The attractiveness of regional towns: inferring quality of life from higher education facilities.
Drummond, Aaron ; Palmer, Matthew A. ; Halsey, R. John 等
INTRODUCTION
More than half of the world's population now resides in urban
areas (United Nations, 2009). Out-migration from regional areas has
raised concerns about numerous economic and health issues, such as food
security (Ehrlich, Ehrlich & Daily, 1993). From this perspective, it
is becoming increasingly important for policy makers to understand--and
perhaps be able to influence--people's decisions about where to
live, particularly with regard to regional areas.
Many models of migration patterns regard economic considerations as
the largest--or sole-determinant in migration decisions (e.g., Eggert,
Krieger, & Meier, 2010; Hicks, 1932). However, although economic
models do well at predicting migration at a macro-level, anomalies in
migration data, such as the absence of a relationship between
unemployment rates and in-migration in many areas (Greenwood, 1975,
1997), suggest that residency decisions are not solely determined by
economic factors. It is possible that psychological factors might
explain some of the variance in migration decisions that is unaccounted
for by economic variables.
Very little is known about how cognitive processes shape residency
decisions. As an initial step toward developing an understanding of this
issue, we investigated one possible role played by heuristic judgment
processes (e.g., Chaiken & Trope, 1999; Tversky & Kahneman,
1974). Consider a situation in which a judgment must be made without
sufficient information to enable an accurate response. In such
situations, people often base their judgment on inferences drawn from
information that is available but not directly relevant to the judgment.
For example, Gigerenzer, Hoffrage, and Kleinbolting (1996) asked
participants which of two foreign towns had the larger population. If
participants did not know the answer, they attempted to use the
information they did have about the two towns to infer their relative
population. For instance, if told that one town had a football team in
the national league and the other did not, participants tended to select
the town with the football team (such teams are likely to be located in
large towns).
We investigated whether this type of heuristic process contributes
to residency decisions. When presented with limited information about a
town, would people use the available information to make inferences
about other aspects of the town relevant to residency decisions? We
provided undergraduate students at a metropolitan university with
information about fictional towns and asked participants to rate the
likelihood that they would be willing to live in each town after they
graduated. The towns differed in terms of their higher education
facilities: a university campus, a community college, or none. At first
glance, it might seem highly intuitive that people would be more willing
to live in towns with better facilities of any kind (e.g., educational,
recreational) because these would afford opportunities to engage in a
wider range of activities. However, we were interested in whether
facilities play a more subtle role. Specifically, we hypothesized that
participants would be more willing to live in towns with higher
education facilities--even if they were not intending to work or study
at these--because the presence of such facilities would lead
participants to infer a better quality of life for residents.
A metropolitan undergraduate sample is important for understanding
the attractiveness of regional towns as potential residences for several
reasons. First, university graduates are more likely to migrate than
non-college-educated youth (Kodrzycki, 2001). Second, in many countries
such as India and the U.S.A., the population of university graduates is
projected to increase over the coming years (e.g., National Science
Board, 2010; Agarwal, 2009). Third, the high migration rate of youth
from regional to metropolitan areas (e.g., Argent & Walmsley, 2008;
Carr & Kefalas, 2009) suggests that regional communities would
benefit from an increase in metropolitan-to-rural migration among youth.
Thus, policies aimed at encouraging metropolitan university students to
move to regional areas may be particularly fruitful for addressing the
current problems with rural out-migration.
EXPERIMENT 1
Method
Participants. Forty undergraduate students (19 female; aged 18 to
25 years, M = 22 years; SD = 2 years) participated for payment. Flinders
University's Social and Behavioural Research Ethics Committee
(SBREC) approved the research, and participants gave informed consent to
participate in written form.
Design and Procedure. Participants received all instructions and
made their responses via computer while alone in a quiet cubicle. We
used a judgment analysis approach (Cooksey, 1996; Houston, 1974).
Participants viewed 12 descriptions of fictional towns, representing a 3
(higher education facilities: university; vocational college; none) x 4
(population size: 5,000; 10,000; 20,000; 50,000) within-subjects design.
Descriptions were identical in all other aspects and were presented in a
random order. Appendix A contains the descriptions.
For each description, participants indicated the likelihood they
would live in the town for longer than 1 year (a) as a percentage rating
and (b) with a "yes/no" response. For the latter decision, the
computer recorded response latency, which reflects decision difficulty
(easy decisions are made quicker than hard ones; e.g., Geller &
Pitz, 1968). Latency was used to compute an implicit measure of
residency intentions as follows. First, raw latency was subtracted from
zero for "no" responses and added to zero for "yes"
responses. Then, the inverse of each latency value was calculated. Thus,
larger positive values represented faster (easier) "yes"
responses and larger negative values represented faster (easier)
"no" responses.
For towns with education facilities, participants were also asked
whether they would seek to work or study at the facilities, assuming
that they lived in that town after graduation. This allowed us to rule
out the possibility that any effect of facilities was simply due to
participants who planned to continue their academic pursuits.
RESULTS AND DISCUSSION
Explicit measure. As hypothesized, residency judgments (% ratings)
were influenced by the presence of education facilities. The main effect
of facilities was significant, F (2, 78) = 50.74, p < .001, and
qualified by a facilities x population interaction, F (6, 234) = 2.26, p
= .038. We explored the interaction via planned comparisons. At each
population level, the town with no university was compared to each of
the towns with education facilities (see Figure 1).
[FIGURE 1 OMITTED]
At each population level, participants reported a higher likelihood
of living in the town for more than one year if the town had a
university versus no higher education facilities (all ts > 4.26, ps
< .001), or a vocational college versus no higher education
facilities (all ts > 2.11, ps < .041). The patterns of
Cohen's d effect size estimates associated with these comparisons
(see Table 1) suggested that the presence of a university had a smaller
effect on residency judgments for towns of population 5,000 than for
larger towns. In contrast, the presence of a vocational college had a
consistent (albeit weaker) effect on residency judgments across the
different town populations.
Importantly, the main effect of education facilities on residency
judgments did not disappear when we considered only participants who
indicated that they did not intend to work or study at the facilities (n
= 13), F (2, 24) = 7.45, p = .003. These participants reported being
more willing to live in towns with a university (M = 45%, SD = 19%) than
those with no education facilities (M = 33%, SD = 23%), t(12) = 3.04, p
= .010, d = 0.86. However, residency likelihood did not differ between
towns with a vocational college (M = 35%, SD = 23%) and those with no
education facilities, t < 1, p = .343.
Implicit measure. The results for the implicit measure of residency
intentions also yielded a significant main effect of facilities, F(2,
78) = 28.47, p < .001, but no facilities * population interaction,
F(6, 234) = 1.30, p = .258. Scores on the implicit measure were higher
(indicating faster, more positive responses) for towns with a university
(M = .023, SD = 0.41) versus no education facilities (M = -0.21, SD =
0.30), t(39) = 3.51, p = .001, d = 0.52. Likewise, scores were higher
for towns with a vocational college (M = -0.06, SD = 0.35) versus no
education facilities, t(39) = 3.51, p = .001, d = 0.52. This general
pattern maintained for participants who indicated that they did not
intend to work or study at the facilities described, although the effect
was non-significant, F (2, 24) = 1.53, p = .237.
To ensure that these effects were not due simply to the proportion
of "yes/no" judgments inherent in this measure, we also
conducted separate linear mixed model analyses of response latencies for
positive residency judgments ("yes" responses) nested within
participants. The results were consistent with the notion that the
education facilities manipulation affected decision ease.
"Yes" responses were faster for towns with a university (M =
2883ms, SD = 2212ms) than those without educational facilities (M =
3967ms SD = 3223ms), F(1, 220) = 5.90, p = .016, d = 0.39. There was
also a nonsignificant trend toward faster "yes" responses for
towns with vocational colleges (M = 3345ms, SD = 2573ms) than those with
no higher education facilities, F(1, 220) = 1.59, p = .208.
The results of Experiment 1 provided evidence from explicit and
implicit measures that information about higher education facilities
influences residency judgments. It is important that towns with a
university were rated as more likely residences even among participants
who did not intend to work or study at the university. This finding
indicates that the results were not driven by participants' desire
to continue their academic pursuits, and is consistent with the notion
that people draw inferences about quality of life a town from the
presence of a university. We tested this idea directly in Experiment 2
by examining whether the presence of a university affected residency
judgments via perceptions of residents' quality of life. We also
included a description of a metropolitan town for comparison.
EXPERIMENT 2
Method
Participants. One-hundred and ninety-five undergraduate students
(99 female, 86 male, 10 non-responses) aged 17 to 48 years (M = 20
years; SD = 4 years) participated for payment. Flinders
University's Social and Behavioural Research Ethics Committee
(SBREC) approved the research, and participants gave informed consent to
participate in written form.
Procedure. Participants completed Experiment 2 as a
pencil-and-paper task while alone in a quiet cubicle. Participants read
three descriptions of fictional towns presented in counterbalanced
order: a metropolitan town, a regional town with a university, and a
regional town without a university. Population size for the regional
towns was also manipulated between-subjects (5,000 or 10,000 people) but
had no effect on any results and is not discussed further.
For each description, participants indicated the likelihood that
they would move to the town and live for longer than one year after
graduation (percentage rating) and completed a 3-item measure of
perceived quality of life in the town. The items were derived from the
results of Bowling's (1995) large survey of adults, which
identified happiness and social life/leisure activities as two
important, non-economic aspects of quality of life. Thus, we defined
quality of life as a combination of resident happiness and sociability,
and the potential activities available within the town. Participants
were asked to indicate on 7-point scales the extent to which each
town's residents were likely to be (a) happy and (b) sociable, and
the extent to which each town was likely to have a lot of potential
activities.
For towns with a university, participants also indicated whether
they or their partner would intend to work or study at the university,
assuming they lived in the town after graduating.
RESULTS AND DISCUSSION
We report results only for the subset of participants who reported
that neither they nor their partner would intend to work or study at the
university (n = 108). Analysis of the entire data set produced virtually
identical results.
The manipulation of town description affected residency judgments,
F (2, 212) = 37.05, p < .001. Pairwise comparisons were conducted
using Bonferroni-corrected alpha levels of .017. As expected,
participants reported a greater likelihood of living in the regional
town with a university (M = 63%, SD = 24%) than the regional town
without a university (M = 53%, SD = 27%), t(107) = 6.76, p < .001, d
= 0.88. Participants also reported being more willing to living in the
metropolitan town (M = 74%, SD = 21%) than the regional town with a
university, t(107) = 4.28, p < .001, d = 0.51, or the regional town
without a university, t(107) = 6.96, p < .001, d = 1.11.
Perceived quality of life scores were affected by the description
manipulation and followed the same pattern as residency judgments, F (2,
212) = 25.94, p < .001. Perceived quality of life was higher for the
regional town with a university (M = 14.5, SD = 2.8) than the regional
town without a university (M = 13.6, SD = 2.9), t(107) = 4.25, p <
.001, d = 0.47. Perceived quality of life was also higher for the
metropolitan town (M = 15.5, SD = 2.9) than the regional towns with a
university, t(107) = 3.74, p < .001, d = 0.45, or without a
university, t(107) = 4.25, p < .001, d = 0.79.
To assess whether the effect of town description on residency
judgments was mediated by perceived quality of life, we followed a
method recommended by Judd, Kenny, and McClelland (2001) for testing
mediation hypotheses involving a categorical, within-subjects
independent variable. Separate analyses were conducted for pairwise
comparisons of the levels of the independent variable. The comparison of
greatest interest was between the two regional towns, which reflected
the effect of the presence of a university on residency judgments.
The mean difference in residency judgments between regional towns
with a university and regional towns without a university was
significantly predicted by the mean difference in quality of life
judgments, suggesting that the effect of university presence on
residency judgments was mediated by perceived quality of life, B = 3.05,
t = 4.80, p < .001. The associated intercept differed significantly
from zero, indicating partial rather than full mediation, B = -7.89, t =
-5.04, p < .001.
We conducted a similar mediation analysis for the comparison
between towns without a university and metropolitan towns. Again, the
effect of the description on residency judgments was mediated by
perceived quality of life, as the mean difference in residency judgments
was predicted by the mean difference in quality of life judgments, B =
4.76, t = 5.24, p < .001. The associated intercept was significantly
different from zero, indicating partial mediation, B = 12.86, t = 3.90,
p < .001.
The results of Experiment 2 align with and extend those of
Experiment 1, offering further support for the idea that, when
considering whether to live in a town, people make use of limited
available information to draw inferences about likely quality of life in
the town.
GENERAL DISCUSSION
Little is presently known about the psychological processes
underlying migration decisions. Although economic factors clearly
influence migration choices (e.g., Eggert et al., 2010), the present
studies suggest that non-economic town characteristics can affect the
evaluation of potential residences by leading people to make inferences
about the potential quality of life within a town. Thus, the presence of
higher education facilities increases the perceived attractiveness of a
potential residence even for those who do not intend to directly use the
facilities. When evaluating a town as a potential residence, it is
highly unlikely that people have access to all relevant information,
especially if the town under consideration is far away. From this
perspective, it makes sense that people use the information they do have
to draw inferences that aid residency decisions. Indeed, this process
may facilitate efficient and effective decision making, provided valid
inferences are drawn (Gigerenzer et al., 1991).
These data have implications for current policy issues. As noted
previously, increased migration from regional to metropolitan areas is a
problem faced by many communities, with more than half of the
world's population now residing in urban areas (United Nations,
2009). The increasing exodus from regional areas is likely to result in
difficulties with education in rural areas (e.g., Drummond, Halsey &
van Breda, 2012) and lower levels of food security at a time when the
world needs to drastically increase food production (Godfray et al.,
2010). Our results suggest that investment in university facilities in
regional areas might help alleviate this problem not only by retaining
young people who want to pursue tertiary education (Artz & Yu, 2011;
Drummond, Halsey & van Breda, 2011), but also via a novel mechanism:
attracting young, university-educated people from metropolitan areas.
The idea that a university may help regional towns attract graduates is
worthy of close attention, particularly given that government investment
in higher education has recently been a contentious political topic
(e.g., Altbach, Reisberg, & Rumbley, 2009; Johnstone, 2011).
Finally, this research also indicates some potentially fruitful
directions for further investigation. One involves examining factors
that determine the extent to which people use limited information to
draw valid versus misleading inferences about towns (e.g., Gigerenzer
& Todd, 1999; Tversky & Kahneman, 1974). Another relates to the
issue of how residency likelihood judgments might translate to migration
behavior (i.e., actually relocating to that town). For example, perhaps
towns that are more attractive are perceived as closer and, therefore,
easier to move to (Alter & Balcetis, 2011). Although migration
behaviour has traditionally been studied by researchers in other
disciplines (e.g., economists, sociologists, and demographers),
psychologists are well placed to contribute to this field.
APPENDIX A: ADDITIONAL METHODOLOGICAL DETAILS
EXPERIMENT 1
Town Descriptions
The following is a description of a town in country Australia. The
town is situated in a picturesque area, with good access to roads. The
town houses approximately [5,000; 10,000; 20,000; 50,000] local
residents who are active within its community. The town has many
facilities to support the community, [including a university; including
a TAFE; but no university or TAFE]. The town holds many community
events, and the residents describe the town as enjoyable, vibrant and
peaceful.
Questions
Would you live in the town described for longer than 1 year after
you graduated? (Yes/ No)
What is the % chance you would live in the town described for
longer than 1 year after you had graduated? (0-100%, in 10% increments)
If you lived in the town after you had graduated, would you be
seeking to work in or attend the educational facilities described?
(Yes/No)
NB: Technical and Further Education (TAFE) colleges are very
common, well-known vocational colleges in Australia
EXPERIMENT 2
Please read the bullet points of the following towns and then
answer the questions on the following pages.
Town A
* Australian Metropolitan Centre
* Picturesque Area
* Has many facilities
* Has a university
* Greater than 1 million residents
Town B
* Australian Regional Centre
* Picturesque Area
* Has many facilities
* Has a university
* [10,000 [50,000] residents
Town C
* Australian Regional Centre
* Picturesque Area
* Has many facilities
* Has a university
* 10,000 [50,000] residents
N.B. Towns were counterbalanced between participants such that they
each appeared as Town A, Town B or Town C.
ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS
This research was funded by The Sidney Myer Chair of Rural
Education and Communities, an initiative of The Myer Foundation and The
Sidney Myer Fund, and supported by ARC Discovery Grant DP1093210 to N.
Brewer et al. We thank Mike Lawson for input on a previous version of
this manuscript, and Kate de Garis for collecting data for this project.
REFERENCES
Agarwal, P. (2009). Indian Higher Education: Envisioning the
Future. New Delhi: Sage.
Altbach, P. G., Reisberg, L., & Rumbley, L. E. (2009). Trends
in global higher education: Tracking an academic revolution. Paris:
United Nations Educational, Scientific and Cultural Organization.
Retrieved 6 December, 2011, from
http://www.unesco.org/new/en/unesco/resources/online-materials/publications/unesdoc- database/
Alter, A. L., & Balcetis, E. (2011). Fondness makes the
distance grow shorter: Desired location seem closer because they are
more vivid. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 47, 16-21.
Argent, N., & Walmsley, J. (2008). Rural youth migration trends
in Australia: An overview of recent trends and two inland case studies.
Geographical Research, 46, 139-152. doi:10.1111/j.1745-5871.2008.00505.x
Artz, G., & Yu, L. (2011). How ya gonna keep 'em down on
the farm: Which land grant graduates live in rural areas? Economic
Development Quarterly, 25, 341-352. doi:10.1177/0891242411409399
Bowling, A. (1995). What things are important in people's
lives? Social Science and Medicine, 41, 1447-1462.
Carr, P. J., & Kefalas, M. J. (2009). Hollowing out the middle:
The rural brain drain and what it means for America. Boston: Beacon
Press.
Chaiken, S., & Trope, Y. (Eds.). (1999). Dual-process theories
in social psychology. New York: Guilford Press.
Cooksey, R.W. (1996). Judgment analysis: Theory, methods, and
applications. San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
Drummond, A., Halsey, R. J., & van Breda, M. (2011) The
perceived importance of university presence in rural Australia.
Education in Rural Australia, 21, 1-18.
Drummond, A., Halsey, R. J., & van Breda, M. (2012)
Implementing the Austrian Curriculum in rural schools. Curriculum
Perspectives, 32, 34-44.
Eggert, W., Krieger, T., & Meier, V. (2010). Education,
unemployment and migration. Journal of Public Economics, 94(5-6),
354-362.
Ehrlich, P. R., Ehrlich, A. H., & Daily, G. C. Food security,
population and environment. Population and Development Review, 19, 1-32.
Geller, E. S., & Pitz, G. F. (1968). Confidence and decision
speed in the revision of opinion. Organizational Behavior & Human
Performance, 3, 190-201.
Gigerenzer, G., Hoffrage, U., & Kleinbolting, H. (1991).
Probabilistic mental models: A Brunswikian theory of confidence.
Psychological Review, 98, 506-528.
Gigerenzer, G., & Todd, P. M. (Eds.). (1999). Simple heuristics
that make us smart. New York: Oxford University Press.
Greenwood, M. J. (1975). Research on Internal Migration in the
United States: A Survey. Journal of Economic Literature, 13, 397-433.
Greenwood, M. J. (1997). Chapter 12 Internal migration in developed
countries. In R. R. Mark & S. Oded (Eds.), Handbook of Population
and Family Economics (Vol. Volume 1, Part B, pp. 647-720): Elsevier.
Godfray, H. C. J., Beddington, J. R., Crute, I. R., Haddad, L.,
Lawrence, D., Muir, J. F., Pretty, J., Robinson, S., Thomas, S. M.,
& Toulmin, C. (2010) Food Security: The challenge of feeding 9
billion people. Science, 327, 812-818.
Hicks, J. R. (1932). The theory of wages. London: Macmillian.
Houston, S. R. (1974). Judgment analysis: Tool for decision makers.
New York: MSS Information Corporation.
Johnstone, D. B. (2011). Financing higher education: Who should
pay? In P. G. Altbach, P. J. Gumport & R. O. Berdahl (Eds.),
American Higher Education in the Twenty-First Century: Social, Political
and Economic Challenges. Baltimore, MD: Johns Hopkins University Press.
Judd, C. M., Kenny, D. A., & McClelland, G. H. (2001).
Estimating and testing mediation and moderation in within-subjects
designs. Psychological Methods, 6, 115-134.
doi:10.1037//1082-989X.6.2.115
Kodrzycki, Y. K. (2001). Migration of recent college graduates:
Evidence from the National Longitudinal Survey of Youth. New England
Economic Review, January/February, 13-34.
National Science Board (2010). Science and Engineering Indicators.
Arlington, VA: National Science Board. Retrieved 6 December, 2011, from
http://www.nsf.gov/statistics/seind10/
Tversky, A., & Kahneman, D. (1974). Judgment under uncertainty:
Heuristics and biases. Science, 185, 1124-1131.
United Nations (2009) World Urbanization Prospects: The 2009
Revision. New York, NY: United Nations. Retrieved 19 March, 2012 from
http://esa.un.org/unpd/wup/index.htm
Aaron Drummond
School of Education, Flinders University
Matthew A. Palmer
School of Psychology, Flinders University Now at School of
Psychology, University of Tasmania
R. John Halsey
School of Education, Flinders University
Table 1. Effect sizes (Cohen's d) for planned comparisons
of education facilities conditions for towns of different
population size
POPULATION SIZE
Comparison 5,000 10,000 20,000 50,000
University vs. 0.87 1.32 1.48 1.28
no facilities
Vocational 0.42 0.58 0.60 0.48
college vs.
no facilities