VOUCHERS AND TEST SCORES.
Peterson, Paul E.
What the numbers show
Students in private schools learn more and score higher on
standardized tests than their counterparts in public schools. Some say
this does not prove that private schools are better but only shows that
children from more motivated families (who are willing and able to pay
the tuition) attend private schools. As former Wisconsin state school
superintendent Herbert Grover, an arch-critic of school choice, has
argued, "Do private school children outperform children in public
schools? It's hard to imagine that they wouldn't, given the
initial advantages they enjoy from their parents."
To see whether students actually learn more as a result of
attending a private school, my colleagues and I are currently evaluating
a school- choice pilot program in New York City funded by the School
Choice Scholarships Foundation (SCSF).
In February 1997 the SCSF offered public-school students from
low-income families who were entering grades one through five a chance
to win a $1,400 annual scholarship, good for at least three years, to
help defray the cost of attending a private school, either religious or
secular. Over 20,000 applications were received. Ninety percent of the
applicants were either Latino or African American. Scholarships were
awarded by means of a lottery. Some 1,200 SCSF scholarships were used to
attend some 225 participating private schools. Students began school in
the fall of 1997.
Because SCSF awarded scholarships by means of a lottery, it was
possible to evaluate the pilot program using a scientific method
regularly employed in medical research, the randomized field trial
(RFT). In a medical RFT, one group is given a pill, the other a placebo.
Individuals are assigned to one or another group by lot, or, in
scientific parlance, at random. This method is preferred over all
others, because the test and control groups, on average, can be assumed
to be similar, save for the medical intervention under investigation.
Positive results from RFTs are required in order to win approval of a
medication from the Food and Drug Administration.
In my view, education innovations should be subjected to similar
testing before being introduced on a wide scale. Unfortunately, this
seldom happens, in part because public schools typically resist
rigorous, independent evaluations, but also because the Department of
Education, unlike the FDA, has not provided strong research leadership.
Fortunately, SCSF was willing to permit a rigorous, independent
evaluation of its pilot program, and my colleagues and I were able to
obtain funds for the evaluation from a broad network of private
foundations.
The lottery was held in mid-May 1997. The firm responsible for the
evaluation, Mathematica Policy Research, administered the lottery in
order to leave no doubt about its integrity; SCSF announced the winners.
To estimate the effects of attending a private school, the
mathematics and reading components of the Iowa Test of Basic Skills were
administered in the spring of 1997 to scholarship applicants. Each
component of the test took approximately one hour to complete. Students
participating in the evaluation were tested again in the spring of 1998.
Both the scholarship students and students in the control group were
tested in locations other than the school they were currently attending.
To guarantee similar testing conditions, both for scholarship students
and students in the control group, the tests were administered under the
supervision of the evaluation team.
Each student's performance was given a national percentile
ranking between one and one hundred. The national average is 50. The
data indicate that these students are educationally disadvantaged:
overall, average test scores were below the 30th percentile. Results
were collected from approximately 85 percent of the participants in the
evaluation, an unusually high response rate from a low-income,
inner-city population.
Our evaluation focused on students entering grades two through
five, because only from them were we able to obtain baseline test-score
data. Baseline test-scores were unavailable for those entering first
grade, because those children were still in kindergarten at the time of
application.
After one year, the national percentile ranking of students
attending private schools was, on average, two points higher in reading
and mathematics than the ranking of the comparison group that remained
in public schools. Differences were uneven in grades two and three, but
choice students in grades four and five achieved substantially higher
scores, six percentile points more in math, and four points more in
reading.
Long-term Rewards
When reporting these effects of school choice, Education Week
headlined them as "modest" the New York Times found them
"slight." Whether or not these gains after one year are slight
or substantial depends in part on what happens in later years.
Nonetheless, there is reason to conclude that the effects of choice on
the performance of students in their middle years is already sizable
enough to merit careful consideration.
Scholars typically calculate effect sizes in standard deviations.
As an indication of both the average score and the degree of variation
from it, standard deviation allows us to compare results across
different data sets. One can grasp its essential quality by keeping in
mind that one standard deviation is approximately the current difference
between the average test scores of blacks and whites nationwide. The
effects of school choice on students in fourth and fifth grade are
roughly one fifth of a standard deviation. If similar effects occur in
subsequent years, these are large enough to bring the scores of minority
students up to the levels currently attained by whites. This would be
taking a large step toward achieving equal educational opportunity
across ethnic groups, something most people would regard as a major
accomplishment.
More to the point, these test scores are not a triviality, or a
hobgoblin only of interest to academic researchers. Students who score
higher on standardized tests are more likely to remain in school, more
likely to achieve a college degree, more likely to remain married and
avoid welfare dependency, and more likely to enjoy a higher family
income. According to the best available estimates, a gain of one
standard deviation in test scores will later in life increase that
person's family income by over 20 percent. If students in the
choice program in New York City simply hold the gains they have already
achieved, one could expect their family income, on average, to be 4
percent higher than it otherwise would have been. Assuming a modest
annual income of $30,000, that's an increase of $1,200 a year. If
these estimates are reasonably accurate, the philanthropists in New York
will realize an ample return on their charity dollar, once these
students enter the labor force.
Another way to consider the effects of the SCSF program is to
compare them to the results of a different intervention. Very few
education innovations of interest have been subjected to a random field
trial, but one. Class size reduction from 25 to 15 students has been
rigorously evaluated by this method. It is worth comparing the results
of a school choice field trial with the results from a class-size
reduction experiment, because both innovations can be introduced rather
straight-forwardly by legislative action. (Other reforms, such as
requiring students to do more homework, are much more difficult to
mandate by legislative fiat.)
The class size RFT was conducted in Tennessee, where students were
randomly assigned to classes of different sizes. No incremental effects
on student learning were observed for students after the first grade.
Among first graders, effect sizes varied between .15 and .30 standard
deviations. Fred Mosteller, one of those involved in the experiment,
observed, "although effect sizes of the magnitude of 0.1, 0.2, or
0.3 may not seem to be impressive gains for a single individual, for a
population they can be quite substantial."
Congress was apparently persuaded by such reasoning and by the
results from the effect sizes observed in Tennessee. After extensive
policy deliberations in which the Tennessee evaluation was frequently
mentioned, in 1998 Congress enacted legislation appropriating $1.1
billion for the purpose of reducing the size of elementary school classes.
The effect sizes observed in our evaluation of the New York
scholarship program in grades four and five do not differ materially
from those observed in Tennessee in grade one. The effects among fourth
and fifth graders of attendance at a private school were, on average,
.23 and .18, not much different from the .15 to .3 effects observed in
the first grade of the Tennessee Study-the only grade where incremental
class size effects were detected. Following Mosteller's guidelines,
these effect sizes, observed after just one year in the program, can be
said to be "quite substantial."
From a cost-benefit perspective, school choice seems a better
intervention than reductions in class size. To get effects of about .2
standard deviations, class sizes in the Tennessee study were reduced
from approximately 25 to approximately 15 students. If such reductions
were introduced as a school reform more generally, it would increase the
size of both the teaching staff and classroom space by 40 percent. Per
pupil costs could be expected to increase by approximately 20 percent
(if it is assumed that classroom costs constitute about half the cost of
public schooling). By comparison, the per pupil cost of school choice is
minimal; the taxpayer may in fact enjoy some savings, if eventually
competition among schools leads to more effective education at lower
cost.
Moreover, the incremental benefits of class size reduction
disappear after first-grade. If larger differences between the test
scores of scholarship students and those in the control group appear in
subsequent years in New York City, the benefits of school choice will
clearly outstrip those obtained through large reductions in class size.
When we initially announced our findings, Sandra Feldman, president
of the American Federation of Teachers, offered the interesting
hypothesis that class size and school size probably accounts for the
results that we observed. "I see it as a validation of the need for
small class sizes, and for smaller schools that are orderly and
disciplined," she said. To test this hypothesis, we conducted a
statistical test in order to ascertain whether any of the following
characteristics could account for the higher test scores in the private
schools: (1) class size (2) school size (3) discipline problems (4)
school-parent communications (5) school resources.
Data on these potential explanatory factors were available from
information contained in the questionnaires administered to parents when
the students were tested. Although parents reported that private schools
were superior in all five respects, only discipline problems had a large
and consistently positive effect on both the math and reading scores of
the two older grades. Class size had no significant effect. Students in
larger (not smaller) schools did slightly better in math (but not
reading). Improved parental communications had a positive effect on math
(but not reading) scores, and additional resources had a positive effect
on reading (but not math) scores. Most importantly, none of these
factors, nor all of them, reduced the size of the effects of receiving a
scholarship to attend a private school in fourth and fifth grade.
These new pilot programs provide new opportunities to find out
whether students learn more when families are given a choice of schools.
Perhaps the program's impact comes from the sheer fact of
choice: the opportunity to better match older students with an
appropriate school. But, more likely, it is some constellation of many
factors that affect scores in ways not easily captured by a statistical
model. In any case, the advantage of attending a private school is not
readily reduced to any one or single set of factors.
As we have pointed out, the advantages of attending a private
school in New York City are not clearly evident until a student enters
fourth and fifth grade. This finding is consistent with other
indications that in American education problems begin during the middle
years of schooling. According to the National Assessment of Educational
Progress (NAEP), students in fourth grade are performing at higher
levels than their counterparts a generation ago. Gains over the past two
decades have been particularly large for students from minority groups.
But NAEP data also show that, after fourth grade, initial gains
disappear. In fact, students nationwide learned less between fourth and
eighth grade in the 1990s than they did in the '70s. The slippage
seems even greater in high school. Similarly, international comparisons
reveal that U.S. fourth-grade students keep up in science and math with
most of their peers abroad (though not with the Japanese and Koreans).
But by eighth grade U.S. students trail those in all other leading
industrial nations, and by twelfth grade they fall to near the bottom of
all participating countries. If the problems in American education
develop in the middle years of schooling, perhaps it is at this point
that the advantages that come with school choice are particularly
evident.
Of course, the findings from New York City are simply first-year
results. Our evaluation is scheduled to continue for two more years, and
only time will tell whether the initial gains are maintained in the
future. It remains to be seen whether school choice, if generalized to a
larger population, will yield comparable gains. But it does seem time to
begin larger-scale experiments.
An Historical Perspective
The unique quality of the SCSF pilot program can be appreciated by
situating its evaluation within the long-running controversy over
research on public and private schools. In the early 1980s two
nationwide studies, one conducted by a team headed by sociologist James
Coleman, the other conducted by John Chubb and Terry Moe, reported that
high school students learned more in private than in public schools.
School choice critics questioned the findings from both studies on the
grounds that the students in private schools came from families more
committed to their children's education.
Both studies had anticipated this argument by taking into account
family background characteristics, such as education and income. But
critics say that no amount of statistical tinkering can ever fully
correct for the selection effect: families who pay to send their child
to private school are almost certainly more involved in and concerned
about their child's education, even after adjusting for demographic
characteristics. Even the Coleman research team admitted, the
"difference between parents, by its very nature, is not something
on which students in public and private schools can be equated" in
a statistical analysis.
Beginning in the late 1980s, a number of publicly and
privately-funded school choice pilot programs began providing
researchers with opportunities to consider the question anew.
Educational outcome information is currently available from programs in
San Antonio, Indianapolis, Milwaukee, and Cleveland. In the next few
years, still more information will become available not only from New
York City but also from other pilot programs that are getting underway
in many other cities, including Washington, D.C., Dayton, and San
Antonio.
These new pilot programs provide new opportunities to find out
whether students learn more when families are given a choice of school.
For one thing, differences in family background have been reduced,
compared to the national surveys mentioned above, because most of these
programs are limited to inner-city children from low-income families.
More importantly, from a research perspective, these scholarships are
often awarded by lottery whenever the number of applicants exceeds the
number of scholarships available. Because a lottery is used to award the
scholarships, these programs can be evaluated by means of an RFT.
Unfortunately, many of the school-choice pilot programs conducted
thus far do not permit an RFT. Privately funded programs in Indianapolis
and San Antonio admitted students on a first-come, first-serve basis.
Such admission procedures have a fairness of their own, and they are
easy to administer, but any findings from these programs may be
contaminated by the selection effect. After all, those families who are
quick, clever and well-connected enough to get a first-come, first-serve
scholarship are likely to have other attributes that favorably affect
their child's educational attainment. Nonetheless, test score
results from these experiments are mainly positive. For example, the
scores of students participating in the school choice program in San
Antonio increased between 1991-92 and 1993-94, while those of the
public-school comparison group fell. In Indianapolis, students in
private schools did better than students in public schools, particularly
in grades six through eight.
Inconclusive Cleveland
Much the same can be said for the disparate findings that have
emerged from research on the state-funded pilot program that began in
Cleveland in the fall of 1996. Although the state required that the
scholarships be awarded by lot, various legal, political, and
administrative problems made it impossible to gather data necessary to
conduct an RFT. As a result, both the research team that I headed and
other researchers were forced to rely upon less precise research
techniques.
In 1997 my colleagues and I found that students attending the Hope
schools, two newly established choice schools serving 25 percent of the
students previously attending public schools, gained 9 national
percentile rank points in math and 6 percentile points in reading. But
because no control group was available for comparison purposes, we
cannot be sure that a comparable group of students would not have
achieved similar gains in Cleveland's public schools.
All in all the evidence that school choice enhances achievement of
low- income students has now become quite substantial.
Another evaluation by Indiana University's School of Education
found no programmatic effects on the test scores of 94 third grade
choice students. The Indiana University evaluation suffers from a number
of limitations:
1. The study analyzed only third-grade test scores; no information
is available for students in kindergarten, first or second grades.
2. To control for student achievement prior to the beginning of the
scholarship program, the evaluation used implausible second-grade scores
collected by the Cleveland Public Schools before the beginning of the
choice experiment when students were still in public school. These
dubious second-grade scores tell us that students from central-city,
low-income, largely one-parent families were performing in second grade,
on average, at approximately the national average. Yet in an
independently proctored test administered one year later, the same
students scored, on average, 40 percentile points in reading. Clearly,
the previous second-grade test scores were inflated.
3. The evaluation excluded Hope school students from the
evaluation, despite the availability of comparable test-score data.
In the end, firm conclusions cannot be drawn from the studies of
the scholarship program in Cleveland. In neither our research nor that
of the Indiana evaluation team was it possible to compare similar groups
of students by means of an RFT.
The state-funded program begun in Milwaukee in 1990 also required
that scholarships be awarded by means of a lottery, if applicants
exceeded places available. In this case, the lottery was successfully
conducted; as a result, data are available from an RFT for the first
four years after the program was started (school years 1990-91 to
1994-95). Unfortunately, no data are available after 1995.
The original evaluation of the Milwaukee choice program did not
carefully analyze the data from the randomized field trial but instead
compared students from low-income families with a cross section of
public school students whose parents were motivated enough to return a
mailed questionnaire. Although this research reported no systematic
achievement effects of enrollment in a private school, its findings are
problematic because the study compared choice students with
public-school students enjoying much more advantaged families.
When these data were released to the general public, my colleagues
and I analyzed the data from the RFT. Although the data collection was
less complete in Milwaukee than in New York City, making the findings
less definitive, they are nonetheless of interest. We found that
enrollment in the program had about the same modest effects for all
students (regardless of grade) during the first year of the program,
just as was observed in New York City. But we also found that choice
students scored much higher in years three and four. The differences in
these years were as much as one quarter of a standard deviation in
reading and one third of a standard deviation in mathematics. Once
again, these gains are large enough that, if similar gains are made in
the remaining years of education, they have the potential of bringing
minority students up to the level currently achieved by white students.
That choice students did not demonstrate improved performance until
the third and fourth years is quite consistent with a common-sense
understanding of the educational process. Choice schools are not magic
bullets that transform children overnight. It takes time to adjust to a
new teaching and learning environment. The disruption of switching
schools and adjusting to new routines and expectations may hinder
improvement in test scores in the first year or two of being in a choice
school. Educational benefits accumulate and multiply with the passage of
time. As Indianapolis choice parent Barbara Lewis explains the process:
"I must admit there was a period of transition, culture shock you
might call it. He had to get used to the discipline and the homework. .
. . But Alphonso began to learn about learning, to respect the kids
around him and be respected, to learn about citizenship, discipline, and
doing your lessons. . . . My son has blossomed into an honor roll student."
Note to Government: More Choice
School choice programs are too recent to provide information on
their effects on college attendance, though the private school choice
program in Milwaukee (PAVE) reports that 75 percent of those who have
graduated from high school have gone on to college. More systematic
information on the effects of attendance at a Catholic high school are
contained in a recent University of Chicago analysis of the National
Longitudinal Survey of Youth, conducted by the Department of Education,
a survey of over 12,000 young people. Students from all racial and
ethnic groups are more likely to go to college if they attend a Catholic
school, but the effects are the greatest for urban minorities. The
probability of graduating from college rises from 11 to 27 percent, if
such a student attends a Catholic high school.
The University of Chicago study confirms results from two other
analyses that show positive effects for low-income and minority students
of attendance at Catholic schools on high school completion and college
enrollment. As one researcher summarized one of these studies, it
"indicates a substantial private school advantage in terms of
completing high school and enrolling in college, both very important
events in predicting future income and well-being. Moreover . . . the
effects were most pronounced for students with achievement test scores
in the bottom half of the distribution."
All in all the evidence that school choice enhances the achievement
of low-income students has now become quite substantial. Although
additional RFTs are desirable, the results from the first year of the
New York City evaluation suggest that, at least for children in grades
four and five, there are clear benefits for low-income minority students
that come from attendance in private schools.
The results from New York tend to confirm findings from a wide
variety of previous studies that used less definitive research methods.
Only time will tell if the choice students in this program score much
higher in later years as they did in Milwaukee.
If Congress regards the research evidence sufficient to justify the
$1.1 billion federal intervention to reduce class size appropriated in
1998, then the evidence is equally sufficient to justify comparable
state and federal expenditures on school-choice experiments.
Paul E. Peterson is the Henry Lee Shattuck Professor of Government
and the director of the Program on Education Policy and Governance at
Harvard University.