The (Un)Emotional Male: Physiological, Verbal, and Written Correlates of Expressiveness.
ROBERTSON, JOHN M. ; WOODFORD, JOYCE ; LIN, CHI-WEI 等
In North American culture, it frequently is asserted that men find
it difficult to talk about their emotions. For more than 25 years, this
topic has been the subject of both theoretical exploration (David &
Brannon, 1976; Friedman & Lerner, 1986; Goldberg, 1976; Pleck, 1981)
and social science research. Most scholarly observers in this area have
described men as having considerable difficulty in verbally expressing
their emotions to others (e.g., Brooks & Gilbert, 1995; Sher, 1993;
Wilcox & Forrest, 1992). Based on this theoretical work, clinicians
have been encouraged to deemphasize the focus on talking about feelings
in therapy and to offer men more opportunities to be active. Examples of
such activities include workshops, consultations, self-help reading,
classes, or other behavioral approaches (Bernstein, 1995; Hurst, 1997;
Kelly & Hall, 1992; Miller, 1995; Robertson, 1989; Shay, 1996;
Wanigaratne & Barker, 1995).
These studies seem to suggest that most men are hypoemotional.
Repeatedly, it is asserted that most men have great difficulty in
identifying their own emotions, in sending emotional messages, or in
accurately interpreting the emotions of others. The term alexithymia has
been used to describe this inability to adequately express affect
(Levant, 1992). Studies that emphasize the hypoemotionality of men
typically compare men with women and then report significant gender
differences. To illustrate, men have been reported to have lower levels
of happiness, life satisfaction, depression, fear, and sadness than
women (Nolen-Hoeksema, 1987; Scherer, Wallbott, & Summerfield, 1986;
Wood, Rhodes, & Whelan, 1989).
However, more recent work has questioned some of the empirical
basis for this perspective. For example, Heesacker and his colleagues
have suggested that it may be mostly a stereotype to suggest that men
are hypoemotional (Heesacker, Wester, Vogel, Wentzel, Mejia-Millan,
& Goodholm, 1999). Although the view that men are hypoemotional (and
that women are hyperemotional) has a long history (cf., Parsons &
Bales, 1955), Heesacker cited several studies that show very little
affective difference between men and women. Specifically, men and women
have shown no significant differences in their abilities to send or
receive emotional messages from others (Conner & Heesacker, 1999),
in their expressions of anger (Burrowes & Halberstadt, 1987), in
their private experiences of emotion (LaFrance & Banaji, 1992), or
in the facial expressions and visual behavior that they use to respond
to emotional questions (Cherulink, 1979).
Other studies have found similar results. Eagly and Steffen (1986)
found that although typical women are judged to be more emotionally
expressive than typical men, when subjects were told that men and women
were in identical roles, subjects reported no differences in their
expectations of men and women. Similarly, in a study that required men
to report on their own emotions (fear, joy, sadness, and love), results
indicated that when men report less intense emotional experiences, they
are more likely to hold stereotypical views about emotional differences
between men and women (Grossman & Wood, 1993). The Heesacker review
concluded that "research on emotion fails to support the perception
of vast affective differences between men and women" (Heesacker et
al., 1999, p. 483,484). This conclusion echoes the findings of others
who have reviewed the literature (Canary & Emmers-Sommers, 1997;
LaFrance & Banaji, 1992).
Although these two strands of literature may seem somewhat
incompatible, it also can be argued that they are complementary.
Granted, many North American men may have difficulty expressing their
emotions verbally; this has been noted for several decades. However, it
may also be true that men do not necessarily have fewer emotions or have
any less capability than women of expressing them. It may be that men
are less verbally expressive of their emotions because they have been
socialized to say less, or because they have a limited capacity to
express themselves, not because they have little emotional arousal in
their lives.
To explore this possibility, it would be useful to look for
within-group differences among men, rather than to compare, men with
women. Two types of questions can be asked. First, do all men experience
significant emotional arousal to the events in their lives? Are there
significant differences in levels of arousal among men? If so, can this
be shown empirically? Second, are some men more verbally expressive of
their emotional arousal than other men? If so, what makes the
difference? Do the themes of traditional masculine socialization help us
understand any within-group differences on the emotional expressiveness
of men?
In considering these questions, two clusters of studies seem
helpful to review. The first group suggests that high levels of
emotional expressiveness in men are unlikely because of the internal
strain it creates. Pleck and his associates (1993) developed a set of
statements illustrating the "masculine ideology" of North
American culture. Included were items involving respect from others,
self-confidence, physical toughness, reluctance to talk about problems,
avoidance of feminine-like behavior, low interest in housework, and
readiness for sexual activity. Other attempts to describe central
masculine socialization themes have included such constructs as
achievement/success/status; toughness/aggressiveness; dominance/control;
analysis/rationality; and avoidance of
emotionality/femininity/homosexuality (David & Brannon, 1976;
O'Neil, 1981). Researchers have investigated the conflicts created
for men by these expectations, using such models as "gender-role
conflict" (O'Neil, Helms, Gable, David, & Wrightsman,
1986), "sex-role strain" (Pleck, 1981), and "gender-role
stress" (Eisler & Skidmore, 1987). A basic idea in this body of
work is that men have been socialized in ways that make the verbal
expression of emotions unlikely because of the "conflict,"
"strain," or "stress" that such expressions might
evoke.
The second group of relevant studies suggests that men are less
emotionally expressive as adults because they have fewer opportunities
to develop those skills. Cross-cultural studies have shown that the
child-care role (requiring high levels of emotional investment and
expressiveness) are much less frequently held by men than by women;
Crano and Aronoff (1978) looked at 186 societies and found that young
boys through the age of five received much less emotional support from
fathers than from mothers. Similarly, personal and emotional care of the
frail elderly is less frequently a responsibility taken by men (Dwyer
& Seecomb, 1991); men are expected to provide less emotionally
involving services, such as household maintenance and repairs or
transportation to appointments. Given the differentiation of these roles
by sex, men simply have fewer opportunities to develop and use skills of
emotional expressiveness.
Based on these two ideas (that traditional masculine ideology makes
the verbal expression of emotions stressful, and that men simply have
few opportunities to develop the skills of emotional expression), it is
plausible to suggest that the problem may not be the inherent
hypoemotionality of men; it may rather be that men have been socialized
not to verbalize their feelings.
To empirically test this theory, two suppositions must be examined:
that virtually all men are significantly physiologically aroused by
emotion-inducing events; and that men vary in how they wish to express
this emotional arousal, based on (perhaps) masculine idealogy. To date,
these links have not appeared in the empirical literature. However,
recent studies have examined physiological data in ways that begin to
explore this possibility. In brief, this body of research hints that
many men actually may be experiencing higher levels of emotional arousal
than they are reporting verbally. For example, Grossman and Wood (1993)
found that when men and women were not given normative instructions
about responding to emotion-inducing slides, men verbally reported less
emotional intensity than women (a finding consistent with socialized
expectations), but they also found that when the instructions normalized
emotional responsiveness, no sex differences were found in physiological
responses to the slides. Further, there is evidence that men are highly
reactive physiologically to stressful events. Gottman and Levenson
(1988) reviewed a series of studies showing that men do indeed
experience physiological arousal when presented with emotion-inducing
events (i.e., changes in blood pressure, basal skin conductance, heart
rate, release of hormones, and corticosteroid excretion).
The purpose of this study was to explore any associations between
two variables of the male experience: what men actually experience
physiologically when emotionally aroused, and how they report those
experiences. To accomplish this, we presented men with three
emotion-inducing events and then compared what they reported about the
experience with measures of their actual physiological responses. We
also were curious about the role of masculine gender-role issues in this
equation. Based on the literature (Bernstein, 1995; Hurst, 1997; Kelly
& Hall, 1992; Miller, 1995; Robertson, 1989; Shay, 1996; Wanigaratne
& Barker, 1995; Wilcox & Forrest, 1992), we hypothesized that
men with more traditional gender-role expectations would not have lower
levels of emotional arousal, but instead would be more reluctant to
express verbally their internal emotional arousal. At the same time we
expected that a more structured exercise would elicit more
expressiveness from these men than would invitations to simply speak
about their experiences.
Because we were interested primarily in within-group differences,
no comparison groups of women were used. That is, we were interested in
examining the stereotype that most men are hypoemotional--that they do
not experience significant levels of emotional responsiveness or that
they do not express their emotions effectively. Further, we were
interested in whether or not masculine socialization might be a variable
that would help explain any within-group differences among men with
regard to emotional expressiveness.
The importance of examining this question is illustrated by the
variety of intrapsychic, interpersonal, and physical health problems men
experience (Cleary, 1987; Courtenay, 2000; Meinecke, 1981). In
particular, emotional inexpressiveness has been associated with marital
problems, alcohol abuse, workplace stress, reluctance to seek
counseling, poor health habits, the use of violence, Type A behavior,
loneliness, low use of social support networks, and sexual difficulties
for men (Eisler & Blalock, 1991; Gordon & Meth, 1990; Pasick,
Gordon, & Meth, 1990). If we can begin to show that the problem is
not a lack of emotional arousal or responsiveness among men, but rather
a lack of opportunity or a lack of social support for expressing
themselves, then these latter factors might be directly addressed.
One final comment concerns the problem of defining basic emotions.
Although a review of this definitional problem is beyond the scope of
this study (e.g., Ekman & Davidson, 1994), it is appropriate to
describe briefly the components of an emotion as conceptualized for the
present work: (a) an emotion requires an antecedent event (intrapsychic,
interpersonal, or environmental) that stimulates physiological arousal;
(b) the arousal functions to focus attention on life tasks that need to
be addressed and provides motivation to make appropriate adaptive
responses (Davidson, 1994; Frijda, 1994; Gray, 1994; Levenson, 1994;
Robertson & Freeman, 1995). Put in real life terms, when a bear
suddenly appears on a mountain pathway in front of a man, physiological
reactions occur (changes in heart rate, skin temperature, etc.). The
purpose of this arousal is to focus attention on the problem and
energize appropriate adaptive responses. The emotion includes the
arousal, the appraisal, and the motivation to make an adaptive response.
For this particular illustration, the emotion might be named fear or
panic. Using this definition, most emotions can be named with a single
descriptive word, and these emotion words can be rated with regard to
intensity (e.g., Averill, 1975).
METHOD
RESEARCH PARTICIPANTS
Participants were recruited from several male-dominated occupations
and were all employed near a major university in the Midwest. Two themes
consistent with traditional masculine socialization formed the selection
criteria: the men were directly responsible for the physical or
emotional well-being of a fairly large number of people; and they were
required to respond immediately and act as problem-solvers to persons in
acute distress. Men theorized to have met these criteria included police
officers, sheriff deputies, jailers, fire fighters, members of the
clergy, and men with administrative responsibilities that included
significant safety concerns. Generally, they were recruited through the
organizations in which they were employed (e.g., announcements in police
departments, fire stations, and so forth). For participating in a study
of "Stress Management Factors," the men were offered free
admission to a four-hour workshop on the management of stressful
emotions at work.
The 69 participants had a mean age of 35.71 (SD =11.81). The
response rate (the proportion of actual participants to those invited to
participate) was relatively high (just over 80%); superior officers and
supervisors strongly encouraged the men to participate. The participants
had completed an average of 4.25 years of education beyond high school.
They identified themselves ethnically by reporting that their ancestors
came mostly from Europe (85%), Africa (4%), or Hispanic countries (4%).
The rest (6%) reported mixed ancestry or left the question blank. They
reported their current relationship status as married (49%), single with
no steady relationship (32%), divorced (10%), and steady but unmarried
relationship (9%).
Because traditional counseling invites men to be emotionally
expressive, the men were asked about their preferences or experiences
with counseling. Most had never talked with a professional helper for
personal concerns (56%), and only a fifth (21%) of the entire sample
indicated that they would be more likely in the future to seek help from
a professional than from family members, relatives, or friends. Yet, 43%
had sought help from a professional counselor or a member of the clergy,
and had attended an average of 5.93 sessions. This latter percentage is
higher than we expected, given the usual counseling utilization rates of
men. Perhaps the higher levels of risk and demand in these occupations
made them more likely to seek help.
INSTRUMENTS
Instruments were selected to measure the following variables: (a)
physiological arousal during an emotion-inducing event; (b) verbal and
structured emotional expressiveness immediately following the
emotion-inducing event; and (c) the presence of gender-role stress.
To measure psychophysiological responses to emotion-inducing
events, participants were attached to the Focused Technology F1000
Instrumentation System (BioMedical Instruments, 1994). A battery of
responses was collected, including skin temperature and electrodermal
response (EDR) data at 7.2 samples per second, electromyographic (EMG)
data at 10 samples per second, and heart rate with electrodes on the
upper body. Responses were measured during three emotion-inducing
events, involving visual/auditory experiencing (a nine-minute
videotape), cognitive functioning (a one-minute mental arithmetic task),
and sensory functioning (a one-minute cold pressor test).
Videotape Segment. Numerous studies have successfully induced
emotional responses with the use of film/videotape stories. Reviews of
these studies (Gerrards-Hesse, Spies, & Hesse, 1994; Martin, 1990)
have noted that viewing videotaped scenes does indeed induce
physiological arousal. For example, segments of the motion picture
"The Champ" have been used to elicit sadness (Marston, Hart,
Hileman, & Faunce, 1984), and the movie "Run" has been
used to induce anxiety (Isen & Gorgolione, 1983).
For the present study, segments of the movie "My Life"
(Netter & Rubin, 1993) were selected because the story deals with
themes many theorists have indicated are difficult for traditionally
socialized men to express (father-son relationships, sadness, and
death). The father-adult son relationship in the story is a subplot of
the larger story about the terminally ill adult son who wanted to
preserve his life story on videotape for his own newly born son. The
father-adult son interactions in the movie were edited together to form
a video that lasted nine minutes and 40 seconds.
Immediately following presentation of the videotape segment,
participants were asked "What words would you use to describe your
reaction to the video?" Answers were recorded on audiotape and then
transcribed. Graduate students were employed to count the number of
emotion words used by participants and to indicate the emotionality
rating for each word using the model developed by Averill (1975).
Previous studies have used the words of research participants to
evaluate the presence of emotions (e.g., Greenwald, Cook, & Lang,
1989; Lang, 1984, 1985), drawn conclusions based on the counting of
English words (Breland, 1996; Thorndike, 1921), and used response time
as a measure of emotionality (Fitzgibbons & Simons, 1992; Hess,
Kappas, McHugo, Lanzetta, & Kleck, 1992; Lang, Greenwald, Bradley,
& Hamm, 1993).
Mental Arithmetic Task. Earlier studies have shown that mental
arithmetic procedures can increase activity in muscle tension, skin
temperature, heart rate, electrodermal response, and systolic blood
pressure (e.g., Anderson, 1989; Arena, 1984). After reviewing the
protocols of several investigators (Carlson, Collins, Stewart,
Porzelius, Nitz, & Lind, 1989; Donat & McCullough, 1983;
Subotnik & Shapiro, 1984), a one-minute arithmetic task was devised
for the present study: participants were given a three digit number on a
4x6 card and asked to count aloud backwards by increments of 13. They
were instructed to work as rapidly as possible while the investigator
wrote down their responses.
Cold Pressor Test. Psychophysiological sensitivity has been found
with the use of the cold pressor procedure (e.g., Reeves & Shapiro,
1983; Subotnik & Shapiro, 1984). Participants in the present study
were asked to immerse one hand in a three-gallon tank filled with
crushed ice and water for as long as they could withstand the pain, up
to a maximum of 60 seconds. Water was kept constant at a temperature of
less than .5 degrees Centigrade. Participants were informed that they
could remove their hand at any time the sensations became too painful.
Emotional Assessment Scale (EAS). After each administration of the
three emotion-inducing events, participants were given a structured
pencil and paper assessment that was generally completed in less than
one minute. The EAS (Carlson et al., 1989) consists of 24 words that
describe eight emotional states (anger, anxiety, disgust, fear, guilt,
happiness, sadness, and surprise). The eight selected emotions were
based on the work of Izard and others (Ekman, 1972; Izard, 1977, 1991).
Three descriptors for each emotion were chosen using the work of
Nunnally (1981), who developed a factor-analytic list of words most
often used for each emotion category.
To complete the exercise, participants used a visual analogue
scale. Participants were asked to measure the intensity of each
emotional descriptor by drawing a slash on a 100-millimeter line. The
line ranged from points labeled "Least Possible" (zero) to
"Most Possible" (100 millimeters). The instrument has been
used to measure momentary and immediate changes in emotional states. The
EAS was scored by measuring the number of millimeters from the left
endpoint up to the slash mark. Scores for each emotion, therefore,
ranged from 0-100.
Carlson et al. (1989) reported that reliability was measured using
both inter-item (.70 to .91) and split-half (.94) procedures. Because
the EAS is designed to measure an emotional state at a particular point
in time, computing reliability over several administrations was not
appropriate. Validity was examined by comparing several of the subscales
with existing measures of emotions, such as the Profile of Mood States (McNair, Lorr, & Doppleman, 1981), the Beck Depression Inventory (Beck, 1972), and the State form of the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory
(Speilberger, Gorsuch, & Luschene, 1970). Results were supportive,
with positive correlations found in the expected directions for such
subscales as sadness (.74), anxiety (.78), anger (.69), and happiness
(-.36).
Semantic Atlas of Emotional Concepts. Intended to be a complete
list of emotional terms in the English language, this instrument was
used to measure both the frequency and the intensity of the emotion
words used by men in our study. Averill and his colleagues (1975) first
reviewed 18,000 psychological terms compiled by Allport and Odbert
(1936), as well as three other emotion word lists. If two of five judges
agreed that a term had emotional connotations, Averill kept the term on
the list. A total of 717 terms were thus retained. Further work using
university undergraduates as subjects eliminated non-emotional terms and
reduced the list to 558 words.
For each word, an emotionality rating was developed. Several
factors were combined (activation/energy, evaluation/pleasantness,
importance/depth of experience, and degree of control). Estimates of
reliability for these ratings were obtained by randomly splitting the
subject pool into halves. Correlations between the split-sample scores
were ranged from .87 to .98 for the four factors. An overall emotional
rating was calculated, based on a scale of -3 (less emotionality) to +3
(more emotionality). To illustrate, the term "furious" had an
emotionality rating of +2.25, and the term "listless" had a
rating of -1.65.
For the present study, a word used by a research participant was
called an emotion if it appeared in Averill's atlas, and the
word's intensity was recorded using Averill's emotionality
rating.
Masculine Gender-Role Stress (MGRS). Developed as a measure of male
gender-role stress (Eisler & Skidmore, 1987), the MGRS consists of
40 items factor analyzed into five subscales: Physical Inadequacy (not
being competitive in sports or sexual rivalries); Emotional
Inexpressiveness (difficulties in expressing feelings or responding to
emotions in others); Subordination to Women (in athletic or sports
settings); Intellectual Inferiority (indecisiveness, inability to handle
situations); and Performance Behavior (work and sexual adequacy). The
underlying theory is that men will experience stress if they believe
they are not behaving as men should behave or if a situation forces them
to act in ways others define as feminine.
Higher scores reflect comparatively higher levels of gender-role
stress and have been correlated positively with stress in expressing
emotions, higher levels of anxiety and anger, poorer health habits, and
higher systolic blood pressure changes in a cold pressor test (Eisler
& Blalock, 1991; Eisler, Skidmore & Ward, 1988). The scale has
shown high internal consistency (alpha coefficients in .90s), and
test-retest reliability over two weeks was .93. Construct validity has
been supported by comparing the MGRS with other traits theoretically
linked with masculine stress, such as inexpressiveness, anger, state
anxiety, stress reactions, and adverse health habits (Eisler et al.,
1988; Saurer & Eisler, 1990).
PROTOCOL
Participants were scheduled for individual 90-minute sessions, with
the entire data set being collected during a four-week period to
minimize the potential confound of contemporary events. They came to a
university health center where the project was explained. The consent
form was signed, and the men were instructed to thoroughly wash their
arms up to the elbow. Sensors were attached to the forefinger of the
nondominant hand, the palm of the hand, the forearms, and clavicles.
Participants were then seated in a recliner chair, with a visual screen
shielding the equipment, a video monitor, and an audiotape recorder.
They were connected to the F1000 system and directed through the
following sequence of events: a baseline rest period (10 minutes);
viewing the videotape stimulus (9 minutes); rest and recovery (five
minutes); performing the arithmetic task (counting backwards by 13s for
one minute); rest and recovery (five minutes); completing the cold
pressor task (placing hand in ice water for up to one minute); final
rest and recovery (sitting quietly and relaxing for five minutes). They
were then ushered into a separate room and given the MGRS. Finally, they
were debriefed by a licensed psychologist who inquired about any
discomfort the procedure might have induced.
RESULTS
PHYSIOLOGICAL AROUSAL
Table 1 shows the means and standard deviations of the
physiological measures for each event in the protocol. It is clear that
each of the three stressors (videotape, math test, and cold pressor)
induced measurable physiological responses. Skin temperature was
reduced, and significant changes were measured for EDR, EMG, and heart
rate. The widest range of responses occurred on the EMG, with more
moderate changes occurring with skin temperature, EDR, and heart rate.
Table 1
Physiological Measures of Responsiveness to Emotion-Inducing Stimuli
Temperature EDR (sweat)
Event M SD M SD
Baseline Rest 92.8 3.9 4.6 4.8
Video Stimulus 91.1 4.4(**) 6.2 5.2(**)
Verbal Response 90.1 4.7(**) 8.8 7.1(**)
EAS--Video 89.9 4.7(**) 8.3 6.2(**)
Rest Period 90.8 4.8(**) 5.8 5.2(**)
Math Stimulus 89.7 4.6(**) 9.1 6.9(**)
EAS--Math 89.0 5.6(**) 8.5 6.1(**)
Rest Period 90.4 5.1(**) 5.9 5.1(**)
Cold Pressor 89.3 5.0(**) 6.9 5.7(**)
EAS--Cold Pressor 88.4 5.0(**) 6.3 5.3(**)
Rest Period 89.2 5.8(**) 5.5 4.9(**)
EMG (muscle) Heart Rate
Event M SD M SD
Baseline Rest 8.0 4.1 69.2 9.9
Video Stimulus 16.8 8.3(**) 68.3 9.2
Verbal Response 20.2 10.3(**) 78.0 12.0(**)
EAS--Video 30.2 8.3(**) 73.2 10.3(**)
Rest Period 9.8 6.2(*) 67.4 12.1
Math Stimulus 15.4 6.3(**) 78.5 12.8(**)
EAS--Math 30.1 9.3(**) 73.5 11.0(**)
Rest Period 8.2 3.9 68.5 10.2
Cold Pressor 30.4 11.2(**) 74.0 9.6(**)
EAS--Cold Pressor 29.8 9.3(**) 70.1 10.1
Rest Period 8.7 5.9 66.0 10.4(**)
(**) p < .01
(*) p < .05
Note: Temperature is reported in degrees Fahrenheit. EDR
(electrodermal response) is reported in micro ohms. EDR numbers
express units of change in conductance, relative to an EDR
reference value; higher numbers reflect greater arousal. EMG
(electromyographic) units are micro volts per event; higher
numbers generally reflect greater arousal. Heart rate is
reported in beats per minute. EAS refers to the Emotional
Assessment Scale (Carlson, et al., 1989). Each data point
is compared to the baseline rest event.
It should also be noted from Table 1 that the simple request to
report on their responses to the stimuli also elicited measurable
arousal. This held true for both forms of expression (i.e., for both the
open-ended verbal responses and the structured exercise [EAS]). Every
time the men were asked to describe their reactions to one of the
stimuli, their bodies responded with measurable arousal.
These results were consistent throughout the sample. Men who scored
high on the MGRS (indicating higher levels of gender-role stress), for
example, displayed physiological arousal patterns virtually
indistinguishable from men who scored lower on the MGRS. A median split
of MGRS scores divided the men into two groups (high gender-role stress
and low gender-role stress). There was no significant difference between
these two groups on any of the physiological measures for any event in
the entire protocol, with p-values ranging from .371 to .830.
A primary finding, then, was that all men responded physiologically
to the emotion-inducing stimuli, regardless of their scores on the
gender-role stress measure. Further, it should be noted that there were
no significant differences within the sample by occupation.
VERBAL AND STRUCTURED EXPRESSIVENESS
Tables 2 and 3 report results regarding expressiveness. To measure
verbal responsiveness, participants were asked to talk about the
stimulus they had just experienced (e.g., "What words would you use
to describe your reactions to the video?"). Audiotaped responses
were analyzed with regard to response time, total number of words used,
number of emotion words used, and an emotionality rating of the emotion
words. In addition, after the men had completed their responses to all
three stressors, the participants were asked how they generally handled
stress in their lives. Again, audiotaped responses were analyzed,
calculating both the response time and the number of words used in
responding to the question. These six elements are reported in Table 2.
Table 2
Verbal Measures of Responsiveness to Emotion-Inducing Stimuli
Measure M SD
Response time in reacting to video stimulus(a) 6.0 4.5
Number of words used in reacting to video 51.2 51.7
Number of emotion words used in responding to video(b) 3.1 2.7
Emotionality rating of emotion words used for video(c) 2.3 2.5
Response time in reporting stressful reactions(a) 4.3 2.5
Number of words used in describing stressful reactions 109.8 86.1
Note: (a) Response time was the number of seconds between the
interviewer's question and the participant's answer; faster
responses times were interpreted as more expressive. (b) Words
were accepted as emotion words if they appeared in the atlas of
English emotion words compiled by Averill (1975) (c) The Averill
(1975) atlas developed an emotionality rating based on a
seven-point scale from -3 to +3, with higher numbers indicating
stronger emotionality.
Table 3
Written Measures of Responsiveness to Emotion-Inducing Stimuli
Videotape Math Task Cold Pressor
Emotion M SD M SD M SD
Anger 25.3 25.8 19.6 24.7 12.5 17.9
Anxiety 26.0 24.5 39.5 29.3 22.9 21.2
Disgust 17.8 20.6 15.6 21.6 8.4 13.1
Fear 19.0 20.7 20.0 22.0 10.4 14.9
Guilt 22.7 23.6 24.3 24.9 9.8 12.9
Happiness 20.6 23.9 11.2 13.8 8.4 11.4
Sadness 33.1 19.0 17.0 21.6 6.9 9.3
Surprise 17.6 19.9 28.3 27.3 29.4 25.4
Stimulus Totals 22.8 22.3 21.9 23.2 13.6 15.8
Average Score
by Emotion
Emotion M SD
Anger 19.1 22.8
Anxiety 29.5 25.0
Disgust 13.9 18.4
Fear 16.5 19.2
Guilt 18.9 20.5
Happiness 13.4 16.4
Sadness 19.0 16.6
Surprise 25.1 24.2
Stimulus Totals 19.4 20.4
Note: Participants reported the intensity of each emotion by drawing
a slash on a 100-millimeter line (range = 1 to 100). Higher numbers
indicate greater self-reported intensity for each emotion (Carlson et
al., 1989).
The men tended to use rather few emotion words when reporting their
reactions to the video. However, the emotion words that they did use
were quite intense (M = +2.28, on a seven-point scale from -3 to +3,
with higher numbers representing more intense emotionality). Variability
in verbosity was wide, as the average number of total words used
(emotion words plus all other words) was 51.16, with a standard
deviation of 51.65. Individual differences in overall wordiness were
quite prominent. The variability on the use of emotion words was much
smaller.
Results on the structured measure of expressiveness (EAS) indicated
that men were quite willing to report emotional responses with the use
of pencil and paper. Table 3 indicates that all eight of the EAS
emotions were reported as present at some point during the protocol.
Within a range of 1 to 100, the videotape (M = 22.8, SD = 22.3) and the
math test (M = 21.9, SD = 23.2) evoked more intense overall ratings than
the cold pressor (M = 13.6, SD = 15.7). With regard to individual
emotions, the highest rankings across the three stressors were given to
anxiety (M = 29.5, SD = 25.0) and surprise (M = 25.1, SD = 24.2), and
the lowest ratings were given to disgust (M = 13.9, SD = 18.4) and
happiness (M = 13.4, SD = 16.4). These findings are not surprising,
given that none of the emotion-inducing events was designed to induced
happiness or disgust. The high standard deviations for all these ratings
again suggested wide individual variation.
For the videotape stimulus, the most intense ratings were for
sadness (M = 33.1, SD = 19.0), anxiety (M = 26.0, SD = -24.5), and anger
(M = 25.3, SD = 25.8). The math test elicited reports of anxiety (M =
39.5, SD = 29.3) and surprise (M = 28.3, SD = 27.3). The cold pressor
generated the most intense responses on the same two dimensions as the
math test, though in the reverse order: surprise (M = 29.4, SD = 25.4)
and anxiety (M = 22.9, SD = 21.2).
It is noteworthy that the emotion with the highest rating in the
EAS exercise was anxiety, given the theoretical construct that
traditional masculinity emphasizes success, independence, and a
reluctance to acknowledge fears. It may suggest that when men are given
a structured way of responding to emotional issues, they are willing to
respond, even if it means acknowledging emotional states generally
minimized by traditional masculine socialization. Supportive of this
idea was the finding that men scoring highest on the gender-role stress
measure (upper half of the MGRS) actually reported greater emotional
intensity on the structured exercise (EAS) than did men in the lower
half of the MGRS, t(67) = -2.27, p [is less than].02. In this sample,
then, men with higher levels of gender-role stress were quite willing to
identify their emotional states on paper in a structured exercise--even
more willing to do so than men reporting less gender-role stress.
It is apparent that our measures of verbal and structured
expressiveness were tapping different dimensions. The correlation
between verbal expressiveness and scores on the EAS was rather low (r =
.06). The implication is that other variables may explain the
difference. In the present study, masculine gender-role stress served as
the independent variable, revealing the preferred modes of emotional
expressiveness (structured or verbal) in men.
GENDER=ROLE STRESS
The MGRS score for this sample of men (M = 80.16, SD = 20.88) was
similar to another sample of adult men reported in the literature: M =
83.2, SD = 21.0 (Watkins, Eisler, Carpenter, Schechtman, & Fisher,
1991). The mean age of both groups was about 35. No significant
differences were found by comparing MGRS scores for the various
occupational groups within the present sample.
The low overall correlation between verbal and structured
expressiveness in the entire sample leaves open the possibility that
masculinity themes may be related to a preference for either structured
or verbal forms of expressiveness. To test this question, a t-test for
paired samples was performed, using gender-role stress as an independent
variable for verbal and structured expressiveness.
Verbal expressiveness scores were computed by transforming into
z-scores the different units of measurement (see Table 2) generated by
the audiotaped responses to the videotape. We combined the intensity of
emotions rating with the response time and the proportion of overall
words that related to emotion; the result was a single z-score measure
of verbal expressiveness. Similarly, the structured exercise scores
(EAS) were transformed to z-scores. The t-test for paired samples
compared the value of verbal expressiveness of each participant with his
overall EAS score, and yielded a paired mean difference for each man.
Two groups were formed by a median split of scores on the MGRS,
indicating high and low gender-role stress.
Results of the two-tailed t-test comparisons were consistent with
theoretical expectations (Table 4). In the high gender-role stress
group, the mean z-score for structured expressiveness (M = .3223) was
higher than the mean score for verbal expressiveness (M = -.1259). The
paired difference (M = .4482) was significant, t(34) = 2.17, p [is less
than] .05. The low gender-role stress group had higher verbal
expressiveness scores (M = .1335) than structured expressiveness scores
(M = -.3525). The paired difference (M = .4860) was significant, t(32) =
-2.00, p [is less than].05. This pattern indicates that high verbal
expressiveness was associated with low gender-role stress, whereas high
structured expressiveness was associated with high gender-role stress.
It seems appropriate to suggest that gender-role stress may predict the
preferred mode of emotional expressiveness. Men with more gender-role
stress related to views of traditional masculinity are more likely to be
comfortable expressing themselves in structured ways, and men with less
masculine gender-role stress are likely to be more comfortable
expressing themselves verbally.
Table 4
Masculine Gender Role Stress as a Predictor of Verbal versus Written
Expressiveness
Masculine Gender-Role Stress (MGRS) Verbal Written t p
High Masculine Stress -.1259 .3223 2.17 .018
Low Masculine Stress .1335 -.3525 -2.00 .027
Note: Values are expressed as combined z-scores, transformed from
measures of verbal expressiveness (response time, number of emotion
words, and emotionality of words) and from structured expressiveness
(Emotional Assessment Scale, from Carlson et al., 1989). High and low
masculine groups were formed by a median split of the MGRS (Eisler &
Skidmore, 1987).
DISCUSSION
What accounts for the belief that men do not easily express
themselves emotionally? Is it that many men experience very low levels
of physiological arousal to emotional stimuli? Is it primarily because
men are socialized to say little about their emotions? Or is it some
combination of both?
Two findings in the present study seem relevant to this question.
The first is that all men in our sample were physiologically aroused by
emotional stimuli; arousal was not absent among men in traditionally
masculine occupations or for men who hold views of masculinity that lead
to stress. The second finding is that regardless of gender-role stress
issues, men are aware of their emotional arousal and are willing to
report it; what varies are their preferences about the most comfortable
format for expressing those emotions. Men with higher levels of stress
related to traditional gender roles appear to prefer structured
approaches, and men with less gender-role stress appear to prefer verbal
expressions.
The first finding that all the men in our sample were
physiologically aroused may not be especially surprising, as it is
consistent with the summary findings of Gottman and Levenson (1988), who
pointed to a series of studies showing high levels of physiological
arousal in men. This finding speaks directly to the question posed at
the outset of the present study. The explanation for the emotional
inexpressiveness of some men is not that they experience low
physiological arousal and therefore have little to report. Men do
respond physiologically to emotional stimuli, and the arousal does not
appear to be related to any predictive variable in our sample
(gender-role stress, occupation, age, and so forth).
The second finding is consistent with the literature indicating
that many men prefer exercises and structured tasks more than open-ended
talk about their inner feelings (Hurst, 1997; Robertson, 1989). It is
also consistent with findings from studies that show men still are far
more likely in family settings to offer services such as household
maintenance, repairs, or transportation to appointments, and less likely
to be involved in family roles that require high levels of emotional
investment (Dwyer & Seecomb, 1991).
Perhaps the best answer to the question that prompted this study is
that both "nature" and "nurture" contribute to an
understanding of men and emotions. With regard to nature, we found that
emotional inexpressiveness in men was not due to an absence of emotional
arousal. In fact, as Gottman and Levenson have argued (1988), the fact
that men have very high levels of physical arousal to stress may
actually contribute to less verbal expressiveness. They noted that while
experiencing conflict with their partners, many men report feeling
flooded or overwhelmed and therefore less able to be verbal.
Physiological arousal may indeed be a factor contributing to low levels
of emotional expressiveness in men--not because the arousal levels are
low, but because they are comparatively high (Gottman & Levenson,
1988).
Regarding the nurture contribution, we found wide variability in
the format preferred by men to express the emotions that they do have.
Some men preferred to express themselves in an activity, and others
preferred words. These differences seem more likely to be influenced by
socialization. Given the differences between gender-role expectations in
this culture, many men simply have fewer opportunities to develop their
skills of emotional expressiveness. It is plausible to argue that many
of these differences among men are learned (Eagly & Steffen, 1986;
Grossman & Wood, 1993).
It is tempting to speculate about implications for counseling that
might stem from these results. For example, when working with male
clients struggling with affect, therapists may be able to increase their
effectiveness by giving attention to the client's gender role
perspectives, and exploring any stress reactions related to those
expectations. Many men may freeze at versions of the question, "How
do you feel?" But those same men may cooperate willingly and
effectively if given a structured way of reporting their feelings. In
the context of the present study, the task of identifying the intensity
of their emotions on 100-millimeter lines was effective; other creative
ways may be equally as effective. This approach is certainly consistent
with theoretical expectations, considering the traditional male emphasis
on problem-solving, task-completion, and structured activities. Given
that more traditionally defined men find talk therapy difficult, if not
ineffective, it follows that the use of written assessments, structured
expressive modalities, and task direction may provide more satisfying
results to the client.
At the same time, it must be noted that men with lower levels of
gender-role stress actually prefer to express their emotions through
words rather than a structured exercise. Again, there are implications
for therapists. Knowing something about the gender-role views of a
particular male client may be useful in treatment.
Another implication of these findings is related to the concept of
stress for men. The present study was described to the research
participants as a study about responses to stress; about 80% of the men
who were invited actually participated. Participants often volunteered
comments to the experimenters about how pleased they were to be able to
participate in a study about how men respond to stress. Seeking to
reduce stress is one of the more common presenting complaints of men
coming to therapy. "Feeling stressed" covers a wide array of
separate physical and emotional problems. When men do not know how to
identify or recognize underlying issues, most symptoms can be described
as stress. Stress generally arises for men when they do not believe
problem-solving options exist or when they are uncertain about how to
maximize healthy and productive responses that might be available. One
approach to men, then, may be to focus on physical and emotional
self-awareness and how these are indications of stress. When placed in
this more socially acceptable category of stress, men may be more
willing to consider behavior and attitude choices designed to reduce
their stress.
This study underlines the need to explore the importance of
gender-role issues in thinking about the emotional components of
specific presenting issues brought by men to counseling--men with
relationship concerns, men as parents, men with sexual concerns, male
friendship development, male interactions at work, and so forth. Given
that men who experience more stress related to traditional masculinity
may respond more readily to treatment that includes exercises or tasks,
therapists can approach these issues with a wider array of treatment
options. For these men, psychotherapy that emphasizes behavioral
changes, activities, and exercises may be more appealing.
Two potential limitations in this study must be noted. First, we
selected working men from a narrow set of male-dominated occupations
(e.g., fire fighters, police officers, and safety officials). To what
degree these findings generalize to men with other occupational
interests or personality styles is unknown. Further, because we were
primarily interested in within-group differences among men, no women
were included in the sample. Comparative observations are therefore
restricted. A second limitation is that the techniques used to induce
stress (video, math test, and cold pressor) are not in themselves tasks
that men perform daily. Although they were effective in meeting internal
validity demands, they may not generalize to other forms of stress which
men encounter.
This study also raises questions for further reflection and study.
Can the use of biofeedback techniques facilitate successful
self-recognition of unexpressed emotionality in men? What motivates men
to desire more effective verbal skills? The use of physiological
measures certainly can assist in uncovering more information about the
emotional content of various presenting issues. It may be that
physiological arousal patterns differ with regard to presenting issues.
One way to test this would be to invite male research participants to
report their emotional responses (both verbally and in written form)
regarding a variety of presenting issues while measuring their
physiological arousal.
These and other questions remain. Nonetheless, for therapists
working with the emotional content of men in therapy, this study does
suggest that virtually all men experience physiological arousal as a
response to emotional stimuli (there are no unemotional men), and that
given the right format, men are willing to share those feelings.
The authors wish to acknowledge the contributions of Arthur J.
Rathbun, Cheryl Harper, Ann Johnson, and Linda L. Gaffney for their
technical and editorial assistance; and of Wendy Grove, Zaneta Barte,
and Cheryl Harper in collecting the data.
REFERENCES
Allport, G. E., & Odbert, H. S. (1936). Trait-names: A
psycho-lexical study. Psychological Monographs, 47 (1, Whole No. 211).
Anderson, C. D. (1989). Expression of affect and physiological
response in psychosomatic patients. Journal of Psychosomatic Research,
25(3), 143-149.
Arena, J. G. (1984). Inter- and intrasession reliability of
psychophysiological post-stress adaptation periods. Journal of
Behavioral Assessment, 6(3), 247-260.
Averill, J. R. (1975). A semantic atlas of emotional concepts.
Abstracted in Catalog of Selected Documents in Psychology, 5, 330.
American Psychological Association and Johnson Associates: Greenwich,
CT.
Beck, A. A. (1972). Depression: Causes and treatment. Philadelphia:
University of Pennsylvania Press.
Bernstein, J. S. (1995). "Real men" don't talk:
Attempting psychodynamic group therapy with inner-city boys. Journal of
Child and Adolescent Group Therapy, 5(2), 83-106.
Bio-medical Instruments, Inc. (1994). Focused Technology F1000
Instrumentation System. Warren, MI: Focused Technology.
Breland, H. M. (1996). Word frequency and word difficulty: A
comparison of counts on four corpora. Psychological Science, 7(2),
96-99.
Brooks, G. R., & Gilbert, L. A. (1995). Men in families: Old
constraints, new possibilities. In R. F. Levant & W. S. Pollack (Eds.), A new psychology of men (pp. 252-279). New York: Basic Books.
Burrowes, B. D., & Halberstadt, A. G. (1987). Self- and
family-expressiveness styles in the experience and expression of anger.
Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 11, 254-268.
Canary, D. J., & Emmers-Sommers, T. M. (1997). Sex and gender
differences in personal relationships. New York: Guilford Press.
Carlson, C. R., Collins, F. L., Stewart, J. F., Porzelius, J.,
Nitz, J. A., & Lind, C. O. (1989). The assessment of emotional
reactivity: A scale development and validation study. Journal of
Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment, 11(4), 313-324.
Cherulnik, P. D. (1979). Sex differences in the expression of
emotion in a structured social encounter. Sex Roles, 5, 413-424.
Conner, K., & Heesacker, M. (1999). Sex, gender, and emotions:
Accuracy of the dyadic communication of emotions. Manuscript submitted
for publication.
Crano, W. D., & Aronoff, J. (1978). A cross-cultural study of
expressive and instrumental role complementarity in the family. American
Sociological Review, 43, 463-471.
Cleary, P. D. (1987). Gender differences in stress related
disorders. In R. C. Barnett, L. Biener, & G. K. Baruch (Eds.),
Gender and stress (pp. 39-72). New York: Free Press.
Courtenay, W. H. (2000). Engendering health: A social
constructionist examination of men's health beliefs and behaviors.
Psychology of Men & Masculinity, 1(1), 4-15.
David, D. S., & Brannon, R. (1976). The forty-nine percent
majority: The male sex role. New York: Random House.
Davidson, R. J. (1994). Complexities in the search for
emotion-specific physiology. In P. Eckman & R. J. Davidson (Eds.),
The nature of emotion (pp. 237-242). New York: Oxford University Press.
Donat, D. C., & McCullough, J. P. (1983). Psychophysiological
discriminants of depression at rest and in response to stress. Journal
of Clinical Psychology, 39(3), 315-320.
Dwyer, J. W., & Seecomb, K. (1991). Elder care as family labor.
Journal of Family Issues, 12, 229-247.
Eagly, A. H., & Steffen, V. J. (1986). Gender stereotypes,
occupational roles, and beliefs about part-time employees. Psychology of
Women Quarterly, 10(3), 252-262,
Eckman, P. (1972). Facial expressions of emotion. In J. K. Cole
(Ed.), Nebraska Symposium on Motivation. Lincoln: University of Nebraska
Press.
Eckman, P., & Davidson, R. J. (1994). The nature of emotion.
New York: Oxford University Press.
Eisler, R. M., & Blalock, J. A. (1991). Masculine gender role
stress: Implications for the assessment of men. Clinical Psychology
Review, 11, 45-60.
Eisler, R. M., & Skidmore, J. R. (1987). MGRS: Scale
development and component factors in the appraisal of stressful
situations. Behavior Modification, 11, 123136.
Eisler, R. M., Skidmore, J. R., & Ward, C. H. (1988). Masculine
gender-role stress: Predictor of anger, anxiety, and health-risk
behaviors. Journal of Personality Assessment, 52(1), 133-141.
Fitzgibbons, L., & Simons, R. F. (1992), Affective response to
color-slide stimuli in subjects with physical anhedonia: A three-systems
analysis. Psychophysiology, 29(6), 613-620.
Friedman, R. M., & Lerner, L. (1986). Toward a new psychology
of men: Psychoanalytic and social perspectives. New York: Guilford.
Frijda, N. H. (1994). Emotions are functional, most of the time. In
P. Eckman & R. J. Davidson (Eds.), The nature of emotion (pp.
112-122). New York: Oxford University Press.
Gerrards-Hesse, A., Spies, K., and Hesse, F. W. (1994).
Experimental inductions of emotional states and their effectiveness: A
review. British Journal of Psychology, 85, 55-78.
Goldberg, H. (1976). The hazards of being male: Surviving the myth
of masculine privilege. New York: Signet.
Gordon, G., & Meth, R. L. (1990). Men as husbands. In R. L.
Meth and R. S. Pasick (Eds.), Men in therapy (pp. 54-77). New York:
Guilford.
Gottman, J. M., & Levenson, R. W. (1988). The social
psychophysiology of marriage. In P. Noller and M. A. Fitzpatrick (Eds),
Perspectives in marital interaction (pp. 182-199). Philadelphia:
Multilingual Matters Ltd.
Gray, J. A. (1994). Three fundamental emotion systems. In P. Eckman
& R. J. Davidson (Eds.), The nature of emotion (pp. 243-246). New
York: Oxford University Press.
Greenwald, M., Cook, E., & Lang P. J. (1989). Affective
judgment and psychophysiological response: Dimensional covariation on
the evaluation of pictorial stimuli. Journal of Psychophysiology, 3,
51-64.
Grossman, M., & Wood, W. (1993). Sex differences in intensity
of emotional experience: A social role interpretation. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 65(5), 1010-1022.
Heesacker, M., Wester, S. R., Vogel, D. L., Wentzel, J. F.,
Mejia-Millan, C. M., & Goodholm, Jr., C. R. (1999). Gender-based
emotional stereotyping. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 46(4),
483-495.
Hess, U., Kappas, A., McHugo, G. J., Lanzetta, J. T., & Kleck,
R. E. (1992). The facilitative effect of facial expression on the
self-generation of emotion. International Journal of Psychophysiology,
12, 251-265.
Hurst, M. A. (1997). The best fit in counseling men: Are there
solutions to treating men as the problem? Unpublished doctoral
dissertation, Ball State University, Muncie, Indiana.
Isen, A. M., & Gorgolione, J. M. (1983). Some specific effects
of four affect-induction procedures. Personality and Social Psychology,
45, 20-31.
Izard, C. E. (1977). Human emotions. New York: Plenum.
Izard, C. E. (1991). The psychology of emotions. New York: Plenum.
Kelly, K. & Hall A. (1992). Mental health counseling for men
[Special issue]. Journal of Mental Health Counseling, 14(3).
LaFrance, M., & Benaji, M. (1992). Toward a reconsideration of
the gender-emotion relationship. In M. S. Clark (Ed.), Review of
personality and social psychology (Vol. 14, pp. 178-201). Newbury Park,
CA: Sage.
Lang, P. J. (1984). Cognition in emotion: Concept and action. In C.
Izard, J. Kagan, & R. Zajonc (Eds.), Emotion, cognition and behavior
(pp. 193-226). New York: Cambridge University Press.
Lang, P. G. (1985). The cognitive psychophysiology of emotion: Fear
and anxiety. In A. H. Tuma & J. D. Maser (Eds 3, Anxiety and the
anxiety disorders (pp. 131-170). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Lang, P. J., Greenwald, M. K., Bradley, M. M., & Hamm, A. O.
(1993). Looking at pictures: Affective, facial, visceral, and behavioral
reactions. Psychophysiology, 30, 261-273.
Levant, R. F. (1992). A gender-aware divorce prevention program.
Men's Studies Review, 9(2), 15-20.
Levenson, R. W. (1994). Human emotions: A functional view. In P.
Eckman & R. J. Davidson (Eds.), The nature of emotion (pp. 123-126).
New York: Oxford University Press.
Marston, A., Hart, J., Hileman, C., & Faunce, W. (1984). Toward
the laboratory study of sadness and crying. American Journal of
Psychology, 97, 127-131.
Martin, M. (1990). On the induction of mood. Clinical Psychology
Review, 10, 669697.
McNair, D. M., Lorr, M., & Droppleman, L. F. (1981). The
profile of mood states. San Diego: Educational and Industrial Testing
Service.
Meinecke, C. E. (1981). Socialized to die younger? Hypermasculinity
and men's health. The Personnel and Guidance Journal, December,
241-245.
Miller, L. (1995). Tough guys: Psychotherapeutic strategies with
law enforcement and emergency services personnel. Psychotherapy, 32(4),
592-600.
Netter, G. (Producer), & Rubin, B. J. (Director). (1993). My
life. [Film]. (Available from Columbia Tristar, 3400 Riverside Drive,
Burbank, California, 91505-4627.
Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (1987). Sex differences in unipolar depression:
Evidence and theory. Psychological Bulletin, 101, 259-282.
Nunnally, J. (1981). The affect inventory. Unpublished manuscript.
Nashville, TN: Vanderbilt University.
O'Neil, J. M. (1981). Patterns of gender role conflict and
strain: Sexism and fear of femininity in men's lives. The Personnel
and Guidance Journal, 60, 203-210.
O'Neil, J. M., Helms, B. J., Gable, R. K., David, L., &
Wrightsman, L. S. (1986). Gender-role conflict scale: College men's
fear of femininity. Sex Roles, 14, 335-350.
Parsons, T., & Bales, R. F. (1955). Family, socialization and
interaction process. New York: Free Press.
Pasick, R. S., Gordon, S., & Meth, R. L. (1990). Helping men
understand themselves. In R. L. Meth and R. S. Pasick, Men in therapy
(pp. 152-180). New York: Guilford.
Pleck, J. H. (1981). The myth of masculinity. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Pleck, J. H., Sonenstein, F. L., & Ku, L. (2. (1993).
Masculinity ideology and its correlates. In S. Oskamp and M. Costanzo
(Eds.), Gender and social psychology (pp. 85-110). Newbury Park, CA:
Sage.
Reeves, J. L., & Shapiro, D. (1983). Heart-rate reactivity to
cold pressor stress following biofeedback training. Biofeedback and
Self-regulation, 8(1), 87-99.
Robertson, J. M. (1989). Men who avoid counseling: Identifying
personality correlates and preferences for assistance alternatives.
Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of California, Santa
Barbara.
Robertson, J. M., & Freeman, R. (1995). Men and emotions:
Developing masculine-congruent views of affective expressiveness.
Journal of College Student Development, 36(6), 606-607.
Saurer, M. K., & Eisler, R. M. (1990). The role of masculine
gender role stress in expressivity and social support network factors.
Sex Roles, 23(5-6), 261-171.
Scher, M. (1993). Men and boys, myths and mythic proportions: A
rejoinder to Kelly and Hall. Journal of Mental Health Counseling, 15,
290-297.
Scherer, K. R., Wallbott, H. G., & Summerfield, A. B. (1986).
Experiencing emotion: A cross-cultural study. Cambridge, England:
Cambridge University Press.
Shay, J. J. (1996). "Okay, I'm here, but I'm not
talking:" Psychotherapy with the reluctant male. Psychotherapy
33(3), 503-513.
Spielberger, C. D., Gorsuch, R. L., & Lushene, R. (1970). The
State-Trait Anxiety Inventory Manual. Palo Alto: Consulting
Psychologists Press.
Subotnik K. L., Shapiro, D. (1984). Heart rate biofeedback and cold
pressor pain: Effects of expectancy. Biofeedback and Self-regulation,
9(1), 55-75.
Thorndike, E. L. (1921). The teacher's word book. New York:
Teachers College, Columbia University.
Wanigaratne, S., & Barker, C. (1995). Clients' preferences
for styles of therapy. British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 34(2),
215-222.
Watkins, P. L., Eisler, R. M., Carpenter, L., Schechtman, K. B.,
& Fisher, E. B. (1991). Psychosocial and physiological correlates of
male gender role stress among employed adults. Behavioral Medicine,
17(2), 86-90.
Wilcox, D. W., & Forrest, L. (1992). The problems of men and
counseling: Gender bias or gender truth? Journal of Mental Health
Counseling, 14(3), 291-304.
Wood, W., Rhodes, N., & Whelan, M. (1989). Sex differences in
positive well-being: A consideration of emotional style and marital
status. Psychological Bulletin, 106, 249-264.
Correspondence concerning this article should be sent to John M.
Robertson, University Counseling Services, Lafene 238, Kansas State
University, Manhattan, Kansas 66502 or johnrobe@ksu.edu.
John M. Robertson, Ph.D., is a psychologist at University
Counseling Services and a member of the graduate faculty for the
Department of Counseling and Educational Psychology at Kansas State
University. He also provides clinical services for Southwind Counseling
Services in Manhattan, Kansas. His professional interests focus on
issues related to the psychology of men, including relationships,
emotionality, abuse, gender-role transitions, and help-seeking behavior.
(johnrobe@ksu.edu)
Joyce Woodford, M.S., is a senior clinician at University
Counseling Services, Kansas State University. Her therapy and research
interests are help-seeking, gender differences, and issues of peace. Her
work has included foci on women's issues, childhood abuse survival,
and relationships. She also has an interest in the contemporary subject
of social interest through Adlerian Individual Psychology. She is
completing a doctorate in clinical psychology at the Fielding Institute.
(joycew@ksu.edu)
Kimberly Kay Danos received her Bachelor of Arts and Science with
majors in biology and Asian studies from Regis University in 1993. She
continued her education at Kansas State University completing her Master
of Science in counseling and personnel services in 1997. Danos enhanced
her graduate education with special emphasis on counseling, biofeedback,
and stress management. She was an active member of the Kansas State
University counseling services staff and research team from 1995 to
1999. In 1999, Danos accepted a position with the Human Services
Planning Alliance in Des Moines, Iowa. She now focuses her research and
work on early childhood issues. Her research continues to encompass
gender studies, applied methods of biofeedback, effects of stress on
brain development, the psychology of poverty, and violent deviant
behavior. (kkdanos@aoi.com)
Mark A. Hurst is a licensed psychologist conducting private
practice and consultation in Olympia, Washington. Raised in a family of
boatbuilders, tugboaters, and school teachers in the rural Pacific
Northwest, he earned a master's degree in clinical psychology from
Eastern Washington University and was awarded the doctorate by Ball
State University in Muncie, Indiana. Dr. Hurst serves as adjunct faculty
at the Evergreen State College, where he sponsors student-directed
internships and creates social psychology and men's studies
courses. His other projects address human development and transition,
media literacy, and film production about men's inner lives.
(mkhurst@home.com)
Chi-Wei Lin, Ph.D. Kansas State University 1999, is director of the
student counseling center and assistant professor in the College of
Humanities and Education at Chung Yuan Christian University. Lin's
clinical and research interests include personality disorders, anxiety
disorders, at-risk youth, and voluntary services. (chiwei@cycu.edu.tw)