as the stress involved in participating in
"feminine" activities. The results of the study revealed a
significant relationship between the measures assessing gender-role
conflict and stress and those assessing religiousness and spiritual
well-being (Mahalik & Lagan). Given the similarity between the U.S.
and Australian cultures, that insight could be also relevant in
Australia. However, in the United States, religion is much more a part
of the dominant culture than it is in Australia. By taking that
difference into account, Mahalik and Lagan's hypothesis may still
have relevance in explaining some societal factors that work against the
psychological well-being of men in Australia.
Following the lead established by Mahalik and Lagan (2001), the
present study was undertaken to investigate the relationship between
masculine gender-role conflict factors and stress, on one hand, and
religious orientation and spiritual well-being, on the other, using a
sample of Australian men. Participants were asked to identify their
level of religiousness (ranging from "not at all" to
"extremely religious") as well as to fill out four
questionnaires: the Gender Role Conflict Scale (GRSC); the Masculine
Gender Role Stress Scale (MGRS); the Intrinsic-Extrinsic Religious
Orientation Scale--I/E-ROS Revised; and the Spiritual Well-Being Scale
(SWBS). Three hypotheses were examined.
First, it was hypothesised that nonreligious men would exhibit
higher levels of masculine gender-role conflict and stress than
religious men. Second, it was predicted that masculine gender role
conflict and stress factors would be related to less intrinsic
religiousness for men. Finally, it was also hypothesised that masculine
gender-role conflict and stress factors would be related to less
spiritual well-being.
METHOD
PARTICIPANTS
The initial sample consisted of 147 volunteer males whose ages
ranged from 18 to 83 years (M = 38.47, SD = 14.29). Four participants
did not give an answer to more than 30% of the questions and were
removed from the study. For data analysis, participants were separated
into two groups: "religious" (committed to religion) and
"nonreligious" (not committed to religion), based on their
self-reported level of religiousness.
The religious group (with self-reported levels of religiousness:
"somewhat," "very," and "extremely
religious") consisted of 73 participants, who averaged 38.90 years
of age (SD = 13.28). They reported their religion as Protestant (53.4%),
Catholic (23.3%), nondenominational (12.3%), and Orthodox (1.4%). Seven
participants (9.6%) did not specify any religion.
The seventy (70) participants who were assigned to the nonreligious
group (with self-reported level of religiousness either "not at
all" or "not very religious") averaged 36.73 years of age
(SD = 15.29).
MATERIALS
A personal particulars sheet and four self-report questionnaires
were used. The personal particulars sheet was used to obtain general
information about participants such as religion, level of religiousness,
age, marital status, and country of birth. Specifically, level of
religiousness was assessed on a five-point scale ("not at
all," "not very," "somewhat," "very."
and "extremely").
The Gender Role Conflict Scale (GRSC) (O'Neil, Helms, Gable,
David, & Wrightsman, 1986) was used to measure levels and patterns
of gender-role conflict in men. The GRCS provides an assessment of four
factors: (a) success, power, and competition (SPC); (b) restrictive
emotionality (RE); (c) restrictive affectionate behavior between men
(RABBM); and (d) conflict between work and family relations (CBWF). It
consists of 37 items that asked about participants' reactions in
potential gender-role conflicts. Each item was scored on a 1-to-6 Likert
scale ("strongly disagree" = 1 to "strongly agree" =
6), and scores were obtained by summing across items for each of four
factors. Higher scores indicate greater gender-role conflict and fear of
femininity.
O'Neil et al. (1986) reported internal consistency for the
four factors (alphas range from .75 to .85) and adequate test-retest
reliabilities (ranging from .72 to .86). Construct validity is supported
by findings of positive relations with depression (Good & Mintz,
1990), traditional male role norms, and psychological distress (Good et
al., 1995).
The Masculine Gender Role Stress Scale (MGRSS) (Eisler &
Skidmore, 1987) consists of 40 items designed to assess men's
experience of stress associated with cognitive, behavioral, and
environmental events related to the male gender role. Eisler and
Skidmore found five factors reflecting experiences typically perceived
as more stressful by men than by women: (a) being in situations that
require emotional expressiveness, (b) feeling intellectually inferior,
(c) feeling physically inadequate, (d) experiencing performance failure,
and e) being subordinate to women. The respondents answer each item
using a six-point Likert type scale ranging from "not
stressful" (0) to "extremely stressful" (5), in terms of
its impact on them (as if they were in the situation). Thus, higher
scores indicate greater masculine role stress.
The coefficient alpha internal consistency of the MGRS is .90,
while alpha coefficients range from .64 to .83 for the MGRS factors
(Eisler & Skidmore, 1987). Validity is supported by findings of
positive correlations between MGRS scores and measures of Type-A
behavior and hostility (Watkins et al., 1991) as well as by findings
from studies that report that men score significantly higher on the MGRS
and the five MGRS factors than do women (Eisler & Skidmore). Thus,
the MGRS scale shows that some stresses in men are gender specific, tap
into negative aspects of commitment to traditional masculine ideology,
and are related to psychological distress.
To measure religious orientation, participants completed the
revised Age Universal Intrinsic-Extrinsic Religious Orientation Scale
(I/E ROS) (Gorsuch & Venable, 1983; Maltby & Lewis, 1996). The
I/E ROS can be used among both religious and nonreligious samples and
among all age groups (Maltby & Lewis, 1996). The scale contains two
separate subscales designed to measure two distinct religious
orientations: an intrinsic (I) and an extrinsic (E) orientation.
Nineteen of the 20 items are scored on the same three-point Likert
continuum scale, with 1 indicating disagreement, 2 indicating that the
respondent is not sure, and 3 indicating agreement. Item six (part of
the I subscale), which measures the frequency of church attendance, was
dropped from the analysis because it did not seem appropriate to treat
intrinsic faith and participation in religious services together. It was
found in previous studies (e.g., Genia, 1993) that frequency of worship
attendance as part of the I scale presents theoretical as well as
methodological problems. Gorsuch (1984) argued that the relationship
between intrinsic commitment and frequency of worship attendance should
be established empirically rather than assumed a priori. The remaining
eight intrinsic and 11 extrinsic items were scored separately.
Gorsuch and Venable (1983) reported internal consistency
reliability of .66 for the E subscale and .73 for the I subscale. Hood
(1970) reported the test-retest reliability of .93 for the total scale.
Construct validity is supported by finding that intrinsicness correlated
positively with measures of religious commitment (.76), while
extrinsicness remained uncorrelated (.03) (Hood, 1970). Also, it was
found that intrinsicness correlates positively with a sense of purpose
in life (Crandall & Rasmussen, 1975), internal locus of control (Kahoe, 1974), and empathy (Watson, Hood, Morris, & Hall, 1984).
Negative correlations were found between intrinsicness and depression
(Genia & Shaw, 1991), trait anxiety (Baker & Gorsuch, 1982),
prejudice, and dogmatism (Donahue, 1985). Positive correlation was found
between high intrinsic orientation scores on the I/E-ROS and high
spiritual well-being scores on the Spiritual Well-Being Scale (Bassett
et al., 1991; Ellison, 1983). It was also found that religious people
with intrinsically oriented faith are more psychologically adjusted than
those who are extrinsically oriented toward religion (Bergin, 1991;
Donahue, 1985).
The Spiritual Well-Being Scale (SWBS) (Paloutzian & Ellison,
1982) is a general measure of the subjective quality of life. The scale
is nonsectarian and may be used with people from a wide range of beliefs
as well as with nonreligious people (Paloutzian & Ellison, 1982). It
serves as a global psychological measure of one's perception of
spiritual well-being. By design, the construction of the SWBS includes
both a religious and a social psychological dimension. The scale
provides an "overall measure of the perceived spiritual quality of
life, as understood in two senses--a religious sense and an existential
sense" (Paloutzian & Ellison, 1982, p. 2). The religious
dimension (religious well-being--RWB) focuses on how one perceives the
well-being of her or his spiritual life as this is expressed in relation
to God. The social psychological dimension (existential well-being--EWB)
concerns how well the person is adjusted to self, community, and
surroundings. This component involves the existential notions of life
purpose and life satisfaction. Based on these concepts, the SWB scale is
a 20-item self-assessment instrument constructed of two subscales, one
that represents the RWB dimension and one that represents the EWB dimension; each subscale contains 10 items. All of the RWB items contain
the word "God." The EWB items contain no specifically
religious language and instead ask about such things as life purpose,
satisfaction, and relations with the people and situations around us. In
order to control for response set bias, approximately half of the items
are worded in a reverse direction so that disagreement with an item
represents higher well-being. Each item is rated on a six-point Likert
scale with answer options ranging from "strongly agree" to
"strongly disagree," and the items are scored from one to six
with a higher number representing more well-being.
Research reports strong test-retest reliability over a one-week
period for the RWB between .88 and .96, for the EWB between .73 and .98,
and for total SWBS between .82 and .99 (Ellison, 1983). The index of
internal consistency ranges from .82 to .94 (RWB), .78 to .86 (EWB), and
.89 to .94 (SWB) (Bufford, Paloutzian, & Ellison, 1991). The SWBS
has good face validity as is evident by the content of the items. The
SWBS is correlated positively with sense of purpose in life, a positive
self-concept, emotional adjustment, and physical health (Bufford,
Paloutzian, & Ellison, 1983; Paloutzian & Ellison, 1982).
PROCEDURE
The Standing Committee on Ethics in Research Involving Humans at
Monash University approved this research study. Participants were
recruited by several different methods. For the religious group, the
investigator passed out the questionnaires after church meetings to
those willing to participate or gave the questionnaires to clergy to
distribute to other church members. Participants in both groups were
also selected by a snowballing technique from everyday situations in
which the investigator was involved (workplace, sport activities,
philosophy group, university).
Participation was voluntary. The only prerequisite for
participation was that the individual be a male 18 years or over. All
participants received a package containing a document explaining the
nature of the study, the personal particulars sheet, four questionnaires
(the GRCS, the MGRS, the SWBS, and the I/E ROS), and a prepaid reply
envelope.
The participants completed questionnaires on their own time and
either returned them to the researcher directly or by mail. All
participant responses were anonymous in an attempt to decrease the
likelihood of socially desirable responses and to increase the chance of
honest replies. Scores on the GRCS, MGRS, SWBS and I/E ROS subscales
were calculated and together with the personal data were entered into a
data file for further statistical analyses.
RESULTS
Raw data were analysed using SPSS Version 11.0 for Windows.
Frequencies and descriptive statistics were calculated for each of the
variables. Examination of the z-scores revealed 13 univariate outliers.
Cases found to be outliers were adjusted (up or down) to the nearest
score that was not an outlier so that they remained extreme cases but
not outside the distribution, as suggested by Tabachnick and Fidell
(1996). Skewness and kurtosis were insignificant for all variables, and
linearity between variables was assessed by inspection of bivariate scatter plots. No within-set multivariate outliers were identified at
the p < .001 criterion for Mahalanobis distance (Tabachnick &
Fidell, 1996). Assumptions regarding within-set multicollinearity were
met.
Reliability measures for each subscale employed were checked using
Chronbach's alpha, and all subscales showed acceptable internal
reliability. Specifically, alphas for factors of the GRCS were .90 for
SPC, .88 for CBWF, .86 for RE, and .89 for RABBM. Alphas for factors on
the MGRS were .73 for physical inadequacy, .74 for emotional
inexpressiveness, .83 for subordination to women, .72 for intellectual
inferiority, and .82 for performance failure. Alphas for I/E ROS
subscales were .90 for internal religious orientation (intrinsic) and
.63 for external religious orientation (extrinsic). The SWBS yielded
alphas of .90 for the RWB and .86 for the EWB subscale.
Means (M) and standard deviations (SD) of the predictor and
criterion variables for both groups of men (religious and nonreligious)
were calculated. Table 1 shows means and standard deviations for the
religious and nonreligious groups on each of the subscales.
Table 1 shows that the means for all subscales of the GRCS and
MGRSS were higher for the nonreligious than for the religious group,
while means for subscales of the SWBS and I/E ROS were higher for the
religious group.
To assess the difference between the religious and the nonreligious
groups in the levels of gender conflict and stress (hypothesis 1), two
independent-measures t-tests were conducted. Significant differences
were found for the level of gender conflict, (t([sub.141]) = 3.67, p
< .01), and for the level of masculine gender stress, (t([sub.141]) =
3.70, p < .01), with the nonreligious group showing significantly
higher levels of conflict and stress than the religious group.
Pearson's product-moment correlations were used to examine the
nature of the relationships between the subscales employed for both
groups. These are presented in Table 2.
Table 2 indicates that significant positive relationships for the
religious group were between SPC and external religious orientation (r =
.37, n = 73, p < .01) and performance failure and external religious
orientation (r = .31, n = 73, p < .01). Significant negative
relationships were found between SPC and internal religious orientation
(r = -.25, n = 73, p < .05), between performance failure and
existential well-being (r = -.29, n = 73, p < .05), and between
performance failure and religious well-being (r = -.34, n = 73, p <
.01).
For the nonreligious group, Table 2 shows significant positive
relationships between external religious orientation and RAM (r = .27, n
= 70, p < .05), CBWF (r = .32, n = 70, p < .01), SW (r = .33, n =
70, p < .01), and with intellectual inferiority (r = .24, n = 70, p
< .05). Significant negative relationships were found between EWB and
SPC (r = -.26, n = 73, p < .05), RE (r = -.24, n = 70, p < .05),
RAM (r = -.35, n = 70, p < .01), CBWF (r = -.24, n = 70, p < .05),
physical inadequacy (r = -.46, n = 70, p < .01), emotional
inexpressiveness (r = -.46, n = 70, p < .01), subordination to women
(r = -.44, n = 70, p < .0 l), and intellectual inferiority (r = -.46,
n = 70, p< .01).
To examine the hypotheses that gender-role conflict and gender-role
stress would relate to religiousness and spiritual well-being in two
distinct groups of men, separate canonical correlation analyses were
conducted. Canonical correlation was used because it is the only
technique available for examining the interrelationships among sets of
multiple dependent variables and multiple independent variables
(Tabachnick & Fidell, 1996). Canonical correlation's goal is to
quantify the strength of the relationship between sets of variables. It
identifies the optimum structure or dimensionality of each variable set
for maximizing the relationship between independent and dependent
variable sets (Tabachnick & Fidell, 1996). For each canonical
analysis, the gender role conflict factors and gender-role stress
variables formed one side of the model, and the measures of internal
religiousness, external religiousness, existential well-being, and
religious well-being formed the other side of the model.
Examining the religious group, the canonical correlation results
indicated that one function was significant, Wilks's [lambda] =
.42, [chi square] ([sub.36]) = 56.29, p < .05. The first canonical
correlation was .60, and it accounted for 36% of the total variance. For
the nonreligious group, results also indicated one significant function,
Wilks's [lambda] = .42, [chi square] ([sub.36]) = 53.33,p < .05.
The first pair of canonical variates, therefore, accounted for the
significant relationships between the two sets of variables in both
groups. Data on the first pair of canonical variates appears in Table 3.
Shown in the table are structure correlational coefficients (loadings)
for the significant canonical function for the religious and the
non-religious groups.
Table 3 indicates that the canonical function for the religious
group was characterized by a strong positive loading on external
religiousness, a moderate positive loading on PF and SPC, a strong
negative loading on internal religiousness and religious well-being, and
moderate negative loading on EWB. These structural coefficients suggest
that religious men who experience greater rigidity about being
successful/powerful/competitive (.47) and greater stress from
performance failure (.54) reported less existential (-.32) and religious
well-being (-.81) and had less of an internal (-.79) and more of an
external (.84) religious orientation.
Table 3 also shows that the canonical function for the nonreligious
group was characterized by strong positive loadings on RE (.79), RAM
(.71), physical inadequacy (.74), emotional inexpressiveness (.80),
subordination to women (.86), and intellectual inferiority (.80). There
were moderate positive loadings on SPC (.64), CBWF (.59), performance
failure (.55), and a strong negative loading on EWB (.89). These
structure coefficients suggest that non-religious men who experience
greater gender stress and gender conflict reported less existential
well-being (-.89).
DISCUSSION
The aim of this study was to explore the relationship between
masculine gender-role conflict and stress factors on one hand and
religious orientation and spiritual well-being on the other, using a
sample of Australian men. Three hypotheses were examined using
quantitative analyses. Support was found for the hypothesis that
nonreligious men would exhibit higher levels of masculine gender-role
conflict and stress than religious men. Support was also found for the
prediction that masculine gender-role conflict and stress factors would
be related to less intrinsic religiousness for men. Results also
indicated that masculine gender-role conflict and stress factors were
related to less spiritual well-being for both religious and nonreligious
men.
The findings of this study that nonreligious men exhibited higher
levels of masculine gender-role conflict and stress in comparison to
religious men are consistent with findings by Maton (1989) and Pargament
and Brant (1998) that religion can moderate the effect of stress. Also,
the findings of the current study appear to be in line with
Thorson's (1998) findings that persons who are more religious
experience less anxiety as well as with findings from Ellison (1991)
that individuals with strong religious faith report a higher level of
life satisfaction and greater personal happiness.
The results of this study, that Australian men with higher levels
of gender-role conflict and stress have less intrinsic and more
extrinsic religious orientation, partly echo findings from the U.S.
samples (Mahalik & Lagan, 2001). Also, the results are consistent
with Mahalik and Lagan's view that rigidity in terms of traditional
masculinity (as well as stress resulting from living up to traditional
male roles) relates to less intrinsic religiousness. It could be argued
that men who strive to live up to a stereotypical "masculine"
ideal, and avoid anything that appears "feminine" (Levant
& Pollack, 1998) are likely to restrict themselves in terms of
developing intrinsically oriented religious behavior (Zock, 1997).
Conversely, it is possible that men whose socialization experiences
develop their "feminine outlook" seek out the connectedness
inherent within religion (Thompson, 1991). Thompson argued that men who
have developed a "feminine side" to their gender perspective
might feel more comfortable crossing over traditional gender boundaries
to participate actively in religion.
Although the inverse relationship between conflict/stress factors
and intrinsic religious orientation was found for both groups
(nonreligious and religious), the pattern was not the same. The results
for the nonreligious group revealed an inverse relationship among all
factors assessing gender-role conflict/stress and intrinsic religious
orientation. Also, the loadings on those factors were high, suggesting
strong rigid masculine identity. As expected, for the religious group,
the loading on the intrinsic factor was much higher compared to the
nonreligious group, and a strong inverse relationship was found only
with the "success, power, competition" (SPC) factor and the
"performance failure" (PF) factor. It appears that even
religious men (with a "feminine mode" and less rigid
"masculinity") are affected in their religious commitment to
the degree that they have internalised messages about success, power,
competition, and performance. Also, this finding indicates that the
image of the Australian male as a strong, sexually active breadwinner is
still present in Australian men. This is not surprising, since in
Australia masculinity is historically associated with authority and
power at all class levels (Connell et al., 1998), and the traditional
image (symbol) of masculinity is of the self-sufficient, rough-and-ready
bronzed man of the ANZAC legend (1) (Connell et al., 1998). Apparently,
men are still socialized to believe in the importance of success, power,
and competitiveness (the ANZAC ideal has been replaced by the successful
sportsman).
One finding of this study is that men who exhibit a higher level of
gender role conflict and stress experience less existential well-being
(EWB). Since EWB is a reflection of a sense of life purpose and life
satisfaction, this finding adds to previous findings (Cournoyer &
Mahalik, 1995; Eisler, Skidmore, & Ward, 1988; Good et al., 1995)
that gender-role conflict and masculine gender-role stress are powerful
predictors of men's psychological distress. This finding is also
consistent with Ellison's (1991) study, which reported that
religious belief enhances cognitive and affective perceptions of life
quality. Again there is a different pattern of the results between the
two groups. Specifically, for the nonreligious group all gender-role
conflict and gender-role stress factors were inversely related to
existential well-being. For the religious group, only SPC (success,
power, and competition) and PF (performance failure) were strongly
inversely related to existential well-being. It appears that both groups
suffer stress, perhaps due to a lack of understanding that men's
roles and society in Australia are changing (and things are not as they
were for previous generations). Those men who are unsuccessful (i.e.,
unemployed) suffer, because acculturation and conditioning results in a
man being valued in terms of "what he does."
Religion appears to be one important way to have a sense of
well-being. The notion that religion provides a framework that gives
meaning and purpose to life, providing a sense of well-being, has been
supported by several empirical investigations (Chamberlain & Zika,
1992; Crandall & Rasmussen, 1975). Although religion may be only one
of many ways in which meaning is assessed, Ellison (1991) found that a
strong faith allows people to hold onto happiness after unemployment,
serious illness, divorce, or bereavement.
Previous findings (Cournoyer & Mahalik, 1995; Good et al.,
1995; Sharpe & Heppner, 1991) that men with high levels of
gender-role conflict and strong masculine identity experience more
negative psychological consequences, including personal strain and
stress, were confirmed by this study. In addition, the results of this
study suggest that intrinsic religiousness--living one's
religion--is associated with more positive psychological outcomes for
men. As gender-role conflict/stress was significantly lower for the
religious than for the nonreligious group, it could be speculated that
such phenomena as the high rate of suicide among Australian men could be
linked to the current low cultural valuation of
religiousness/spirituality. Thus, it is advisable that psychologists,
especially counselors, break away from viewing psychology as a
one-dimensional science and recognize the multifaceted layers of
existence. In keeping with the predominant "illness model,"
counselors are more often concerned with risk factors for depression,
suicide, substance misuse, and violence than with the less-publicized
protective factors, which include "connectedness" and
"spirituality" (Hassed, 2000). Gauging a client's
spiritual awareness, at the very least, should form an important part of
a thorough client history. One cannot really be said to know another
person without an understanding of his or her responses to the most
important questions that human beings ask themselves (Hassed, 2000). A
psychology that fails to recognize that religiousness and spirituality
(in the context of life meaning) are very important aspects of human
existence risks losing its human relevance and becoming a somewhat
emotionally bankrupt discipline. In general, all those who are
attempting to deal with men's health issues should give more
attention to a spiritual dimension in men's lives. Also, society as
a whole should accept men having a broader range of characteristics than
the narrow view of stereotyped masculinity. A nonharmful action in
redefining masculine roles would be to discard the pressures for men to
exhibit high-risk behavior in order to be accepted as men. The
boundaries of maleness need to be widened to include views that are
presently considered feminine--that it is okay to show emotion, to talk
about feelings, to be religious/spiritual.
LIMITATIONS AND FUTURE RESEARCH
Some of the participants objected to the limitations imposed by
questions in the I/E ROS and the SWBS, often adding in margins
qualifications or alternatives they felt were needed to represent their
own views. While the measures used are validated instruments, those
comments indicated how challenging it is to gather scientifically
appropriate data of what religiousness and spirituality mean for the
individual. What makes this task difficult is the fact that a religious
tradition is a complex phenomenon of doctrine, community, identity,
structure, and rituals. Since a person receives his or her religion
through a form of religious tradition, it is important to remember that
it is the individual who has internalized the religious formation, thus
making the measurement of the concepts even more challenging. It is
possible that the results of this study are affected by methodological
flaws, such as inadequacies in the way religiousness and spiritual
well-being were measured by using the I/E ROS-Revised and the SWBS,
especially with the nonreligious participants. However, the large number
of studies that have used both measures and the consistency of the
psychometric properties across the range of different samples (some of
which included nonreligious participants) provide a strong argument for
not dismissing the results of this study.
A general criticism of self-report measures is that respondents may
respond in a "socially desirable" fashion. Men who are
inclined to portray themselves favorably would tend to provide lower
scores on certain subscales of the GRCS and the MGRS (i.e., they would
describe themselves as more sexually capable). For future research,
additional insights could be gained by using a form for peer ratings
(for the GRCS and the MGRS). Taking into account correlations between
self-reported and peer-rated measures might be useful.
The fact that most of the religious participants were Christians
indicates the need for caution in extending the findings to other
religious groups in the Australian community. To draw any stronger
conclusions and in order to be truly representative of the contemporary
multicultural Australian society, future research could use a much
larger and diverse sample, which would include men from at least the
major religious denominations. A mix of denominational and
congregational data is also required to clarify the precise links
between collective religious participation, private beliefs, and various
dimensions of personal well-being. Future research could also explore
more fully the links between the various aspects of
religiousness/spirituality and aspects of self-concept and personality.
For example, Bochner's (1976) study of religious role salience showed that Christians and Moslems provide very different
self-references. "I am a Moslem" is among the top three
self-descriptions, while Christians either did not mention their faith
at all or did so after giving other, for them, more salient
self-references (Bochner, 1976).
CONCLUSION
By using a sample of Australian men, this study extended previous
research by Mahalik and Lagan (2001) that tapped the area of the
restrictive nature of masculine socialization in relation to men's
religiousness and spiritual well-being. The results of this study
supported the notion that men who hold less stereotyped views about
being "male" experience less gender-role conflict and stress,
more intrinsic religiousness, and more well-being. In the light of the
unfortunate fact that in Australia a tragically high number of men take
their own lives, it is believed that the results of this study shed some
additional light on the negative affects of masculine gender-role
strain. In addition, they give credence to the recommendation that, in
order to help men, psychotherapists and psychologists should be prepared
to address religious and spiritual issues in the context of their
clinical and counseling interventions.
REFERENCES
Allport, G.W., & Ross, J.M. (1967). Personal religious
orientation and prejudice. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
5, 432-443.
Argyle, M. (2000). Psychology and religion: An introduction.
London: Routledge.
Argyle, M., & Beit-Hallahmi, B. (1975). The social psychology
of religion. London: Routledge & Kegan Paul.
Australian Bureau of Statistics. (2000). Australian Social Trends
2000--Health--Mortality and Morbidity: Suicide. Canberra, Australian
Capital Territory: Author.
Australian Bureau of Statistics. (2001). Australian Social Trends
2001--Religion. Canberra, Australian Capital Territory: Author.
Baker, M., & Gorsuch, R. (1982). Trait anxiety and
intrinsic/extrinsic religiousness. Journal for the Scientific Study of
Religion, 21, 119-122.
Batson, C.D., & Ventis, W.L. (1982). The religious experience:
A socio-psychological perspective. New York: Oxford University Press.
Bergin, A.E. (1991). Values and religious issues in psychotherapy and mental health. American Psychologist, 46, 394-403.
Bergin, A., Masters, K., & Richards, P. (1987) Religiousness
and mental health reconsidered: A study of an intrinsically religious
sample. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 34, 197-204.
Blazina, C., & Watkins, C.E., Jr. (1996). Masculine gender role
conflict: Effects on college men's psychological well-being,
chemical substance use, and attitudes toward help-seeking. Journal of
Counseling Psychology, 43, 461-465.
Bochner, S. (1976). Religious role differentiation as an aspect of
subjective culture. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 7, 3-19.
Bufford, R.K., Paloutzian, R.F., & Ellison, C.W. (1991). Norms
for the spiritual well-being scale. Journal of Psychology and Theology,
19, 56-70.
Chamberlain, K., & Zika, S. (1992). Religiosity, meaning in
life, and psychological well-being. In J.F. Schumaker (Ed.), Religion
and mental health (pp. 138-148). New York: Oxford University Press.
Connell, R.W. (1996). Masculinities. Sydney: Allen & Unwin.
Connell, R.W., Schofield, T., Walker, L., Wood, J., Butland, D.L.,
Fisher, J., et al. (1998). Men's health: A research agenda and
background report. Report submitted to the Commonwealth Department of
Health & Aged Care, Canberra.
Cournoyer, R.J., & Mahalik, J.R. (1995). Cross-sectional study
of gender role conflict examining college-aged and middle-aged men.
Journal of Counseling Psychology, 42, 11-19.
Crandall, J., & Rasmussen, R. (1975). Purpose in life as
related to specific values. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 31, 483-485.
DeVaus, D., & McAllister, I. (1987). Gender differences in
religion: A test of the structural location theory. American
Sociological Review, 52, 472-481.
Donahue, M.J. (1985). Intrinsic and extrinsic religiousness: Review
and meta-analysis. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 48,
400-419.
Eisler, R.M. (1995). The relationship between masculine gender role
stress and men's health risk: The validation of the construct. In
R.F. Levant & W.S. Pollack (Eds.), A new psychology of men (pp.
207-225). New York: Basic Books.
Eisler, R.M., & Skidmore, J.R. (1987). Masculine gender role
stress: Scale development and component factors in the appraisal of
stressful situations. Behavior Modification, 11, 123-136.
Eisler, R.M., Skidmore, J.R., & Ward, C.H. (1988). Masculine
gender stress: Predictor of anger, anxiety, and health-risk behaviors.
Journal of Personality Assessment, 52, 133-141.
Ekhardt, B.N., & Goldsmith, W.M. (1984). Personality factors of
men and women pastoral candidates: Part 1. Motivational profile. Journal
of Psychology and Theology, 12, 109-118.
Ellison, C.W. (1983). Spiritual well-being: Conceptualization and
measurement. Journal of Psychology and Theology, 11, 330-340.
Ellison, C.G. (1991). Religious involvement and subjective
well-being. Journal of Health and Social Behavior, 32, 80-99.
Erikson, E.H. (1958). Young man Luther: A study in psychoanalysis
and history. New York: Norton.
Erikson, E.H. (1969). Gandhi's truth. On the origin of
militant non-violence. New York: Norton.
Fragoso, J.M., & Kashubeck, S. (2000). Machismo, gender role
conflict, and mental health in Mexican American men. Psychology of Men
& Masculinity, 2, 87-97.
Francis, L.J. (1991). The personality characteristics of Anglican
ordinands: Feminine men and masculine women? Personality and Individual
Differences, 12, 1133-1140.
Francis, L.J., & Wilcox, C. (1996). Religion and gender
orientation. Personality and Individual Differences, 20, 119-121.
Gartner, J., Larson, D.B., & Allen, G. (1991). Religious
commitment and mental health: A review of the empirical literature.
Journal of Psychology and Theology, 19, 6-25.
Genia, V. (1993). A psychometric evaluation of the Allport-Ross I/E
scales in a religiously heterogeneous sample. Journal for the Scientific
Study of Religion, 32, 284-290.
Genia, G.E., & Shaw, D.G. (1991). Religion, intrinsic-extrinsic
orientation, and depression. Review of Religious Research, 32, 274-283.
Goldsmith, W.M., & Ekhardt, B.N. (1984). Personality factors of
men and women pastoral candidates: Part 2: Sex role preferences. Journal
of Psychology and Theology, 12, 211-221.
Good, G.E., & Mintz, L.B. (1990). Gender role conflict and
depression in college men: Evidence for compounded risk. Journal of
Counseling and Development, 69, 17-21.
Good, G.E., Robertson, J.M., O'Neil, J., Fitzgerald, L.F.,
Stevens, M., DeBord, K.A., et al. (1995). Male gender role conflict:
Psychometric issues and relations to psychological stress. Journal of
Counseling Psychology, 42, 3-10.
Gorsuch, R.L. (1984). Measurement: The boon and bone of
investigating religion. American Psychologist, 39, 228-236.
Gorsuch, R.L. (1995). Religious aspects of substance abuse and
recovery. Journal of Social Issues, 51, 65-83.
Gorsuch, R.L., & Venable, G.D. (1983). Development of an Age
Universal I-E scale. Journal for the Scientific Study of Religion, 22,
181-187.
Hassan, R. (1995). Suicide explained: The Australian experience.
Melbourne: Melbourne University Press.
Hassed, C.S. (2000). Depression: Dispirited or spiritually
deprived? The Medical Journal of Australia, 173, 545-547.
Hood, R., Jr. (1970). Religious orientation and the report of
religious experience. Journal for the Scientific Study of Religion, 9,
285-291.
Kahoe, R.D. (1974). Personality and achievement correlates of
intrinsic and extrinsic religious orientations. Journal of Personality
and Social Psychology, 29, 812-818.
Kennedy, G.J. (1998). Religion and depression. In H.G. Koenig
(Ed.), Handbook of religion and mental health (pp. 128-146). New York:
Academic Press.
Kidd, M. (2002). Suicide and manhood. Manhood online. Retrieved
October 18, 2002, from http://www.manhood.com.au/scripts/manhood.
Koenig, H.G. (1998). Handbook of religion and mental health. New
York: Academic Press.
Levant, R.F., & Pollack, W.S. (1995). The new psychology of
men. New York: Basic Books.
Loewenthal, K.M. (2000). The psychology of religion: A short
introduction. Oxford: Oneworld Publications.
Mahalik, J.R., & Lagan. H.D. (2001). Examining masculine gender
role conflict and stress in relation to religious orientation and
spiritual well-being. Psychology of Men & Masculinity, 2, 2-33.
Maltby, J., & Lewis, C.A. (1996). Measuring intrinsic and
extrinsic orientation toward religion: Amendments for its use among
religious and non-religious samples. Personality and Individual
Differences, 21,937-946. Mann, L. (1997). Youth suicide: Can primary
care help? Australian Nursing Journal, 4, 5-7.
Maton, K.I. (1989). The stress-buffering role of spiritual support:
Cross-sectional and prospective investigations. Journal for the
Scientific Study of Religion, 28, 310-323.
O'Neil, J.M. (1981). Male sex-role conflicts, sexism, and
masculinity: Psychological implications for men, women, and the
counselling psychologist. The Counseling Psychologist, 9, 61-81.
O'Neil, J.M. (1982). Gender role conflict and strain in
men's lives: Implications for psychiatrists, psychologists, and
other human-service providers. In K. Solomon & N. B. Levy (Eds.),
Men in transition: Theory and therapy (pp. 5-49). New York: Plenum.
O'Neil, J.M. (1990). Assessing men's gender role
conflict. In D. Moore & F. Leafgren (Eds.), Problem solving strategies and interventions for men in conflict (pp. 23-38).
Alexandria, VA: American Counseling Association.
O'Neil, J.M., Good, G.E., & Holmes, S. (1995). Fifteen
years of theory and research on men's gender role conflict: New
paradigms for empirical research. In R.F. Levant & W.S. Pollack
(Eds.), The new psychology of men (pp. 164-206). New York: Basic Books.
O'Neil, J.M., Helms, B., Gable, R., David, L., &
Wrightsman, L. (1986). Gender role conflict scale: College men's
fear of femininity. Sex Roles, 14, 335-350.
Paloma, M.M., & Gallup, G.H., Jr. (1991). Varieties of prayer:
A survey report. Philadelphia: Trinity Press International.
Paloutzian, R.F., & Ellison, C.W. (1982). Loneliness, spiritual
well-being and the quality of life. In L.A. Peplau & D. Perlman
(Eds.), Loneliness: A sourcebook of current theory, research and therapy
(pp. 224-236). New York: Wiley.
Pargament, K.I., & Brant, C.R. (1998). Religion and coping. In
H.G. Koenig (Ed.), Handbook of religion and mental health (pp. 111-128).
New York: Academic Press.
Pleck, J.H. (1981). The myth of masculinity. Cambridge: MIT Press.
Pleck, J.H. (1995). The gender role strain paradigm: An update. In
R.F. Levant & W.S. Pollack (Eds.), A new psychology of men (pp.
11-32). New York: Basic Books.
Pollner, M. (1989). Divine relations, social relations, and
well-being. Journal of Health and Social Behavior, 30, 92-104
Rando, R.A., Rogers, J.R., & Brittan-Powell, C.S. (1998).
Gender role conflict and college men's sexually aggressive attitudes and behavior. Journal of Mental Health Counseling, 20,
359-369.
Schreurs, A. (2002). Psychotherapy and spirituality: Integrating
the spiritual dimension into therapeutic practice. London: Jessica
Kingsley Publishers.
Sharpe, M.J., & Heppner, P.P. (1991). Gender role, gender role
conflict, and psychological well-being in men. Journal of Counseling
Psychology, 38, 323-330.
Slater, W., Hall, T.W., & Edwards, K.J. (2001). Measuring
religion and spirituality: Where are we and where are we going? Journal
of Psychology and Theology, 29, 4-24.
Spilka, B., Hood, R.W., & Gorsuch, R.L. (1985). The psychology
of religion: An empirical approach. Englewood Cliffs, N J: Prentice
Hall.
Tabachnick, B.G., & Fidell, L.S. (2001). Using multivariate
statistics (4th ed.). Boston: Allyn and Bacon.
Yacey, D. (2000). Re-enchantment: The new Australian spirituality.
Sydney: Harper Collins Publishers.
Thompson, E.H. (1991). Beneath the status characteristics: Gender
variation in religiousness. Journal for the Scientific Study of
Religion, 30, 381-394.
Thorson, J.A. (1998). Religion and anxiety: Which anxiety? Which
religion? In H.G. Koenig (Ed.), Handbook of religion and mental health
(pp. 147-160). New York: Academic Press.
Turbott, J. (1996). Religion, spirituality and psychiatry:
Conceptual, cultural and personal challenges. Australian and New Zealand Journal of Psychiatry, 30, 720-727.
Vaughan, F. (1991). Spiritual issues in psychotherapy. Journal of
Transpersonal Psychology, 23, 105-119.
Watkins, P.L., Eisler, R.M., Carpenter, L., Schechtman, K.B., &
Fisher, E.B. (1991). Psychosocial and physiological correlates of male
gender role stress among employed adults. Behavioral Medicine, 17,
86-90.
Watson, P.J., Hood, R.W., Jr., Morris, R.J., & Hall, J.R.
(1984). Empathy, religious orientation, and social desirability. The
Journal of Psychology, 117, 211-216.
Wulff, D.M. (1997). Psychology of religion: Classic and
contemporary. New York: Wiley.
Zock, H. (1997). The predominance of the feminine sexual mode in
religion: Erikson's contribution to the sex and gender debate in
the psychology of religion. International Journal for the Psychology of
Religion, 7, 187-198.
NOTE
(1.) ANZAC stands for Australian and New Zealand Army Corps. The
soldiers in those forces quickly became known as ANZACs, and the pride
they soon took in that name endures to this day. ANZAC Day (25 April)
marks the anniversary of the first major military action fought by
Australian and New Zealand forces during the First World War.
Australians recognize 25 April as an occasion of national commemoration.
It is a day when Australians reflect on the many different meanings of
war.
DUBRAVKO JURKOVIC and GORDON A. WALKER
Monash University, Australia
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to
Gordon Walker, Department of Psychology, School of Psychology,
Psychiatry and Psychological Medicine, Faculty of Medicine, Nursing and
Health Sciences, Monash University, 900 Dandenong Road, Caulfield East,
Melbourne, Australia 3145. Electronic mail:
gordon.walker@med.monash.edu.au.
Table 1
Means and Standard Deviations for Religious (N = 73) and
Nonreligious (N = 70) Groups on Each of the Subscales Employed
Religious Nonreligious
Variable M SD M SD
Gender Role Conflict Scale
* success, power, and competition 34.05 11.75 40.04 14.84
* restrictive emotionality 23.33 7.81 30.29 10.87
* restrictive affectionate behavior
between men 19.88 7.25 23.96 10.50
* conflict between work and family 18.42 7.57 20.90 8.61
Masculine Gender Role Stress Scale
* physical inadequacy 14.52 7.05 18.57 8.48
* emotional inexpressiveness 8.23 4.77 11.07 5.70
* subordination to women 5.26 3.89 7.51 6.52
* intellectual inferiority 8.75 5.03 10.73 6.01
* performance failure 19.77 7.36 24.56 5.74
Spiritual Well-Being Scale
* existential well-being 49.49 7.73 42.13 9.86
* religious well-being 49.53 10.24 22.20 10.97
Internal-External Religious Orientation Scale
* internal orientation 20.19 3.43 10.93 2.80
* external orientation 16.86 4.01 15.64 3.13
Table 2
Correlation Matrix for Gender-Role Conflict Factors, Gender-Role Stress
Factors, Spiritual Well-Being Scales, and Internal-External Religious
Orientation Scale Scores
Variable 1 2 3 4 5
1. SPC .46 ** .41 ** .50 ** .49 **
2. RE .49 ** .63 ** .36 ** .38 **
3. RAM .56 ** .64 ** .32 ** .40 **
4. CBWF .64 ** .38 .51 ** .34 **
5. PI .55 ** .40 ** .41 ** .32 **
6. EI .39 ** .62 ** .54 ** .31 ** .72 **
7. SW .56 ** .45 ** .47 ** .33 ** .75 **
8. II .53 ** .51 ** .54 ** .40 ** .79 **
9. PF .43 ** .27 * .35 ** .31 ** .76 **
10. EWB -.26 * -.24 * -.35 ** -.24 * -.46 **
11. RWB .02 -.11 -.09 -.11 -.01
12. IRO .03 -.09 -.10 -.02 -.09
13. ERO .33 .22 .27 * .32 ** .15
Variable 6 7 8 9 10
1. SPC .37 ** .32 ** .57 ** .56 ** -.23
2. RE .45 ** .34 ** .42 ** .36 ** -.21
3. RAM .53 ** .40 ** .47 ** .21 -.01
4. CBWF .25 * .18 .48 ** .41 ** .14
5. PI .57 ** .56 * .66 ** .71 ** -.20
6. EI .53 ** .65 ** .32 ** -.16
7. SW .70 ** .57 ** .34 ** -.04
8. II .78 ** .77 ** .54 ** -.18
9. PF .61 ** .54 ** .70 ** -.29 *
10. EWB -.46 ** -.44 ** -.46 ** -.33 **
11. RWB -.09 .12 -.03 -.12 .22
12. IRO -.13 -.05 -.04 -.23 .15
13. ERO .19 .33 ** .24 * .03 -.22
Variable 11 12 13
1. SPC -.18 -.25* .37**
2. RE -.08 -.02 .12
3. RAM .06 .07 .16
4. CBWF -.11 -.18 .18
5. PI -.19 -.08 .21
6. EI -.12 -.07 .23
7. SW .04 .00 .09
8. II .05 .09 .06
9. PF -.34 ** -.21 .31 **
10. EWB .66 ** .46 ** -.29
11. RWB .79 ** -.48 **
12. IRO .39 ** -.49 **
13. ERO .36 ** .51 **
* p < .05, two tailed. ** p < .01, two tailed
Note. Above the diagonal are correlations for the religious group
(n = 73); below the diagonal are correlations for the nonreligious
group (n = 70). SPC = success, power, and competition; RE = restrictive
emotionality; RAM = restrictive affectionate behavior between man;
CBWF = conflict between work and family; PI = physical inadequacy;
EI = emotional inexpressiveness; SW = subordination of women;
II = intellectual inferiority; PF = performance failure;
EWB = existential well-being; RWB = religious well-being;
IRO = internal religious orientation; ERO = external religious
orientation.
Table 3
Structure Correlational Coefficients (Canonical Loadings) for the
Significant Canonical Function Examining Masculine and Religious
Variables for Religious and Nonreligious Sample
Function Function
Variable Religious Nonreligious
Gender-Role Conflict Scale
* success, power, and competition .47 .64
* restrictive emotionality .09 .79
* restrictive affectionate behavior .06 .71
between men
* conflict between work and family .25 .59
Gender-Role Stress Scale
* physical inadequacy .29 .74
* emotional inexpressiveness .27 .80
* subordination to women .02 .86
* intellectual inferiority -.12 .80
* performance failure .54 .55
Spiritual Well-Being Scale
* existential well-being -.32 -.89
* religious well-being -.81 -.06
Internal-External Religious Orientation Scale
* internal orientation -.79 -.14
* external orientation .84 .56