Intelligence: a measure of human capital in nations.
Meisenberg, Gerhard ; Lynn, Richard
Japan is a rich country, and Nigeria is a poor country. There is no
lack of explanations for this discrepancy. Some authors have offered
geography as an ultimate explanation for economic disparities between
countries and world regions (Diamond, 1997; Hibbs & Olsson, 2004;
Nordhaus, 2006). Everything else being equal, countries with greater
natural resources and greater proximity to world markets should be
richer. Nigeria has more natural resources than Japan and is closer to
the old industrial centers of Europe. Therefore Nigeria should be richer
than Japan.
History and culture fare not much better than geography as
explanations for macroeconomic trends and developmental disparities. For
example, the backwardness of African countries today has been blamed on
the trans-Atlantic slave trade of the 18th and early 19th centuries
(Nunn, 2008). This, however, begs the question of why Europeans enslaved
Africans but Africans did not enslave Europeans.
Economic institutions are a more proximal explanation for worldwide
economic disparities. For example, the current poverty of formerly rich
countries has been blamed on Europeans who "introduced or
maintained already-existing extractive institutions to force the local
population to work in mines and plantations" (Acemoglu et al, 2002,
p. 1279) during the colonial age. When geography is pitted against
institutions as explanation for economic disparities in today's
world, institutions are the more immediate predictor (Easterly &
Levine, 2003; Rodrik et al, 2002).
Institutions are made by people. Therefore the immediate causes of
institutional quality and economic outcomes are to be sought in the
physical, cognitive or attitudinal traits of the human actors. According
to this view, Japan is rich and Nigeria is poor because the Japanese
possess more "human capital" than the Nigerians. The
importance of human capital at the country level is supported by the
observation of large differences in labor productivity between countries
(Hall & Jones, 1999).
Human capital includes both cognitive and non-cognitive resources.
Value systems have been stressed by many writers, from Max Weber's
(1930) "spirit of capitalism" to Gregory Clark's notion
that the industrial revolution was triggered not by new incentives, but
by people responding differently to incentives that had been in place
for ages (Clark, 2007). There is ample evidence for associations of
non-cognitive traits with prosperity and economic growth (e.g., Knack
& Keefer, 1997; McCauley et al, 1999), but the direction of
causality is difficult to ascertain.
In addition, the measurement of non-cognitive traits is fraught with conceptual and psychometric ambiguities. Perhaps for this reason,
cognitive traits have received the greater attention, as is evidenced by
the inclusion of measures for literacy, school enrolment and related
measures in the Human Development Reports of the United Nations, the
World Development Indicators of the World Bank, and similar
compilations. Primary school enrolment (Sala-iMartin et al, 2004),
secondary school enrolment (Mankiw et al, 1992), and the average years
of schooling of the adult population (Barro & Lee, 1993) have all
been proposed or used as measures of human capital. Many authors
consider the introduction of mass education an essential condition for
economic growth (e.g., Easterlin, 1981).
However, educational degrees and years spent in school are not
directly relevant for economic outcomes because they do not guarantee
that children have successfully acquired important cognitive or
non-cognitive skills. Attained skills, abilities and knowledge are
measured more directly in international school achievement tests that
assess skills in curricular subjects such as mathematics, science,
and/or reading.
Therefore the results of school achievement tests have been
proposed as measures of human capital (Lee & Barro, 1997), and they
have been used for the country-level prediction of economic growth
(Hanushek & Kimko, 2000; Hanushek & WoBmann, 2007, 2009;
Rindermann, 2008a), democracy and rule of law (Rindermann, 2008b) and
the spread of AIDS (Rindermann & Meisenberg, 2009). In an extension
of this approach, national differences in the high reaches of the
ability distribution have been implicated as especially important for
economic wealth, patents, democracy, and rule of law (Rindermann et al,
2009).
Intelligence tests ("IQ tests") provide an alternative
measure of cognitive skills. Data quality varies widely. In many
countries, major intelligence tests have been standardized with
representative population samples. In other cases, IQ tests have been
applied to convenience samples that may or may not be representative of
the general population. The results of these studies have been surveyed
in Lynn & Vanhanen (2002, 2006) and Lynn (2006). IQ was found to be
a reasonably close correlate of GDP (Lynn & Vanhanen, 2002) and
several other economic outcomes (Lynn & Vanhanen, 2006). Also at the
level of population groups within countries, higher IQ is related to
more education, greater prosperity, less criminal involvement, and
reduced fertility (Lynn, 2008a).
IQ and school achievement are closely related. At the individual
level within countries, correlations between IQ tests and school
achievement tests are typically between 0.5 and 0.7 (Jencks, 1972;
Jensen, 1998; Mackintosh, 1998), but can be as high as 0.8 (Deary et al,
2006). At the country level, correlations between the results of IQ
tests and scholastic assessments are in the vicinity of 0.9 (Lynn &
Mikk, 2007; Lynn et al., 2007; Lynn & Meisenberg, 2010). Therefore
the two types of test appear to measure identical or closely related
constructs.
The study of cognitive differences between countries is a rapidly
developing field. The present paper updates the country-level data for
IQ and school achievement. It further investigates the relationship
between these two cognitive measures, and compares the correlates of IQ
with those of school achievement. We finally integrate both types of
data into a composite measure of human capital for 168 countries, and
demonstrate some economic, political and cultural correlates of this
measure. Thus we provide an overview of the range of country-level
traits that appear to be related more closely to intelligence than to
per-capita GDP and other conditions.
Methods and Data Sources
1. International school assessments General strategy
The most important international school assessment studies are
TIMSS (Trends in International Mathematics and Science Study) and PISA (Program for International Student Assessment). TIMSS assessments of 8th
graders in mathematics and science were conducted 1995, 1999, 2003 and
2007, and PISA assessments of 13-year-olds were done 2000, 2003, 2006
and 2009. 74 countries participated in at least one TIMSS assessment,
and 18 participated in all four. 65 countries participated in at least
one PISA assessment, and 30 participated in all four. 47 countries have
data for both TIMSS and PISA, and 92 have data for either TIMSS or PISA
or both.
Several other international student assessments have been
performed, some dating back to the 1970s. More recently, regional
scholastic assessments have been performed in the less developed
countries of Latin America and Africa. Together with TIMSS and PISA,
these additional sources provide quantitative data for 131 countries.
Because TIMSS and PISA appear to be the most reliable assessments,
and because adult intelligence is expected to be more closely related to
cognitive ability at age 13 or 14 than at younger ages, we adopted the
strategy of calculating the average of PISA and 8th-grade TIMSS scores
for those countries participating in at least one assessment. Missing
data were extrapolated into this data set from the other assessments,
producing a total of 131 countries with information about scholastic
achievement.
TIMSS and PISA
TIMSS is organized by the IEA (International Association for the
Evaluation of Educational Achievement), and assessments are performed in
a 4-year cycle. Tests of mathematics and science are administered in
grades 4 and 8, with a larger number of countries participating in grade
8 than in grade 4. The results are publicly available at:
http://timss.bc.edu/timss2003.html, and:
http://nces.ed.gov/timss/tables07.asp. Further information is
available in Gonzalez et al (2004), Martin et al (2004, 2008), and
Mullis et al (2004, 2008).
PISA is organized by the OECD in a 3-year cycle. Children aged 13
are tested in mathematics, science and reading. The results are
available at:
http://www.oecd.org/dataoecd/30/18/39703566.pdf,
http://nces.ed.gov/pubs2002/2002116.pdf, and:
http://pisacountry.acer.edu.au/.
Both TIMSS and PISA are graded with methods based on item response
theory, which models student proficiency as a latent variable. In both
assessments the results are published separately for each tested subject
and are reported on a 500/100 scale. In TIMSS the mean score of 500 is
the average of the countries participating in the first TIMSS assessment
in 1995, and in PISA it is the average of the participating OECD
countries. The individual-level, within-country standard deviation is
about 85 in TIMSS and 95 in PISA.
Within each assessment the scores of the different subjects were
highly correlated at the country level, as expected from the results of
earlier studies (Rindermann, 2006, 2007). They were averaged separately
for each of the four TIMSS and four PISA assessments. Minor trend
adjustments were made based on the countries participating in TIMSS 2007
and PISA 2009, respectively. For example, 27 countries participated both
in TIMSS 1995 and TIMSS 2007. Mean and standard deviation of these 27
countries in TIMSS 1995 were adjusted to the same mean and standard
deviation that these countries had in TIMSS 2007, and all other
countries in TIMSS 1995 were adjusted accordingly. The averaged TIMSS
scores and the averaged PISA scores were brought to the same mean and
standard deviation of 500 [+ or -] 50 for those 47 countries that
participated in at least one TIMSS and one PISA assessment. These
adjusted scores were averaged based on the number of assessments in
which each country participated. Regressions in which the score was
predicted by IQ and age at testing (which varied slightly among
countries) showed no consistent age-effect in either TIMSS or PISA.
These scores are somewhat biased measures for the average cognitive
ability in the country because they measure only the proficiency of
children who are still in school in grade 8 (TIMSS) or at age 13 (PISA).
Therefore the proportion of children still in school in grade 8 or at
age 13 was estimated from the Barro-Lee data set for years of schooling
obtained from:
http://www.barrolee.com/data/dataexp.htm. The TIMSS and PISA scores
were adjusted assuming that those not in school would score 40 points
(about 7 IQ points) lower than those in school.
Other assessments scored with methods of item response theory
Several assessments other than TIMSS and PISA were graded with
modern methods of item response theory and published on a 500/100 scale.
Those used for the extrapolation of data points missing in the original
TIMSS/PISA data set were:
TIMSS 2007, 4h grade included Yemen, which did not participate in
any of the PISA and 8th-grade TIMSS assessments.
PIRLS Reading, 2001 was organized by the IEA to assess reading
literacy of 4th-graders. 34 countries participated. Data are available
at http://nces.ed.gov/surveys/pirls/. This study provides data for
Belize.
IAE Reading 1991 assessed reading literacy of 9 and 14 year olds in
30 countries. The results are published in Elley (1992). This assessment
provided data for Venezuela at age 9 and 14, and Nigeria and Zimbabwe at
age 14.
The raw scores were adjusted for age at testing in those
assessments that showed non-trivial age effects. This was followed by
adjustment for the approximate proportion in school at the age/grade of
testing. To make the scores numerically equivalent to the TIMSS/PISA
scale, the mean and standard deviation for each assessment were
equalized with those of the TIMSS/PISA score for the countries
participating in both kinds of assessment.
Older studies
Some older scholastic assessments are available for which the
results were published as "percent correct" scores:
IAEP Mathematics 1990/91 assessed mathematics in 13-yearolds. 19
countries participated, of which Mozambique did not participate in TIMSS
or PISA. Results are published in Lapointe (1992).
The Second International Science Study 1983/84 tested children from
23 countries at age 14 and from 17 countries at age 10. The age 10 test
provided data for Nigeria, and the age 14 test for Nigeria, Papua New
Guinea and Zimbabwe. The results are published in Keeves, 1992.
The Second International Mathematics Study 1981 was organized by
the IEA to assess mathematics in 13-year-olds. 17 countries
participated, including Nigeria and Swaziland. The raw scores are
published in Medrich & Griffith (1992).
The results of these assessments show nonlinear relationships with
IQ and TIMSS-PISA score, and therefore nonlinear model fitting was
employed after adjustments for age (if applicable) and proportion in
school had been made.
Regional assessments
The SACMEQ (Southern and Eastern Africa Consortium for Monitoring
Educational Quality) assessments of 2000/01 and 2007 tested mathematics
and reading of 6th graders in the countries of South and East Africa.
The results are available at http://www.sacmeq.org/indicators.htm and:
http://www.sacmeq.org/downloads/sacmeqIII/WD01_SACMEQ_I
II_Results_Pupil_Achievement.pdf. The math-reading average was used for
each assessment, and adjustments were made for the proportion of
children in school in grade 6. Correlations with IQ were .570 with
SACMEQ II (p = .067) and .722 with SACMEQ III (p = .012) for the 11
countries having both measures. SACMEQ provides data for Kenya, Lesotho,
Malawi, Mauritius, Mozambique, Namibia, Seychelles, Swaziland, Tanzania,
Uganda, Zambia, Zanzibar and Zimbabwe. Results are published on a
500/100 scale.
Only two of the countries in SACMEQ (Botswana, South Africa)
participated also in TIMSS, and none in PISA. For these two countries,
the SACMEQ scores were 177.1 points (SACMEQ II) and 184.5 points (SACMEQ
IIII) higher than the TIMSS/PISA scores (weighted by the number of times
they participated in TIMSS). SACMEQ scores for all participating
countries were adjusted accordingly, without changing the standard
deviation.
The PASEC (Programme d'Analyse des Systemes Educatifs de la
CONFEMEN) assessments for Francophone African countries (Conference des
Ministres, 2008) include 11 countries, none of whom have participated in
TIMSS or PISA. French and mathematics were tested in 5th grade, but only
the math scores were used because of large differences between countries
in the proportion of children speaking French at home. Only 5 of the
countries have an IQ score, and the PASEC-IQ correlation is a
non-significant .254. For scaling, the means and standard deviations of
the PASEC math scores were brought to the same mean and standard
deviation with IQ for the 5 countries having both measures, followed by
rescaling from the 100/15 IQ metric to the 500/100 school assessment
metric and adjustment for the proportion of children still in school at
5th grade. This study provides data for Benin, Burkina Faso, Burundi,
Cameroon, Chad, Comoros, Congo (B), Cote d'Ivoire, Gabon,
Madagascar and Senegal.
The 1999 MLA (Monitoring Learning Achievement) assessments of
UNESCO/UNICEF assessed reading/writing, mathematics and life skills in
4th grade. Results are reported for 11 African countries in Chinapah et
al (2000). 3 of these countries (Botswana, Morocco, Tunisia) had
participated in TIMSS and/or PISA. Correlations with IQ were .006 for
life skills, .767 for literacy (p = .010) and .639 for numeracy (p =
.047) for the 10 countries having both kinds of measure. Consequently, a
composite of literacy and numeracy was used as the measure of school
achievement. This composite was scaled to the 500/100 metric. For those
9 countries in MLA that also had school achievement data from other
sources, the standard deviation of the MLA measure was adjusted to the
standard deviation of the other measures. This measure was adjusted for
the proportion of children in school in grade 4, and finally adjusted to
the TIMSS/PISA mean for the 3 countries with scores from TIMSS and/or
PISA. MLA provides data for Madagascar, Malawi, Mali, Mauritius, Niger,
Senegal, Uganda and Zambia.
SERCE (Second Regional Comparative and Explanatory Study) was
performed in 16 Latin American countries between 2002 and 2008. Children
in grades 3 and 6 were tested in mathematics and reading. Results are
published on a 500/100 scale (Valdes et al, 2008). The average of the
6th grade mathematics and reading scores was used. This measure was
adjusted for the proportion of children in school. The final measure was
created by adjusting the SERCE scores to the mean and standard deviation
of the TIMSS/PISA scores for the 9 countries having both measures. The
SERCE scores correlate at r = .965 (p<.001) with TIMSS-PISA (N = 9)
and r = .442 (p = .131) with IQ (N = 13). They provide data for Costa
Rica, Cuba, the Dominican Republic, Ecuador, Guatemala, Nicaragua and
Paraguay.
Additional sources
The only school achievement data for India are from the First
International Science Study in 1970 (Comber & Keeves, 1973), and a
recent study with a subset of the 2007 TIMSS study in the states of
Rajasthan and Orissa (Das & Zajonc, 2010). The school achievement
score of India was averaged from these two sources.
2. IQ
Compilations of national IQs have been published by Lynn (2006) and
Lynn & Vanhanen (2002, 2006). The current data set is based on Lynn
& Vanhanen (2006), with amendments and additions published in Lynn
(2010). Measured IQs are available for 136 countries. The IQ for
Northern Ireland is taken from Lynn (1979).
3. Estimates of data quality
The quality of the IQ data was defined based on the number of
independent studies available for each country and the total sample size
in all studies combined. The following scores were given for total
sample size:
1 <200
2 200-500
3 500-999
4 1000-1999
5 2000-4999
6 5000-9999
7 >10,000
The IQ quality score was calculated by adding this score to the
number of independent IQ studies available for the country, with the
maximum capped at 25.
For school achievement, countries were awarded 2 points for each
PISA or 8th-grade TIMSS study in which they participated. Those that did
not participate in PISA or 8th-grade TIMSS were awarded 1 point for each
of the other assessments in which they participated. The maximum score
was 16 for countries participating in all four PISA and all four TIMSS
studies.
4. Properties of IQ and school achievement compared.
The correlation between IQ and school achievement is .889 for the
99 countries having both measures, and both have virtually the same
correlates (Table 1). However, the relationship between the
within-country and between-country standard deviations is different. For
IQ, the within-country standard deviation is 15 by definition, at least
for Britain. For school achievement, the individual-level standard
deviation in TIMSS is set at 100 for those countries that participated
in the 1995 assessment, and in PISA it is 100 for the participating OECD
countries. Within-country standard deviations in Britain and other
advanced nations are approximately 85 in TIMSS and 95 in PISA, and these
standard deviations are not changed substantially during the scaling
procedure. Therefore school achievement was scaled directly to the IQ
metric, assigning a score of 100 to Britain and assuming a
within-country standard deviation of 90 for school achievement:
SA absolute = (SchAch - 521.9) x 15/90 + 100
The between-country standard deviation is 38.6% higher for this
measure of school achievement (SA absolute in the appendix) than for IQ:
15.18 versus 10.95 (N = 99). The discrepancy is best attributed to the
generally low quality of schooling in low-scoring countries, which
depresses school achievement to a greater extent than IQ. In this sense,
school achievement is more "culturally biased" than IQ.
5. Calculation of a measure of "human capital."
For construction of a combined measure of (cognitive) human
capital, school achievement was not scaled directly to the IQ metric. We
instead adjusted the international mean and standard deviation for
school achievement to those of IQ, based on the 99 countries having both
measures (SA relative in the appendix). The resulting scores were
averaged with weighting for data quality. For countries having only IQ
data or only school achievement data, these scores were used (Human
capital in the appendix).
In all there are 101 countries and territories (including England
and Scotland in addition to the United Kingdom) whose human capital
score is based on school achievement and IQ. For 37 countries it is
based on IQ alone, and for 30 on school achievement alone. Scores for 28
additional countries and territories were estimated from the scores of
neighboring countries with similar population, culture, and economic
development, as described for IQ in Lynn & Vanhanen (2002, 2006).
Provincial data were used in two cases: The estimate for Afghanistan was
derived from the measured IQ in the Northwest Frontier Province of
Pakistan (Ahmad et al., 2008), which is inhabited by ethnic Pashtuns
living under conditions similar to Pashtuns in Afghanistan; and the
estimate for Bhutan was the average of Nepal, India and Tibet (Lynn,
2008b). Estimates are included in the last column of the appendix as
numbers in parentheses.
6. Other country-level measures
lgGDP is the logarithm of gross domestic product adjusted for
purchasing power, averaged for the years 1995-2005. Data are from the
Penn World Tables (Heston et al., 2009). Missing data were extrapolated
into this data set from the World Development Indicators of the World
Bank. The logarithmic transformation was used because of the highly
skewed nature of GDP worldwide, which approximates to a normal
distribution in the logarithmic form.
Education measures length of schooling for adults 25+ years old,
based on the Barro-Lee data set for 143 countries
(http://www.barrolee.com/data/dataexp.htm). Missing data points were
extrapolated from World Bank and United Nations sources.
No Corruption was averaged from Transparency International's
Corruption Perception Index for the years 1998-2003
(http://www.transparency.org) and the "no corruption" index
from the Governance Indicators of the World Bank for 1996 or earliest
available date:
(http://info.worldbank.org/governance/wgi/mc_countries.asp).
Economic Freedom was averaged from the unrotated first factors of
maximum-likelihood factor analyses of areas 2-5 of the Fraser
Institute's Economic Freedom Index for the periods 1995-2005
(Gwartney et al., 2010), and domains 1, 2, and 5-8 of the Heritage
Foundation Index for 1995-2005 (http://www.heritage.org/research/). This
measure indexes the extent of business regulation and red tape.
Big Government is calculated from area 1 of the Fraser
Institute's Economic Freedom Index for the periods 1995-2005 (size
of government), and domains 3 and 4 of the Heritage Foundation Index for
1995-2005 (fiscal freedom and government spending). These measures are
factorially and conceptually different from the other components of the
Fraser Institute and Heritage Foundation indices for "economic
freedom."
Gini index: The primary data source is the World Income Inequality
Database (WIID2a) of the United Nations University at
www.wider.unu.edu/wiid/wiid.htm, as described in Meisenberg, 2007.
Missing data points were extrapolated from the World Bank's World
Development Indicators of 2005, the Human Development Report 2005
(United Nations, 2005), and the CIA's World Factbook of 2009.
Political freedom is the average of political rights + civil
liberties from Freedom House at:
http://www.freedomhouse.org/research/freeworld, average 1985-2005.
Democracy is defined as Vanhanen's democracy index (average
1985-2004), from the Finnish Social Science Data Archive at:
http://www.fsd.uta.fi/english/data/catalogue/FSD1289/.
Suicide is the average of the standardized male and female suicide
rates, average of available years since 1985, reported by the World
Health Organization at:
http://www.who.int/mental_health/prevention/suicide/country_rep
orts/en/index.html.
Life expectancy is life expectancy at birth for the years
2000-2005, as reported in the 2005 Human Development Report (United
Nations, 2005).
Infant mortality is from the 2004 Human Development Report (United
Nations, 2004).
TFR is the Total Fertility Rate, averaged for the years 1990 to
2005, from the World Development Indicators of the World Bank.
Religiosity is the average of three measures: (1) the average of
four questionnaire items about belief in God and emotional involvement
with religion from the World Values Survey, 1981-2008 combined data file
v.20090901, 2009, available at www.worldvaluessurvey.org; (2) A question
about the importance of religion from the Gallup World Poll:
(https://worldview.gallup.com/signin/login.aspx?ReturnUrl=%2f),
accessed February 10, 2011; and (3) reverse scored atheism rate
according to Zuckerman (2005). Missing data points were extrapolated
from the available measure(s).
Happiness is a question in the World Values survey (Are you
happy?).
Life satisfaction is averaged from a question about overall life
satisfaction in the World Values Survey, and a question about the best
possible life (Cantrill ladder) in the Gallup World Poll, 2006-2009
average. These two measures correlate at r = .830.
Properties of the measures
Table 1 shows the correlations of alternative human capital
measures and of log-transformed GDP with a number of country-level
variables. A number of observations can be made:
1. All "development indicators," including
log-transformed GDP, education, school achievement, intelligence,
freedom and democracy, form a positive manifold. However, the
correlations between school achievement and IQ are higher than any of
the other correlations in the table. This high correlation justifies the
averaging of these two cognitive measures into a single measure of
(cognitive) human capital, or intelligence.
2. The correlates of IQ and school achievement are very similar. In
addition to their high intercorrelation, this observation provides
further justification for averaging of these two measures into a
combined measure of cognitive human capital, or intelligence.
3. The cognitive measures are nearly as highly correlated with
lgGDP as with education (measured as years in school). This shows that
the cognitive outcomes are more than the predictable outcomes of
prolonged schooling.
4. Economic growth is related more closely to the cognitive
measures than to education. This suggests a causal role of intelligence
for economic growth, confirming earlier reports about such a
relationship (Hanushek & Wossmann, 2007, 2009; Jones &
Schneider, 2006; Meisenberg, in press; Weede, 2004; Weede & Kampf,
2002).
5. In addition to economic growth, variables that are related more
closely to intelligence than to length of schooling and lgGDP include
the Gini index of income inequality, suicide, life expectancy, total
fertility rate, and religiosity. Other outcomes, however, including
corruption, economic freedom and life satisfaction, are related more
closely to education and/or lgGDP than to intelligence.
Tables 2 and 3 show the results of regression models in which
various outcomes are predicted by the combined measure of human capital
(average of IQ and school achievement, without estimates), schooling,
log-transformed GDP, and a dummy variable for countries with communist
history. Because small countries are more likely than larger ones to be
atypical in many ways, the regressions were limited to countries with a
population of more than 250,000.
The results of Tables 2 and 3 confirm that some outcomes are
related more closely to intelligence than to schooling or lgGDP. These
include economic growth (positive), Gini index (negative), suicide
(positive), life expectancy (positive), and religiosity (negative). In
this larger sample, the total fertility rate is predicted to similar
extents by intelligence, schooling, and lgGDP.
Other variables are related more closely to education or lgGDP than
to intelligence. They are likely to correlate with intelligence mainly
because intelligence correlates highly with education and lgGDP. Length
of schooling (in addition to communist history) appears to be the most
important predictor of democracy, economic freedom and political
freedom. Outcomes that clearly are related most closely to lgGDP (in
addition to communist history) rather than intelligence are corruption
and life satisfaction.
Proposed uses of human capital measures
Country-level data on school achievement and IQ can be used for a
number of purposes:
1. Studies of educational quality. Although our measure of
schooling is about equally related to IQ and school achievement,
prolonged, intensified, or more efficient schooling is expected to raise
school achievement to a greater extent than it raises IQ. Therefore the
difference between school achievement and IQ can be used as a measure
for the quality of the educational system. However, these studies need
to take account of the fact that the between-country standard deviation,
relative to within-country standard deviations, is greater for school
achievement than for IQ. Because this difference is best attributed to
systematic differences in schooling quality between high-scoring and
low-scoring countries, the comparison between school achievement and IQ
should be based on school achievement scaled directly to the IQ metric
(SA absolute in the appendix).
2. Studies of economic outcomes. Earlier studies have shown that
high scores on cognitive measures predict faster economic growth
(Hanushek & Kimko, 2000; Hanushek & WoBmann, 2007, 2009; Jones
and Schneider 2006; Rindermann, 2008a; Weede 2004; Weede and Kampf,
2002) and lower levels of income inequality (Meisenberg, 2007, 2008, in
press). These results are confirmed by the correlations and regression
coefficients in Tables 1 and 2. Presumably, high intelligence promotes
economic growth because technical skills and "managerial
capital" (Bruhn et al., 2010) are required for economic growth. The
reasons for the relationship of high intelligence with low income
inequality are uncertain. Possibly, high-IQ societies are more efficient
at restraining the exploitation of the poor by the rich or at
redistributing resources from the rich to the poor. Another possibility
is that high average intelligence in the population reduces income
inequality by market forces. Where cognitive skills are abundant, the
skill premium is expected to be low; and where highly skilled
individuals are rare, the skill premium is expected to be high.
3. Studies of cultural traits. Tables 1 and 3 show that high
cognitive ability is associated with lower religiosity. Conversely,
happiness and life satisfaction rise with rising prosperity, not with
rising intelligence, confirming earlier results that had been obtained
with less complete data (Meisenberg, 2004). The suicide rate is
increased by high intelligence, confirming earlier work (Voracek, 2005).
The ways in which cognitive ability interacts with these cultural and
psychological traits remain to be determined.
4. Studies of the determinants of human capital. There is no
generally accepted theory to explain why, for example, the level of
human capital (a.k.a. intelligence) is so much higher in Japan than in
Nigeria. Genetic theories have been proposed by some (e.g., Lynn, 2006).
These theories have been attacked by others (e.g., Wicherts et al.,
2010), but without convincing alternative explanations. Precise
knowledge about the current level of intelligence in different countries
is required for these investigations.
5. Studies of temporal trends. Measured intelligence has been
rising through most of the 20th century in advanced industrialized societies (Flynn, 1987). It seems to be stagnating in advanced societies
today (Cotton et al., 2005; Shayer & Ginsburg, 2009; Sundet et al.,
2004; Teasdale & Owen, 2008), but is rising in at least some of the
less developed countries (Batterjee, in press; Colom et al., 2006;
Khaleefa et al., 2009; Meisenberg et al., 2006). At this time, only the
periodic scholastic assessment tests, including PISA and TIMSS, are
sufficiently accurate to allow close tracking of cognitive skills for a
substantial number of countries on a time scale of about one decade.
Together with standardization studies of major IQ tests, these can be
used to study both the determinants of trends in cognitive ability, and
their consequences.
Appendix
Measures of human capital by country:
SchAch, school achievement on the 500/100 scale.
SA absolute, SchAch scaled to a mean of 100 and within-country
standard deviation of 15 for the United Kingdom, assuming a
within-country standard deviation of 90 for SchAch.
SA relative, SchAch scaled to the IQ metric by equalizing mean and
standard deviation with IQ.
SAq and IQq, quality of data for school achievement and IQ.
Human capital, weighted average of SA relative and IQ. Estimates
are in parentheses.
SchAch SA absolute SA relative
Afghanistan
Albania 390.7 78.1 82.9
Algeria 389.9 78.0 82.8
Andorra
Angola
Argentina 400.6 79.8 84.1
Armenia 487.7 94.3 94.5
Australia 532.5 101.8 99.9
Austria 521.0 99.9 98.5
Azerbaijan 406.6 80.8 84.8
Bahamas
Bahrain 432.0 85.0 87.8
Bangladesh
Barbados
Belarus
Belgium 526.9 100.8 99.3
Belize 339.0 69.5 76.7
Benin 264.1 57.0 67.7
Bermuda
Bhutan
Bolivia
Bosnia 464.6 90.4 91.8
Botswana 369.7 74.6 80.4
Brazil 403.4 80.2 84.4
Brunei
Bulgaria 480.4 93.1 93.7
Burkina Faso 292.2 61.7 71.0
Burundi 325.7 67.3 75.1
Cambodia
Cameroon 339.4 69.6 76.7
Canada 538.0 102.7 100.6
Cape Verde
Centr. Afr. R.
Chad 259.3 56.2 67.1
Chile 431.4 84.9 87.8
China 605.6 113.9 108.7
Hong Kong 559.3 106.2 103.1
Macau 532.5 101.8 99.9
Tibet
Colombia 395.5 78.9 83.5
Comoros 288.3 61.1 70.6
Congo (B.) 288.0 61.0 70.5
Congo (K.)
Cook Islands
Costa Rica 444.9 87.2 89.4
Cote d'Ivoire 230.9 51.5 63.7
Croatia 489.5 94.6 94.8
Cuba 500.1 96.4 96.0
Cyprus 462.8 90.2 91.5
Czech Rep. 526.8 100.8 99.2
Denmark 508.5 97.8 97.0
Djibouti
Dominica
Dominican R. 315.4 65.6 73.8
East Timor
Ecuador 349.8 71.3 78.0
Egypt 407.0 80.8 84.8
El Salvador 357.7 72.6 78.9
Equ. Guinea
Eritrea
Estonia 539.0 102.8 100.7
Ethiopia
Fiji
Finland 555.6 105.6 102.7
France 516.1 99.0 98.0
Gabon 338.8 69.5 76.6
Gambia
Georgia 420.5 83.1 86.5
Germany 516.1 99.0 98.0
Ghana 293.1 61.9 71.1
Greece 484.7 93.8 94.2
Guatemala 340.2 69.7 76.8
Guinea
Guinea-Bissao
Guyana
Haiti
Honduras
Hungary 525.2 100.6 99.0
Iceland 511.2 98.2 97.4
India 434.1 85.4 88.1
Indonesia 408.9 81.2 85.1
Iran 433.3 85.2 88.0
Iraq
Ireland 523.2 100.2 98.8
Israel 480.8 93.2 93.7
Italy 492.3 95.1 95.1
Jamaica
Japan 558.7 106.1 103.1
Jordan 438.6 86.1 88.6
Kazakhstan 406.3 80.7 84.7
Kenya 371.5 74.9 80.6
S. Korea 567.4 107.6 104.1
Kuwait 387.8 77.7 82.5
Kyrgyztan 320.4 66.4 74.4
Laos
Latvia 500.3 96.4 96.1
Lebanon 425.8 84.0 87.1
Lesotho 273.2 58.5 68.7
Liberia
Libya
Liechtenstein 536.1 102.4 100.4
Lithuania 497.0 95.8 95.7
Luxembourg 494.0 95.4 95.3
Macedonia 453.7 88.6 90.5
Madagascar 322.3 66.7 74.7
Malawi 232.1 51.7 63.8
Malaysia 499.6 96.3 96.0
Maldives
Mali 270.8 58.2 68.5
Malta 477.7 92.6 93.3
Mariana Isl.
Marshall Isl.
Mauritania
Mauritius 388.1 77.7 82.6
Mexico 428.9 84.5 87.5
Micronesia
Moldova 470.8 91.5 92.5
Mongolia
Montenegro 415.0 82.2 85.8
Morocco 367.6 74.3 80.1
Mozambique 322.9 66.8 74.7
Myanmar
Namibia 272.8 58.5 68.7
Nepal
Netherlands 539.2 102.9 100.7
Neth. Antilles
New Caledonia
New Zealand 528.5 101.1 99.4
Nicaragua 357.2 72.5 78.8
Niger 209.2 47.9 61.1
Nigeria 348.8 71.1 77.8
Norway 505.0 97.2 96.6
Oman 402.1 80.0 84.2
Pakistan
Palestine 394.0 78.7 83.3
Panama 371.0 74.9 80.5
Papua NG 486.6 94.1 94.4
Paraguay 357.0 72.51 78.8
Peru 370.7 74.8 80.5
Philippines 371.0 74.9 80.5
Poland 514.9 98.8 97.8
Portugal 487.0 94.2 94.5
Puerto Rico
Qatar 342.8 70.1 77.1
Romania 455.6 89.0 90.7
Russia 503.9 97.0 96.5
Rwanda
St. Lucia
St. Vincent
W. Samoa
Sao Tome & P.
Saudi Arabia 369.0 74.5 80.3
Senegal 289.3 61.2 70.7
Serbia & M. 457.0 89.2 90.8
Seychelles 384.7 77.1 82.2
Sierra Leone
Singapore 584.4 110.4 106.2
Slovakia 514.4 98.8 97.8
Slovenia 524.2 100.4 98.9
Solomon Isl.
Somalia
S. Africa 289.0 61.2 70.7
Spain 499.5 96.3 96.0
Sri Lanka
Sudan
Suriname
Swaziland 370.9 74.8 80.5
Sweden 519.9 99.7 98.4
Switzerland 515.8 99.0 97.9
Syria 410.9 81.5 85.3
Taiwan 566.1 107.4 104.0
Tajikistan
Tanzania 358.7 72.8 79.0
Zanzibar 308.1 64.4 73.0
Thailand 459.0 89.5 91.1
Togo
Tonga
Trinidad & T. 422.9 83.5 86.7
Tunisia 409.1 81.2 85.1
Turkey 446.2 87.4 89.5
Turkmenistan
Uganda 305.7 64.0 72.7
Ukraine 480.0 93.0 93.6
Un. Arab Em. 472.6 91.8 92.7
United K. 521.9 100.0 98.7
England 523.6 100.3 98.9
Scotland 500.7 96.5 96.1
N. Ireland
USA 512.4 98.4 97.5
Uruguay 442.2 86.7 89.1
Uzbekistan
Vanuatu
Venezuela 357.3 72.6 78.9
Vietnam
Yemen 239.2 52.9 64.7
Zambia 240.9 53.2 64.9
Zimbabwe 324.1 67.0 74.9
Human
SAq IQ IQq Capital
Afghanistan (75.0)
Albania 2 82.9
Algeria 2 82.8
Andorra (97.2)
Angola (69.9)
Argentina 4 96.0 9 92.3
Armenia 4 92.0 3 93.5
Australia 16 98.0 10 99.2
Austria 10 99.5 4 98.8
Azerbaijan 4 84.8
Bahamas (84.0)
Bahrain 4 81.0 2 85.6
Bangladesh 81.0 4 81.0
Barbados 80.0 2 80.0
Belarus (95.1)
Belgium 14 99.0 7 99.2
Belize 1 76.7
Benin 1 67.7
Bermuda 90.0 4 90.0
Bhutan (84.2)
Bolivia 87.0 6 87.0
Bosnia 2 94.0 4 93.3
Botswana 4 72.15 2 77.2
Brazil 8 87.0 13 86.0
Brunei (88.9)
Bulgaria 12 92.5 6 93.3
Burkina Faso 1 71.0
Burundi 1 75.1
Cambodia (92.0)
Cameroon 1 64.0 2 68.2
Canada 16 100.0 6 100.4
Cape Verde (76.0)
Centr. Afr. R. 64.0 5 64.0
Chad 1 67.1
Chile 8 91.0 9 89.5
China 2 105.5 16 105.9
Hong Kong 14 108.0 16 105.7
Macau 6 99.9
Tibet 92.0 2 92.0
Colombia 8 83.5 7 83.5
Comoros 1 70.6
Congo (B.) 1 73.0 7 72.7
Congo (K.) 68.0 8 68.0
Cook Islands 89.0 2 89.0
Costa Rica 1 86.0 89.4
Cote d'Ivoire 1 71.0 2 68.6
Croatia 4 99.0 7 97.5
Cuba 1 85.0 5 86.8
Cyprus 8 91.5
Czech Rep. 14 98.0 7 98.8
Denmark 10 98.0 5 97.4
Djibouti (74.8)
Dominica 73.0 5 73.0
Dominican R. 1 82.0 6 80.8
East Timor (85.4)
Ecuador 1 88.0 5 86.3
Egypt 4 81.0 5 82.7
El Salvador 2 78.9
Equ. Guinea (72.3)
Eritrea 75.5 4 75.5
Estonia 6 99.0 7 99.8
Ethiopia 68.5 8 68.5
Fiji 85.0 3 85.0
Finland 10 97.0 5 100.8
France 10 98.0 9 98.0
Gabon 1 76.6
Gambia 62.0 4 62.0
Georgia 2 86.5
Germany 10 99.0 16 98.6
Ghana 4 70.0 6 70.5
Greece 10 92.0 10 93.1
Guatemala 1 79.0 3 78.5
Guinea 66.5 6 66.5
Guinea-Bissao (66.5)
Guyana (86.0)
Haiti (70.0)
Honduras 81.0 6 81.0
Hungary 16 96.5 8 98.2
Iceland 10 101.0 4 98.4
India 2 82.0 19 82.6
Indonesia 12 87.0 8 85.8
Iran 8 83.5 9 85.6
Iraq 87.0 6 87.0
Ireland 10 92.5 10 95.7
Israel 12 95.0 14 94.4
Italy 16 97.0 13 95.9
Jamaica 71.0 12 71.0
Japan 16 105.0 20 104.1
Jordan 10 84.0 7 86.7
Kazakhstan 2 84.7
Kenya 2 74.0 11 75.0
S. Korea 16 106.0 9 104.8
Kuwait 4 86.5 9 85.3
Kyrgyztan 4 74.4
Laos 89.0 2 89.0
Latvia 14 96.1
Lebanon 4 82.0 4 84.6
Lesotho 2 68.7
Liberia (66.4)
Libya 84.6 8 84.6
Liechtenstein 8 100.4
Lithuania 12 92.0 5 94.6
Luxembourg 8 95.3
Macedonia 4 90.5
Madagascar 2 82.0 2 78.3
Malawi 3 60.0 3 61.9
Malaysia 6 88.5 7 92.0
Maldives 81.0
Mali 1 69.5 8 69.4
Malta 2 97.0 2 95.2
Mariana Isl. 81.0 2 81.0
Marshall Isl. 84.0 3 84.0
Mauritania 74.1
Mauritius 3 89.0 5 86.6
Mexico 8 88.0 8 87.7
Micronesia (84.0)
Moldova 4 92.5
Mongolia 100.0 6 100.0
Montenegro 4 85.8
Morocco 6 84.0 9 82.4
Mozambique 3 64.0 2 70.4
Myanmar (86.7)
Namibia 2 72.0 2 70.4
Nepal 78.0 4 78.0
Netherlands 12 100.0 10 100.4
Neth. Antilles 87.0 2 87.0
New Caledonia 85.0 2 85.0
New Zealand 14 99.0 11 99.3
Nicaragua 1 78.8
Niger 1 61.2
Nigeria 4 71.0 6 73.7
Norway 14 100.0 2 97.0
Oman 2 84.5 7 84.4
Pakistan 84.0 4 84.0
Palestine 4 86.0 4 84.6
Panama 2 80.5
Papua NG 1 82.5 4 84.9
Paraguay 1 84.0 6 83.3
Peru 2 85.0 9 84.2
Philippines 4 90.0 3 84.6
Poland 8 95.0 13 96.1
Portugal 10 94.5 6 94.5
Puerto Rico 83.5 8 83.5
Qatar 6 83.0 6 80.1
Romania 12 91.0 6 90.8
Russia 16 96.5 6 96.5
Rwanda 76.0 2 76.0
St. Lucia 62.0 2 62.0
St. Vincent 71.0 2 71.0
W. Samoa 88.0 5 88.0
Sao Tome & P. (70.0)
Saudi Arabia 4 79.0 6 79.5
Senegal 2 70.5 5 70.6
Serbia & M. 10 88.5 2 90.5
Seychelles 2 82.2
Sierra Leone 64.0 3 64.0
Singapore 10 108.5 5 106.9
Slovakia 12 98.0 4 97.8
Slovenia 12 96.0 7 97.8
Solomon Isl. (84.9)
Somalia (71.8)
S. Africa 6 72.0 14 71.6
Spain 14 97.0 8 96.3
Sri Lanka 79.0 2 79.0
Sudan 77.5 14 77.5
Suriname 89.0 4 89.0
Swaziland 3 80.5
Sweden 14 99.0 8 98.6
Switzerland 10 101.0 6 99.1
Syria 2 80.5 7 81.6
Taiwan 10 105.0 18 104.6
Tajikistan 79.6
Tanzania 2 72.5 8 73.8
Zanzibar 2 73.0
Thailand 12 88.0 6 90.1
Togo 69.1
Tonga 86.0 2 86.0
Trinidad & T. 2 86.7
Tunisia 12 84.0 2 84.9
Turkey 10 88.5 8 89.1
Turkmenistan (79.6)
Uganda 3 72.0 2 72.4
Ukraine 2 95.0 2 94.3
Un. Arab Em. 4 83.0 6 86.9
United K. 14 100.0 7 99.1
England 8 101.0 7 99.9
Scotland 6 97.0 7 96.6
N. Ireland 96.3
USA 16 98.0 11 97.7
Uruguay 6 96.0 2 90.8
Uzbekistan (79.6)
Vanuatu (84.0)
Venezuela 1 84.0 6 83.3
Vietnam 94.0 3 94.0
Yemen 1 83.0 6 80.4
Zambia 3 75.0 5 71.2
Zimbabwe 4 71.5 4 73.2
References
Acemoglu, Daron, S. Johnson & J.A. Robinson (2002) Reversal of
fortune: geography and institutions in the making of the modern world
income distribution. Quarterly Journal of Economics 117: 1231-1294.
Ahmad, Riaz, S.J. Khanum, Z. Riaz & R. Lynn (2008) Gender
difference in means and variance on the Standard Progressive Matrices in
Pakistan. Mankind Quarterly 49: 50-57.
Barro, Robert J. & W. Lee (1993) International comparisons of
educational attainment. Journal of Monetary Economics 32: 363-394.
Batterjee, Adel A. (in press) Intelligence and education: the Saudi
case. Mankind Quarterly.
Bruhn, Miriam, D. Karlan & A. Schoar (2010) What capital is
missing in developing countries? American Economic Review: Papers and
Proceedings 100: 629-633.
Chinapah, Vinayagum, E.L.M. H'ddigui, A. Kanjee, W. Falayajo,
C.O. Fomba, O. Hamissou, A. Rafalimanana & A. Byamugisha (2000) With
Africa for Africa. Towards Quality Education for All. Pretoria: Human
Sciences Research Council.
Clark, Gregory (2007) A Farewell to Alms. A Brief Economic History
of the World. Princeton: Princeton University Press.
Colom, Roberto, C.E. Flores-Mendoza & F.J. Abad (2006)
Generational changes on the Draw-a-Man Test: a comparison of Brazilian
urban and rural children tested in 1930, 2002 and 2004. Journal of
Biosocial Science 35: 33-39.
Comber, L.C. & J.P. Keeves (1973) Science Education in Nineteen
Countries. New York: Wiley.
Conference des Ministres de l'Education des pays ayant le
francais en partage (2008) Synthese des Resultats PASEC VII, VIII et IX.
Available at: http://www.confemen.org/IMG/pdf/Synthese_PASEC_VII_VIII_IX_f inal.pdf.
Cotton, Sue M., P.M. Kiely, D.P. Crewther, B. Thomson, R. Laycock
& S.G. Crewther (2005) A normative and reliability study for the
Raven's Coloured Progressive Matrices for primary school aged
children from Victoria, Australia. Personality and Individual
Differences 39: 647-659.
Das, Jishnu & T. Zajonc (2010) India shining and Bharat
drowning: comparing two Indian states to the worldwide distribution in
mathematics achievement. Journal of Development Economics 92: 175-187.
Deary, Ian J., S. Strand, P. Smith & C. Fernandes (2006)
Intelligence and educational achievement. Intelligence 35: 13-21.
Diamond, Jared (1997) Guns, Germs and Steel: The Fates of Human
Societies. New York: Norton.
Easterlin, Richard A. (1981) Why isn't the whole world
developed? Journal of Economic History 41: 1-19.
Easterly, William & R. Levine (2003) Tropics, germs, and crops:
how endowments influence economic development. Journal of Monetary
Economics 50: 3-39.
Elley, Warwick B. (1992) How in the World Do Students Read? IEA
Study of Reading Literacy. International Association for the Evaluation
of Educational Achievement, Report No. ISBN-92-9121-002-3. Accessed 15
June 2009 at: http://eric.ed.gov/ERICDocs/data/ericdocs2sql/content/storage_01/00 00019b/80/13/07/42.pdf
Flynn, J.R. (1987) Massive IQ gains in 14 nations: what IQ tests
really measure. Psychological Bulletin 101: 171-191.
Gonzalez, Eugenio J., J. Galia, A. Arora, E. Erberber & D.
Diaconu (2004) Reporting student achievement in mathematics and science.
In: M.O Martin, I.V.S. Mullis & S.J. Chrostowski (eds): TIMSS 2003
Technical Report, pp. 274-307. Chestnut Hill (MA): TIMSS & PIRLS
International Study Center. Accessed 9 June 2009 at:
http://timss.bc.edu/timss2003i/technicalD.html
Gwartney, James D., J.C. Hall & R. Lawson (2010) Economic
Freedom of the World: 2010 Annual Report. Vancouver (BC): Fraser
Institute. Data retrieved from www.freetheworld.com.
Hall, Robert E. & C.I. Jones (1999) Why do some countries
produce so much more output per worker than others? Quarterly Journal of
Economics 114: 83-116.
Hanushek, Eric A. & D.D. Kimko (2000) Schooling, labor-force
quality, and the growth of nations. American Economic Review 90:
1184-1208.
Hanushek, Eric A. & L. WoBmann (2007) Education quality and
economic growth. Working paper available at:
http://edpro.stanford.edu/hanushek/admin/pages/files/uploads/Edu_Q
uality_Economic_Growth-1.pdf
Hanushek, Eric A. & L. WoBmann (2009) Do better schools lead to
more growth? Cognitive skills, economic outcomes, and causation. IZA Discussion Papers No. 4575.
Heston, Alan, R. Summers & B. Aten (2009) Penn World Table
Version 6.3. Center for International Comparisons of Production, Income
and Prices at the University of Pennsylvania, accessed Dec 15, 2010, at:
http://pwt.econ.upenn.edu/php_site/pwt63/pwt63_retrieve.php.
Hibbs, Douglas A. Jr. & O. Olsson (2004) Geography,
biogeography, and why some countries are rich and others are poor.
Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA 101: 3715-3720.
Jencks, Christopher (1972) Inequality: A Reassessment of the
Effects of Family and Schooling in America. New York: Basic Books.
Jensen, Arthur R. (1998) The g Factor: The Science of Mental
Ability. Westport (CT): Praeger.
Jones, Garett & W.J. Schneider (2006) Intelligence, human
capital, and economic growth: a Bayesian averaging of classical
estimates (BACE) approach. Journal of Economic Growth 11: 71-93.
Keeves, John P. (1992) Learning Science in a Changing World.
Cross-National Studies of Science Achievement: 1970 to 1984.
International Association for the Evaluation of Educational Achievement
Report No. ISBN-92-9121001-3. Accessed 14 June 2009 at:
http://eric.ed.gov/ERICDocs/data/ericdocs2sql/content_storage_01/00
00019b/80/15/2b/8d.pdf
Khaleefa, Omar, A. Sulman & R. Lynn (2009) An increase of
intelligence in Sudan, 1987-2007. Journal of Biosocial Science 41:
279-283.
Knack, Stephen & P. Keefer (1997) Does social capital have an
economic payoff? A cross-country investigation. Quarterly Journal of
Economics 112: 1251-1288.
Lapointe, Archie E. (1992) Learning Mathematics. Educational
Testing Service Report No. ETS22-CAEP-01; ISBN-0-88685-120-3. Princeton
(NJ): ETS. Accessed 16 June 2009 at:
http://eric.ed.gov/ERICDocs/data/ericdocs2sql/content_storage_01/00
00019b/80/24/21/ac.pdf
Lee, Jong-Wha & R.J. Barro (1997) Schooling quality in a
cross-section of countries. NBER Working Papers No. 6198.
Lynn, Richard (1979) The social ecology of intelligence in the
British Isles. British Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology
1979:18,1-12.
Lynn, Richard (2006) Race Differences in Intelligence. An
Evolutionary Analysis. Augusta (GA): Washington Summit.
Lynn, Richard (2008a) The Global Bell Curve. Race, IQ, and
Inequality Worldwide. Augusta (GA): Washington Summit.
Lynn, Richard (2008b) IQ and mathematics ability of Tibetans and
Han Chinese. Mankind Quarterly 48: 505-510.
Lynn, Richard (2010) National IQs updated for 41 nations. Mankind
Quarterly 50: 275-296.
Lynn, Richard & T. Vanhanen (2002) IQ and the wealth of
nations. Westport (CT): Praeger.
Lynn, Richard & T. Vanhanen (2006) IQ and global inequality.
Augusta (GA): Washington Summit.
Lynn, Richard & J. Mikk (2007) National differences in
intelligence and educational attainment. Intelligence 35: 115-121.
Lynn, Richard, G. Meisenberg, J. Mikk & A. Williams (2007)
National IQs predict differences in scholastic achievement in 67
countries. Journal of Biosocial Science 39: 861-874.
Lynn, Richard & G. Meisenberg (2010) National IQs calculated
and validated for 108 nations. Intelligence 38: 353-360.
Mackintosh, Nicholas J. (1998) IQ and Human Intelligence. Oxford:
Oxford University Press.
Mankiw, N. Gregory, D. Romer & D. Weil (1992) A contribution to
the empirics of economic growth. Quarterly Journal of Economics 107:
407-438.
Martin, Michael O., I.V.S. Mullis, E.J. Gonzales & S.J.
Chrostowski (2004) TIMSS 2003 International Science Report. Chestnut
Hill (MA): IEA. Accessed 23 Jan. 2007 at:
http://timss.bc.edu/timss2003i/intl_reports.html
Martin, Michael O., I.V.S. Mullis & P. Foy (2008) TIMSS 2007
Science Report. Chestnut Hill (MA): TIMSS & PIRLS International
Study Center. Accessed 9 June 2009 at:
http://timss.bc.edu/TIMSS2007/intl_reports.html
McCauley, Clark, V. Ottati & Y.-T. Lee (1999) National
differences in economic growth: the role of personality and culture. In:
Y.-T. Lee, C.R. McCauley & J.G. Draguns (eds): Personality and
Person Perception Across Cultures. Mahwah (NJ): Erlbaum.
Medrich, Elliott A. & J.E. Griffith (1992) International
Mathematics and Science Assessment: What Have We Learned? National
Center for Education Statistics Research and Development Report.
Washington DC: U.S. Department of Education.
Meisenberg, Gerhard (2004) Talent, character, and the dimensions of
national culture. Mankind Quarterly 45: 123-168.
Meisenberg, Gerhard (2007) Does multiculturalism promote income
inequality? Mankind Quarterly 47: 3-39.
Meisenberg, Gerhard (2008) How does racial diversity raise income
inequality? Journal of Social, Political and Economic Studies 33: 3-26.
Meisenberg, Gerhard (in press) National IQ and economic outcomes.
Personality and Individual Differences.
Meisenberg, G., E. Lawless, E. Lambert & A. Newton (2006) The
social ecology of intelligence on a Caribbean island. Mankind Quarterly
46: 395-433.
Mullis, Ina V.S., M.O. Martin, E.J. Gonzales & S.J. Chrostowski
(2004) TIMSS 2003 International Mathematics Report. Chestnut Hill (MA):
IEA. Accessed 23 Jan. 2007 at:
http://timss.bc.edu/timss2003i/intl_reports.html
Mullis, Ina V.S., M.O. Martin & P. Foy (2008) TIMSS 2007
International Mathematics Report. Chestnut Hill (MA): TIMSS & PIRLS
International Study Center. Accessed 9 June 2009 at:
http://timss.bc.edu/TIMSS2007/intl_reports.html
Nordhaus, William D. (2006) Geography and macroeconomics: new data
and new findings. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA
103: 3510-3517.
Nunn, Nathan (2008) The long-term effects of Africa's slave
trades. Quarterly Journal of Economics 123: 139-176.
Rindermann, Heiner (2006) Was messen internationale
Schulleistungsstudien? Schulleistungen, Schulerfahigkeiten, kognitive
Fahigkeiten, Wissen oder allgemeine Intelligenz? [What do international
school achievement studies measure? School achievement, pupils'
abilities, mental abilities, knowledge or general intelligence?]
Psychologische Rundschau 57(2), 69-86.
Rindermann, Heiner (2007) The g-factor of international cognitive
ability comparisons: the homogeneity of results in PISA, TIMSS, PIRLS
and IQ-tests across nations. European Journal of Personality 21:
667-706.
Rindermann, Heiner (2008a) Relevance of education and intelligence
at the national level for the economic welfare of people. Intelligence
36: 127-142.
Rindermann, Heiner (2008b) Relevance of education and intelligence
for the political development of nations: democracy, rule of law and
political liberty. Intelligence 36: 306-322.
Rindermann, Heiner & G. Meisenberg (2009) Relevance of
education and intelligence at the national level for health: the case of
HIV and AIDS. Intelligence 37: 383-395.
Rindermann, Heiner, M. Sailer & J. Thompson (2009) The impact
of smart fractions, cognitive ability of politicians and average
competence of peoples on social development. Talent Development &
Excellence 1: 3-25.
Rodrik, D., A. Subramanian & F. Trebbi (2002) Institutions
rule: the primacy of institutions over geography and integration in
economic development. NBER Working Papers 9305.
http://www.nber.org/papers/w9305
Sala-i-Martin, Xavier, G. Doppelhofer & R. Miller (2004)
Determinants of long-run growth: a Bayesian averaging of classical
estimates (BACE) approach. American Economic Review 94: 813-835
Shayer, Michael & D. Ginsburg (2009) Thirty years on--a large
anti-Flynn effect? (II): 13-and 14-year olds. Piagetian tests of formal
operations norms 1976-2006/7. British Journal of Educational Psychology
79: 409-418.
Sundet, Jon Martin, D.G. Barlaug & T.M. Torjussen (2004) The
end of the Flynn effect? A study of secular trends in mean intelligence
test scores of Norwegian conscripts during half a century. Intelligence
32: 349-362.
Teasdale, Thomas W. & D.R. Owen (2008) Secular declines in
cognitive test scores: a reversal of the Flynn effect. Intelligence 36:
121-126.
United Nations (2004) Human Development Report. Retrieved 15 Oct
2008 at http://hdr.undp.org/en/media/HDR04_complete.pdf
United Nations (2005) Human Development Report. Retrieved 10 Oct
2008 at http://hdr.undp.org/en/media/HDR05_complete.pdf
Valdes, Hector, E. Trevino, C.G. Acevedo, M. Castro, S. Carrillo,
R. Costilla, D. Bogoya & C. Pardo (2008) Student Achievement in
Latin America and the Caribbean. Santiago (Chile): Regional Bureau for
Education in Latin America and the Caribbean OREALC/UNESCO, available
at: http://www,llece.org/public/component/option,com_publicaciones/task
,ver/id,6/Itemid,6/lang.en/
Voracek, Martin (2005) National intelligence, suicide rate in the
elderly, and a threshold intelligence for suicidality: an ecological
study of 48 Eurasian countries. Journal of Biosocial Science 37:
721-740.
Weber, Max (1930) The Protestant ethic and the spirit of
capitalism. London: Allen & Unwin.
Weede, Erich (2004) Does human capital strongly affect growth
rates? Yes, but only if assessed properly. Comparative Sociology 3:
115-134.
Weede, Erich & S. Kampf (2002) The impact of intelligence and
institutional improvements on economic growth. Kyklos 55: 361-380.
Wicherts, Jelte M., D. Borsboom & C.V. Dolan (2010) Why
national IQs do not support evolutionary theories of intelligence.
Personality and Individual Differences 48: 91-96.
Zuckerman, Phil (2005) Atheism: contemporary rates and patterns.
In: M. Martin (ed): The Cambridge Companion to Atheism, pp. 47-67.
Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Gerhard Meisenberg (1)
Ross University School of Medicine
Richard Lynn (2)
University of Ulster, Coleraine
(1) Email: Gmeisenberg@rossmed.edu.dm
(2) Email: LYNNR540@aol.com
Table 1. Correlations of human capital measures with some other
country-level variables. Human capital (HC) is the composite of IQ
and school achievement. N, number of countries; *, p<0.05; **,
p<0.01; ([.sub.NS]), non-significant. All other correlations are
significant at p<0.001.
IQ SA
School achievement (SA) 0.889 1
Human capital (HC) 0.981 0.949
Education 0.757 0.747
lgGDP 1995-2005 0.745 0.717
Econ. Growth 1985-2009 0.426 0.467
Corruption 1996-2003 -0.599 -0.576
Econ. Freedom 1995-2005 0.635 0.607
Big Government 1995-2005 0.258 * 0.359
Gini index -0.614 -0.638
Democracy 1985-2005 0.684 0.611
Pol. Freedom 1985-2005 0.557 0.464
Suicide 0.557 0.595
Life Exp. 2000-2005 0.838 0.761
Infant Mortality 2002 -0.834 -0.777
TFR 1995-2005 -0.839 -0.837
Religiosity 1981-2008 -0.779 -0.788
Happiness -0.047 ([sub.NS]) -0.058 ([sub.NS])
Life satisfaction 0.576 0.503
HC Education
School achievement (SA)
Human capital (HC) 1
Education 0.774 1
lgGDP 1995-2005 0.759 0.711
Econ. Growth 1985-2009 0.451 0.108ns
Corruption 1996-2003 -0.606 -0.582
Econ. Freedom 1995-2005 0.648 0.621
Big Government 1995-2005 0.299 ** 0.378
Gini index -0.646 -0.489
Democracy 1985-2005 0.667 0.675
Pol. Freedom 1985-2005 0.544 0.593
Suicide 0.598 0.498
Life Exp. 2000-2005 0.833 0.648
Infant Mortality 2002 0.838 -0.755
TFR 1995-2005 -0.862 -0.774
Religiosity 1981-2008 -0.797 -0.739
Happiness -0.067 ([sub.NS]) -0.120 ([sub.NS])
Life satisfaction 0.568 0.471
lgGDP N
School achievement (SA) 99
Human capital (HC) 99
Education 99
lgGDP 1995-2005 1 99
Econ. Growth 1985-2009 0.215 ([sub.NS]) 82
Corruption 1996-2003 -0.751 99
Econ. Freedom 1995-2005 0.761 98
Big Government 1995-2005 0.248 * 98
Gini index -0.468 87
Democracy 1985-2005 0.609 95
Pol. Freedom 1985-2005 0.526 97
Suicide 0.241 * 68
Life Exp. 2000-2005 0.782 98
Infant Mortality 2002 -0.837 99
TFR 1995-2005 -0.754 99
Religiosity 1981-2008 -0.586 97
Happiness 0.214 ([sub.NS]) 73
Life satisfaction 0.753 96
Table 2: Regression models predicting economic and political
outcomes with human capital, schooling, log-transformed GDP,
and history of communist rule. * <.05; **. <.01; ***, <.001.
Corruption Big
Predictor Growth freedom Economic govt.
Human capital 0.659 *** -0.228 * 0.181 * 0.124
Schooling -0.445 ** -0.239 * 0.405 *** 0.087
lgGDP 0.163 -0.378 *** 0.319 ** 0.076
Communism 0.214 * 0.345 *** -0.376 *** 0.104
N 124 150 145 145
Adj [R.sup.2] 0.324 0.606 0.684 0.068
Gini Democracy
Predictor index Freedom Political
Human capital -0.566 *** 0.411 *** 0.126
Schooling 0.142 0.608 *** 0.702 ***
lgGDP -0.073 -0.118 -0.060
Communism -0.191 -0.402 *** -0.353 ***
N 129 143 147
Adj [R.sup.2] 0.339 0.625 0.482
Table 3. Regression models predicting quality of
life measures with human capital, schooling,
log-transformed GDP, and history of
communist rule. * <.05; **. <.01; ***,
<.001.
Life Infant
Predictor Suicide Expectancy mortality TFR
Human capital 0.387 ** 0.471 *** -0.327 *** -0.289 ***
Schooling 0.160 0.042 -0.248 ** -0.238 ***
lgGDP -0.053 0.396 *** -0.383 *** -0.392 ***
Communism 0.298 * -0.007 0.003 -0.218 ***
N 89 147 150 150
Adj. [R.sup.2] 0.345 0.704 0.763 0.823
Predictor Religious Happy Satisfied
Human capital -0.506 *** -0.156 0.186
Schooling -0.265 ** -0.019 0.069
lgGDP -0.012 0.339 0.556 ***
Communism -0.174 ** -0.570 *** -0.255 ***
N 140 93 138
Adj. [R.sup.2] 0.656 0.458 0.601