The Inventory of Father Involvement: a pilot study of a new measure of father involvement.
Hawkins, Alan J. ; Bradford, Kay P. ; Palkovitz, Rob 等
In the past few years, a number of fathering scholars have sensed
the need for improved measurement of the construct of father involvement
if empirical work on this topic and its importance to child development,
adult development, and family well-being is to continue to progress
(Hawkins & Palkovitz, 1999; Lamb, 1999; Marsiglio, Amato, Day, &
Lamb, 2000). Although the study of father involvement goes back several
generations, only in the last 10-15 years have a large number of
scholars come to this area of inquiry. Good measurement work is
challenging and takes time. In order to mature, the field needs a
focused and sustained effort among family and child development scholars
to match their measures of father involvement to the rich and complex
concept it seeks to understand. In this paper, we offer an initial
attempt to advance the measurement of father involvement.
There are many important methodological critiques of the extant empirical literature on father involvement (e.g., Marsiglio, Amato, Day,
& Lamb, 2000), but first among those, we believe, is an accurate
conceptualization and operationalization of the meaning of the concept
(Sabatelli, 1988). Current scholars understand that father involvement
is a much richer and deeper construct than typical operationalizations
suggest. Hawkins and Palkovitz (1999) and others argue that the term
father involvement, as it has been used over the past 25 years, is
conceptualized and operationalized primarily as a temporal and readily
observable phenomenon (Lamb, 1997; Lamb, 1999; Lamb, Pleck, Chamov,
& Levine, 1985; Palkovitz, 1997; Pleck, 1997). That is, father
involvement is portrayed as time that fathers spend with children, or
discrete events tallied, usually in direct interaction with children.
This portrayal is a result of the pioneering work of developmental
psychologists, many of whom emphasize methodologies that lend themselves
to quantifiable time and observable interaction (Lamb, 1999). In
addition, the emphasis on temporal involvement fits with a broader
social agenda: the need for fathers to assume a greater load of direct
caregiving because of mothers' greater involvement in paid labor.
Time--or the lack of it--may be a crucial way that parents--men and
women--think about their involvement with children (Daly, 2001;
Hochschild, 1997). But time is not the only important dimension to
father involvement (Palkovitz, 1997). Father involvement is a
multidimensional construct that includes affective, cognitive, and
ethical components, as well as observable behavioral components, and
that includes indirect forms of involvement (e.g., providing, supporting
mother), as well (Hawkins & Palkovitz, 1999; Lamb, 1999).
In this paper, we report on an initial effort by a team of scholars
to address the need for broader and richer conceptualizations of father
involvement that go beyond the narrow interest in the amount of time
fathers spend with their children. This empirical work follows earlier
conceptual work (Hawkins & Palkovitz, 1999; Palkovitz, 1997) that
attempted to broaden and enrich our notion of the meaning of father
involvement. (For a more detailed critique of measurement issues
regarding father involvement, see Hawkins & Palkovitz, 1999). Our
goal was to develop a reliable and valid self-report instrument for
fathers that captured the breadth and richness of father involvement,
but was short enough for inclusion in large-scale surveys of broader
family issues. We readily admit up front that such a task is beyond the
scope of just one empirical inquiry. Nevertheless, we share our initial
results in the hope of encouraging further and similar efforts.
THE IFI PILOT STUDY
To generate potential items for a self-report measure, we worked
with groups of students in graduate seminars to give more breadth to the
concept of father involvement (Palkovitz, 1997). In addition, we
consulted a handful of recent scholarly works related to father
involvement to further enrich the item pool and guide construction of
the measure (Doherty, Kouneski, & Erickson, 1998; Dollahite &
Hawkins, 1998; Snarey, 1993). From this work we generated more than 100
potential items for the measure, which we eventually reduced to 43 items
termed the "Inventory of Father Involvement" (IFI). We sought
to include items that tapped behavioral, cognitive, affective, and
moral/ethical dimensions of father involvement and that allowed for
indirect as well as direct involvement. We also attempted to word items
in ways that would be applicable for fathers in both married and
unmarried or divorced household structures.
A pilot survey was conducted that included a sample of fathers
reporting on "how good a job" they were doing on the 43
diverse indicators of The Inventory of Father Involvement. The survey
also asked the fathers to rate the importance of each item to being a
good father. Finally, the survey asked fathers to "grade
themselves" (A, B, C, D, F) on seven global indicators of father
involvement.
SAMPLE AND PROCEDURES
The week of Father's Day, 1998, 2,200 surveys were mailed to a
nationally representative sample of fathers. Two follow-up notices were
sent inviting fathers to complete and mail back the seven-page survey.
We received back 739 completed surveys for a response rate of 34%. This
rate is typical of mail-in surveys for men with no incentive provided
for completing the task.
Analyses here are limited to 723 fathers who were between the ages
of 22-59. (One father was 19 years old, and 15 fathers were older than
60. The surveys for both the young and older fathers contained
considerable missing data. Hence, we decided to drop them from the
analyses.) Approximately 84% reported having fathered or adopted 1-3
children; the modal number of children was two. The mean age of
respondents was 41 (SD = 7.3). Ninety-one percent were married. One
percent had never married and 16% had married two or more times. About
91% reported being White, with about two percent each reporting they
were African American, Mexican American, Hispanic (other than Mexican
American), and Asian. The respondents reported many different religious
preferences, with Roman Catholicism being by far the largest group
(29%). Nearly half reported attending church services regularly; 15% did
not attend services at all. About two percent of the sample had less
than a high school education; about a quarter of the sample had only a
high school education. Again, about another quarter of the sample
completed a four-year college degree, while 29% reported having
completing some college. About 20% of the sample reported post-graduate
education. Only about 2% of the sample were currently unemployed. Mean
work hours for these fathers was 47 (SD = 9.5). About 4% of the sample
worked less than 40 hours a week; 47% worked from 40-45 hours a week;
another 23% worked from 46-50 hours a week; a quarter of the sample
worked more than 50 hours a week. Most respondents were in dual-earner
families; only about 30% of the spouses of the respondents were not in
paid employment. Of those employed-for-pay spouses, the mean number of
work hours was 34 (SD = 12.7). About 36% of employed spouses worked 30
hours or less. Another 59% worked from 31-40 hours a week. Another 4%
worked more than 50 hours a week.
Although there was SES variation in our sample, it was clear that
those who returned the survey rated themselves as doing a better job of
fathering than a truly nationally representative sample. We included in
the survey a question taken from a national Gallup Poll survey in 1997
(Newport, 1997): "Using an A, B, C, D, and F grading scale like
they do in school, what grade would you give yourself for the job you
are doing in bringing up your children?" Comparing the responses of
our sample to the responses of the Gallup Poll survey, we found that 11%
of fathers in the IFI sample gave themselves a grade of C or D,
comparable to the Gallup sample of 13%. (No fathers in either sample
gave themselves a grade of F.) However, fathers in the IFI sample were
much more likely to give themselves a grade of A (41%) compared to the
Gallup Poll fathers (23%). Consequently, fewer fathers in the IFI sample
gave themselves a grade of B (49%) compared to the Gallup Poll fathers
(62%). Thus, our IFI sample had a much higher proportion of fathers who
rated themselves as doing an excellent job as opposed to just a good job
compared to a more representative national sample.
EXPLORATORY ANALYSES OF THE IFI STRUCTURE
Although we constructed the IFI with the notion that father
involvement was multidimensional, we did not begin our analyses with a
precise hypothesis of the number and nature of those dimensions
(although we had some expectations). A preliminary, exploratory
principle components analysis of the 43 items with a promax rotation
(expecting and getting substantial correlations among the interrelated components) yielded nine factors with eigen values greater than one that
were relatively distinct and straightforward to interpret. (Four items
were dropped because they did not load well on any of these factors.)
The IFI distinguishes nine distinct, potentially important dimensions of
father involvement, including the more instrumental and traditional
dimensions (i.e., providing, support of the mother, disciplining and
teaching responsibility, and encouraging success in school), as well as
dimensions reflecting some of the additional tasks expected of
contemporary fathers (i.e., giving praise and affection, spending time
together and talking, being attentive to their children's daily
lives, reading to their children, and encouraging children to develop
their talents). A second-order principle components analysis of the nine
factors suggested the possibility that two second-order factors might
exist, one capturing the more instrumental and "traditional"
aspects of father involvement and the other capturing some of the
"new father" aspects emphasized in most contemporary
investigations of fathering.
CONFIRMATORY ANALYSES OF THE IFI STRUCTURE
From these exploratory analyses we proceeded with more rigorous
confirmatory factor analyses using structural equation modeling (SAS's proc calis procedure). A handful of potential structures
were tested, all variations of nine first-order factors. We began with a
simple, first-order model allowing for correlations among all nine
factors. The numerous high correlations among the factors in this
first-order model suggested the possibility of a second-order factor
structure that would better model the data. We tested a second-order
factor structure with two second-order factors ("traditional"
involvement and "new-father" involvement) as well as a more
parsimonious second-order model with a single global factor of father
involvement. The model with two second-order factors produced highly
correlated second-order factors (.87). There was little difference
between this model and a third-order factor model. Although there were
not large differences in goodness of fit between the various models
tested, the most parsimonious as well as the best fitting model was the
model depicting nine first-order factors indicating a single, global
second-order factor of father involvement. Moreover, this model most
closely reflected our initial expectations of the structure of the IFI.
This model is depicted in Figure 1. Table 1 provides the items with
their respective factors and Cronbach alpha statistics. (Because the
content of the specific IFI items is central to this article, we urge
the reader to examine them at length. Also, note that four more items
were eliminated in these confirmatory analyses because their disturbance terms were highly correlated with numerous other terms and eliminating
them did not detract from the conceptual richness of the factors. This
left the WI with 35 items. Some additional correlated disturbance terms
were allowed where they made sense conceptually in order to increase the
fit of the model.)
[FIGURE 1 OMITTED]
A SHORTER-VERSION IFI
A goal of our research team was to produce a self-report measure of
father involvement that would be brief enough to be used for most survey
purposes. Hence, we wanted to shorten the IFI from the 35 items depicted
in Figure 1. Because three of the factors had more than three items
loading on them, we limited them to three items each, using several
criteria for deciding to include them: the items retained had strong
factor loadings, greater item variation in the sample, strong
correlations with other items in the factor (to maintain high internal
consistency reliability), and strong face validity. A shorter version of
the IFI with just 26 items (the "Providing" factor only had
two items), but still nine first-order factors and a single, global,
second-order factor, is depicted in Figure 2. This model has an even
better fit than the larger model (due to fewer covariances to account
for). The reduction in the number of items on the three shortened
factors still left them with strong internal consistency reliability
coefficients.
[FIGURE 2 OMITTED]
VALIDITY ANALYSES
An initial attempt at establishing the validity of the IFI was made
in two ways. First, in order to assess face validity, a focus group of
fathers provided feedback about the instrument. Second, construct or
concurrent validity was examined by correlating each IFI factor with
conceptually related items included in other parts of the questionnaire.
Face Validity. In a focus group setting, we asked fathers to give
us their feedback on the IFI. Five fathers from Northeast Pennsylvania
participated in the focus group session. They reflected the wide
diversity of socioeconomic, occupational, ethnic, and religious
backgrounds of the area. Two of the fathers were from a local,
low-income housing project sponsored by the area housing authority. One
of these fathers was Hispanic, and the other was a stepfather with one
biological child also. One African-American father attended who had two
children and who was cohabiting with his partner. A white police officer
also attended, as well as a white business professor who was active in
his Jewish temple.
Measures in italics as follows:
Using an A, B, C, D, and F grading scale like they do in school,
what grade would you give yourself for the job you are doing in each of
the following areas:
a. Bringing up your children (Rear Children)
b. Providing your children's basic needs (food, clothing,
shelter, & health care) (Basic Needs)
c. Providing your children moral guidance (teaching them right from
wrong) (Moral Guide)
d. Spending time with your children doing things they like to do
(Time Together)
e. Telling your children that you love them (Verbalize Love)
f. Knowing where your children go and what they do with their
friends (Monitoring)
g. Setting roles and limits for your children's behavior
(Setting Limits)
The fathers reported that the questions on the IFI captured well
their ideas of fathering; there were very few suggestions for additional
items. They said the questions were straightforward and understandable.
We were encouraged by these comments.
One interesting finding that emerged from the focus group that
could impact validity was that some fathers rated their involvement in
certain areas in the context of other kinds of involvement. That is, for
instance, one father mentioned that although he wasn't able to
attend as many of the events his children participated in (e.g., school,
sports) as he wanted to, he figured he was doing the best that he
possibly could given the long hours he needed to work to provide for his
family, and thus, gave himself a 6 ("Excellent") for that
item. It appears that some fathers will rate themselves higher on
individual aspects of father involvement because of the limitations that
other forms of involvement--especially providing--place on them. This
helps to explain the pattern of consistency or the high response
"bias" in most fathers' responses. Perhaps an
introduction that explains that few fathers can be excellent in all
areas because of conflicts, etc., would allow fathers to rate each
involvement item independent of the other items and create more
variation in their patterns of response.
Construct Validity. Construct validity was investigated by
correlating each of the nine IFI factors with conceptually related items
included in other parts of the questionnaire. The results are summarized
in Table 2. For example, the scale "Discipline and
Responsibility" was significantly correlated with another item in
the questionnaire (but not included in the IFI), "What grade would
you give yourself for ... setting rules and limits for your
children's behavior?" (r = .61, p < .001). Similarly, the
association between the "Praise and Affection" scale and the
item "What grade would you give yourself for .... telling your
children you love them?" was significant (r = .73, p <.001).
Generally, the pattern of correlations for eight of the nine
factors, as well as their magnitudes, supports associations that would
be expected conceptually. The exception to this rule was the factor,
"School Encouragement," in which the expected pattern of
correlations was considerably weaker.
In addition, we conducted a series of independent-group t-tests
which compared the means of fathers in intact married families to
fathers who were separated or divorced on several of the IFI factors
that we hypothesized would be different. Specifically, we hypothesized
that non-resident fathers would not rate themselves as highly as fathers
residing with their children on spending time with their children, being
attentive to daily matters, establishing a good co-parenting
relationship with the mother, economic providing, reading to their
children, and disciplining and teaching their children. The hypothesized
differences were found for attentiveness (t = 2.02,p <.05, ES = .33),
providing (t = 3.49, p <.001, ES = .58), co-parenting (t = 8.92, p
<.001, ES = 1.50). Surprisingly, the hypothesized differences were
not found for disciplining and teaching (t = 1.71, n.s.), spending time
(t = 1.04, n.s.), and reading to children (t = 1.16, n.s.). We suspect
that fathers in non-intact marriages were responding to these items in
the context of their non-residential fatherhood and rated themselves as
doing their best given the difficult circumstances. Hence, the
differences are minimized.
DISCUSSION AND FUTURE DIRECTIONS
Our first efforts to advance the self-report measurement of father
involvement based on a broader, richer, and deeper conceptualization
seem to have met with some success. The IFI distinguishes nine distinct
dimensions of father involvement (i.e., providing, support of the
mother, disciplining and teaching responsibility, and encouraging
success in school, giving praise and affection, spending time together
and talking, being attentive to their children's daily lives,
reading to their children, and encouraging children to develop their
talents). The items that constitute these scales tap into cognitive,
affective, and ethical aspects of fathering and include both direct and
indirect involvement. Furthermore, all nine dimensions can be seen as
indicators of a single, global construct of father involvement. The
dimensions generally have good internal consistency reliability,
although providing and attentiveness could use some improvement (alphas
= .69). Validity analyses suggest that the dimensions operate in
theoretically expected ways. And the nine dimensions can be captured in
an instrument with 26 items, which is short enough for most survey
requirements.
This multidimensional operationalization of father involvement
should allow researchers to explore more subtle theoretical linkages
between aspects of father involvement and children's development
and well-being, men's adult development and well-being, and family
processes and well-being. We hope researchers will undertake such
investigations and improve our empirical knowledge in the field of
father involvement. We also see potential educational and clinical uses
for the IFI. In both settings, fathers may benefit from a personal
analysis of their strengths and weaknesses as a father from an
instrument that is designed to tap a wide range of cognitive, affective,
and behavioral dimensions central to men's conceptualizations of
what it means to be a father.
While we believe the IFI offers researchers a richer and more
refined measure of father involvement, we should also point out the
continuity of our measure with the three-fold conceptualization of
father involvement--engagement, accessibility, and
responsibility--suggested by Lamb, Pleck, Charnov, and Levine (1987) and
well used in the field for the last 15 years. Most of the IFI subscales
can be fit into one or more of those categories of involvement (with the
possible exception of mother support). The engagement dimension is well
covered by several IFI subscales, particularly discipline and teaching
responsibility, school engagement, time and talking together, praise and
affection, reading and homework support, and attentiveness. Discipline
and teaching responsibility, school engagement, providing, developing
talents and future concerns, and attentiveness tap into the
responsibility dimension. The accessibility dimension is at least
implied in a handful of scales, especially in school encouragement,
reading and homework support, and attentiveness dimensions.
While we are encouraged by our initial efforts with the IFI, much
remains to be done. Further development plans include at least the
following four things. First, some further refinement of the items and
the ways questions are asked may help to increase the variation in the
measure. In addition, we note there are only three to four items
directly tapping into cognitive and affective aspects of father
involvement (e.g., planning for the future, monitoring media, telling
children you love them). These important dimensions of father
involvement are not as prominent in the IFI as they could be. More could
be done to include items that tap these ways of being involved. Second,
sampling bias in our pilot work suggests the need to further test the
effectiveness of the IFI on a truly representative sample of fathers. A
third and related area for investigation is the ability of the IFI to
compare effectively across such divergent groups as unmarried fathers,
non-custodial fathers, stepfathers, and fathers in intact marriages. Our
intention in the wording of most IFI items was to maximize the potential
for cross-group comparisons. An equivalence-of-measures study is needed
for the IFI across these diverse groups of fathers. Fourth,
fathers' self-reports of their involvement with their children
should be compared to reports from their children and their
spouses/partners. Each perspective is likely to yield important and
somewhat different information that more fully captures (or
triangulates) the concept of father involvement. In addition, such
triangulation will yield important validity checks for the fathers'
self-report measure and likely attenuate associations between father
involvement and child outcomes. Fourth, the validity of the IFI needs
further testing against a broader set of outcomes for children, fathers,
and families.
Hence, there is still much work to do on this and other measures.
But a broader and richer measure of father involvement seems plausible.
And more refined measurement work will ultimately yield a more
fine-grained understanding of the important social-psychological and
developmental linkages between fathers and children.
Table 1
Inventory of Father Involvement (IFI) Dimensions, Items, and Cronbach
Alphas
Instructions: Think of your experience as a father over the past 12
months. Please rate how good a job you think you did as a father on
each of the items listed below. If an item is not applicable to your
situation, circle "NA" for not applicable.
(Response choices were 0 through 6, with 0 anchored by "Very Poor" and 6
anchored by "Excellent." "NA" was also a response choice.)
Note: The first three items for each dimension were used in the shorter,
26-item version of the IFI. Any additional items, listed below the line,
were included in the longer, 35-item version of the IFI.
Discipline and Teaching Responsibility (Cronbach's Alpha = .85 (a))
* Disciplining your children.
* Encouraging your children to do their chores.
* Setting rules and limits for your children's behavior.
* Teaching your children to be responsible for what they do.
* Paying attention to what your children read, the music they listen
to, or TV shows they watch.
* Enforcing family rules.
School Encouragement (Cronbach's Alpha = .82)
* Encouraging your children to succeed in school.
* Encouraging your children to do their homework.
* Teaching your children to follow rules at school.
Mother Support (Cronbach's Alpha = .87)
* Giving your children's mother encouragement and emotional support.
* Letting your children know that their mother is an important and
special person.
* Cooperating with your children's mother in the rearing of your
children.
Providing (Cronbach's Alpha = .69)
* Providing your children's basic needs (food, clothing, shelter, and
health care).
* Accepting responsibility for the financial support of the children
you have fathered.
Time and Talking Together (Cronbach's Alpha = .80 (b))
* Being a pal or a friend to your children.
* Spending time just talking with your children when they want to
talk about something.
* Spending time with your children doing things they like to do.
* Working with your children on chores around the house.
* Helping your children find purpose and direction in their lives.
* Taking your children to interesting places (your work, parks,
museums, ocean, etc.)
* Talking to your children about what's going on in their lives.
* Listening to your children's views or concerns.
Praise and Affection (Cronbach's Alpha = .79 (c))
* Praising your children for being good or doing the right thing.
* Praising your children for something they have done well.
* Telling your children that you love them.
* Showing physical affection to your children (touching, hugging,
kissing).
Developing Talents and Future Concerns (Cronbach's Alpha = .75)
* Encouraging your children to develop their talents.
* Encouraging your children to continue their schooling beyond high
school.
* Planning for your children's future (education, training).
Reading and Homework Support (Cronbach's Alpha = .83)
* Encouraging your children to read.
* Reading to your younger children.
* Helping your older children with their homework.
Attentiveness (Cronbach's Alpha = .69)
* Attending events your children participate in (sports, school,
church events).
* Being involved in the daily or regular routine of taking care
of your children's basic needs or activities (feeding, driving them
places, etc.).
* Knowing where your children go and what they do with their friends.
Notes:
(a.) Cronbach's alpha for the longer version of the "Discipline and
Teaching Responsibility" scale was .88).
(b.) Cronbach's alpha for the longer version of the "Time and Talking
Together" scale was .90).
(c.) Cronbach's alpha for the longer version of the "Praise and
Affection" scale was .83). Additional items in the pilot IFI not used:
Being a good example to your children. Providing your children moral
guidance (teaching them right from wrong). Teaching your children to
work. Building or fixing things for your children. Doing things or
fixing things to keep your children safe from physical harm or accident.
Praying for your children. Teaching your children how to fix things or
do things around the house.
Table 2
IFI Construct Validity Analysis: Zero-Order Correlations of IFI
Factors with Conceptually Related Independent Measures
F1: Discipline, Responsibility
Rear Children .38 ***
Setting Limits .61 ***
Moral Guide .48 ***
Monitoring .43 ***
Religious Attend. .16 ***
Child Problems -.10 **
F2: School Encouragement
Rear Children .09
Setting Limits .49 ***
Education -.08
Child Problems -.15 **
F3: Mother Support
I .46 ***
F4: Providing
Rear Children .15 ***
Basic Needs .57 ***
F5: Time & Talking
Rear Children .50 ***
Time Together .64 ***
Work Hours -.10 **
F6: Praise & Affection
Rear Children .46 ***
Verbalize Love .73 ***
Religious Attend. .09 *
Child Problems -.18 ***
F7: Developing Talents
Rear Children .35 ***
Child Problems -.10 **
F8: Reading/Homework Support
Rear Children .36 ***
Education .11 *
Child Problems -.13 **
F9: Attentiveness
Rear Children .46 ***
Monitoring .52 ***
Time Together .47 ***
* p<.05
** p<.01
*** p<.001
REFERENCES
Daly, K. J. (2001). Deconstructing family time: From ideology to
lived experience. Journal of Marriage and Family, 63, 283-294.
Doherty, W. J., Kouneski, E. F., & Erickson, M. F. (1998).
Responsible fathering: An overview and conceptual framework. Journal of
Marriage and the Family, 60, 277-292.
Dollahite, D. C., & Hawkins, A. J. (1998). A conceptual ethic of generative fathering. The Journal of Men's Studies, 7(1),
109-132.
Hawkins, A. J., & Palkovitz, R. (1999). Beyond ticks and
clicks: The need for more diverse and broader conceptualizations and
measures of father involvement. The Journal of Men's Studies, 8,
11-32.
Hochschild, A. R. (1997). The time bind. New York: Metropolitan
Books.
Lamb, M. E. (1999). The history of research on father involvement:
An overview. In E. Peters & R. D. Day (Eds.), Fatherhood: Research,
interventions, and policies (pp. 23-41). Binghamton, NY: Haworth.
Lamb, M. E. (1986). The changing role of fathers. In M. E. Lamb
(Ed.), The father's role: An applied perspective (pp. 3-27). New
York: John Wiley.
Lamb, M. E., Pleck, J. H., Charnov, E. L., & Levine, J. A.
(1987). A biosocial perspective on paternal behavior and involvement. In
J. B. Lancaster, J. Altmann, A. S. Rossi, L. R. Sherrod (Eds.),
Parenting across the lifespan: Biosocial perspectives (pp. 111-142).
Hawthorne, NY: Aldine.
Marsiglio, W., Amato, P., Day, R. D., & Lamb, M. E. (2000).
Scholarship on fatherhood in the 1990s and beyond. Journal of Marriage
and the Family, 62, 1173-1191.
Newport, F. (1997, March). Americans' relationships with their
children: Much remains the same. The Gallup Poll Monthly, 13-17.
Palkovitz, R. (1997). Reconstructing "involvement":
Expanding conceptualizations of men's caring in contemporary
families. In A. J. Hawkins & D. C. Dollahite (Eds.), Generative
fathering: Beyond deficit perspectives (pp. 200-216). Thousand Oaks, CA:
Sage.
Pleck, J. H. (1997). Paternal involvement: Levels, sources, and
consequences. In M. E. Lamb (Ed.), The role of the father in child
development (3rd ed., pp. 66-103). New York: John Wiley & Sons.
Sabetelli, R. M. (1988). Measurement issues in marital research: A
review and critique of contemporary survey instruments. Journal of
Marriage and the Family, 50, 891-915.
Snarey, J. (1993). How fathers care for the next generation: A
four-decade study. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Alan
J. Hawkins, Brigham Young University, 380-A Kimball Tower, Provo, Utah
84602. Electronic mail may be sent to hawkinsa@byu.edu.
Alan J. Hawkins and Kay P. Bradford, School of Family Life, Brigham
Young University, Provo, Utah; Rob Palkovitz, Department of Individual
and Family Studies, University of Delaware, Newark, Delaware; Shawn L.
Christiansen, Department of Family and Consumer Studies, Central
Washington University, Ellenshurg, Washington; Randal D. Day, School of
Family Life, Brigham Young University, Provo, Utah; Vaughn R. A. Call,
Department of Sociology, Brigham Young University, Provo, Utah.
Earlier versions of this paper were presented at the National
Council on Family Relations Annual Conference, November 12, 1999,
Irvine, California, and the Workshop on Measuring Father Involvement,
sponsored by Brigham Young University, National Center on Fathers and
Families, and the NICHD Child and Family Well-being Network, at the
National Institutes of Health, Bethesda, Maryland, February 8, 2001.