Managing cognitive load in the mathematics classroom.
Chinnappan, Mohan ; Chandler, Paul
Context
Contemporary debates on effective pedagogies for K-12 mathematics
have called for shifts in the way teachers and teacher educators
conceptualise mathematics as a subject and how it should be taught. This
is reflected by changes in the curriculum including the inclusion of a
strand called Working Mathematically within K-12 mathematics curriculum
documents not only in New South Wales but also across Australia (New
South Wales Board of Studies, 2002). This strand brings focus to mental
processes that underpin students' ability to acquire mathematical
principles, concepts, and conventions, and the use of this knowledge in
the solution of problems.
The focus on cognitive processes that support mathematical learning
and problem solving is a welcome change. However, there is a paucity of
information about the nature of links that need to be made between
process and mathematics content, and how students might be assisted to
construct the links.
In this paper, we outline results of research about cognitive load
that is associated with mental processes, the management of this load so
that students can be better supported in the construction of connected
mathematical information, and the interpretation of that information in
making sense of worked examples. We attempt to show that worked examples
can be effective in promoting useful and powerful mathematics schemas.
Cognitive processes that underlie mathematics problem solving
An understanding of the cognitive processes that drive mathematics
learning and knowledge organisation is critical for the design of
effective approaches to mathematics teaching. Figure 1 shows a model of
human memory structures and the processing of information. This model is
based on components of working memory advanced by Baddeley and Hitch
(2000). The model identifies two key attributes about how students deal
with mathematical information.
Firstly, it shows connections between the processing of incoming
mathematical information, reorganisation and the subsequent retrieval of
that information for later use. Secondly, the model draws attention to
the types of cognitive load that students could experience as they
attempt to make sense of incoming mathematical information.
[FIGURE 1 OMITTED]
There are three types of memory: sensory memory, working memory
(WM), and long term memory (LTM). In the following sections we look at
both working and long term memory in some detail as these are more
directly related to loads that can be exerted on the processing of
incoming information.
Working memory (WM)
Working memory can be approximated to the idea of consciousness. If
we are consciously aware of information then we are utilising working
memory. A multitude of models of working memory has been proposed over
the decades. Despite differences, all models tend to share two common
basic characteristics about working memory: limitations in processing
capacity and duration.
During a learning episode, new information from the environment is
processed through WM. However, there are limitations to both the storage
capacity of WM and the duration of time new information can be held and
processed in WM. Although learners can process around seven separate
items of information at any one time (Miller, 1956) this number
significantly declines if items need to be compared or contrasted in
some way (Kalyuga, 2006). For example, many young students starting at
Stage 1 or 2 multiplication would have difficulty performing 87 x 28 as
a mental operation as the task overloads the capacity of WM. This is so
because the solution could involve two levels of processing. For
example, students will usually have to compute 80 x 28 and 7 x 28
mentally, following which they have to add the resulting values. Both
these computations, in turn, demand the use of further mental
strategies.
Long-term memory (LTM)
In contrast to working memory, LTM is characterised by its
limitless capacity for the storage of organised information.
Mathematical knowledge and problem solving strategies are stored in the
long-term memory. Information in LTM is also more robust and unlike WM,
information in LTM is stored in a more or less permanent form (Newell
& Simon, 1972).
The question of how information is stored in LTM and later
retrieved for use has been the subject of discussion for many decades. A
number of models has been advanced in discussion of this issue
(Marshall, 1995). Barlett (1932) coined the term "problem
schema" in his classic paper about general constructions that allow
people to categorise information and the way it is used. Schemas are the
structures in long-term memory that allow us, for example, to read the
text on this page effortlessly, so that reading this sentence places
little load on WM. For an individual without a relevant sophisticated
schema base (a child, or perhaps an adult without the schemas that
included academic jargon), this task would be effortful. In cases where
word schemas are not developed, the task would be impossible. Thus,
schemas in LTM allow us to negotiate effortlessly the world around us.
Understanding how a schema develops is important when devising
appropriate teaching strategies.
Schema development
The quality of students' mathematical knowledge can exert a
major influence on the deployment of that knowledge during learning and
solution attempts. Quality of mathematical knowledge can be interpreted
in terms of the degree of organisation of the different bits of
mathematical information that constitute that knowledge. The framework
of a schema helps to visualise connections that exist between core ideas
and their components, and among the components. These comprise
mathematical definitions and rules as well as knowledge about how to
deal with a particular class of problems.
For instance, the understanding of the relations between parts and
whole of a fraction, can be facilitated for a group of children by first
involving them in an activity that embodies the notion of fractions,
such as slicing a string into two equal parts. During this activity the
children may be introduced to terms such as "half,"
"part" and "whole" in reference to the string that
is being sliced. Thus, via the activity, the children have the
opportunity to develop meaningful relations between the three terms. In
a subsequent lesson, when the children are introduced to the term
"fraction" with discussion about parts and whole, this word
enters into the WM for a brief period, perhaps lasting a few seconds.
During this period, children need to establish links to related prior
concepts such as parts and wholes so that a meaningful schema about
fractions is developed and stored in the LTM. That is, the child
accommodates and assimilates concepts of fractions into an existing
schema consisting of half, part and whole. Likewise, Chinnappan (1998)
showed that geometric knowledge that was organised into meaningful and
well-connected schemas played a critical role in fostering
students' ability to use that knowledge appropriately during
solution attempts.
Investigations conducted by Kirschner (2002) led to the conclusion
that mathematical knowledge bases that are effectively organised in the
form of schemas will facilitate more effective activation and use of
knowledge during problem solving.
High levels of performance in mathematics rely heavily on schema
acquisition. The development of schemas is an ongoing process involving
cycles of modification and assimilation of incoming information. The
establishment of schemas lessens the burden on the finite mental
resources of working memory. Hence, mathematics instruction should
attempt to promote activities that will facilitate schema development
while being sensitive to the limited processing capacity of WM.
Types of cognitive load
The mental resources required of working memory to learn, perform
or understand a task can vary quite dramatically between tasks. Some
mathematics tasks may involve little cognitive load while others will be
very complex and, therefore, heavy in cognitive load. If a mathematical
task exceeds the mental resources available in working memory then
cognitive overload will occur. There are at least three types of
cognitive load that can be imposed on learners.
Intrinsic, extraneous and germane loads
The level of intrinsic load relates to the complexity of a task
relative to a particular learner. Learning to memorise a mathematical
formula, such as that for the area of a circle (A = [pi][r.sup.2]), is a
task that is low in complexity and would impose a low intrinsic load. To
process this information, students simply need to consider the various
elements of this information in isolation. Students would not need to
process concurrently any other information, such as the formula for
circumference, A = 2[pi]r. Thus, recalling a simple formula could be
taught with little or no interaction with other elements of information.
It is a "low-element-interactivity task" (Sweller &
Chandler, 1994).
However, applying the formula (A = [pi][r.sup.2]) to a novel
mathematical problem requires the learner to relate and compare parts of
the formula (specifically radius, r, and area) with other learning
elements in the problem. This is a task that is high in intrinsic load.
In mathematics, most tasks involve high intrinsic load (generated
by high levels of element interactivity) because mathematics tasks
demand that students draw upon multiple elements of information and
integrate that information to solve a problem. Cognitive load becomes an
issue (due to working memory limitations) when information is high in
complexity, and thus high in intrinsic load.
In summary, mathematics tasks will range in complexity from low
intrinsic load (low-element-interactivity) or high intrinsic load
(high-element-interactivity). Intrinsic load and the degree of element
interactivity will also be dependent on the learner. A task that is low
in intrinsic load for an experienced mathematics teacher could be very
high in intrinsic load for a student. Teachers need to be aware of the
intrinsic load (natural complexity) associated with any mathematical
task. In some cases, tasks may need to be segmented into sub-tasks in
order to control intrinsic load (Mayer and Chandler, 2001).
Extraneous load is imposed solely by the instructional format that
is used during the course of teaching. Instructional format refers to
the organisation of texts and visuals used by teachers to help learners
understand a given concept or problem context. Mathematics can be taught
in a variety of ways and each format of instruction can be expected to
generate its own extraneous cognitive load. In certain formats, students
are given written texts only, while other formats may involve an amalgam
of written texts (scripts) and visual texts (diagrams/animations).
Research shows that a split-attention effect tends to be induced where
written texts are not integrated with visual texts.
For example, practical work, demonstrations, problem-solving, and
studying worked examples will introduce different levels of extraneous
load. Sweller (1994) investigated extraneous cognitive load that could
be imposed by the format of worked problems in the domain of geometry.
Figure 2 shows the format of a conventional geometry problem and its
solution, whereas Figure 3 shows a worked example for the same problem
in an integrated format.
[FIGURE 2 OMITTED]
In the conventional worked example, we have a diagram located above
the text which outlines the solution steps. Seen separately, neither the
diagram nor the text below the diagram give the student much meaningful
information. In order to comprehend the problem, the solver has to
integrate the diagram and the solution steps. The processing of the
diagram and attempts to connect it with the information presented in the
solution steps requires the solver to draw on considerable cognitive
resources, thus introducing extraneous cognitive load. This load can be
attributed solely to the format of the worked example. The search by the
solver to map the diagram with solution steps reduces available
cognitive resources that can be used for schema development and
automation.
The integrated worked example (Figure 3), on the other hand,
releases the solvers' mental capacity such that he or she can focus
more on the relational dimensions of the problem, thus developing useful
schema for problems of the type presented in the worked example.
[FIGURE 3 OMITTED]
Germane Loads refers to activities that involve cognitive load and
effort that directly relate and contribute to schema development and
automation. Germane activities may include self-explanations (Chi,
Bassock, Lewis, Reimann & Glaser 1989), mental imagery (Cooper,
Tindall-Ford, Chandler & Sweller, 2001) and study of rich worked
examples. Learning activities that are germane in nature bring about
meaningful learning (van Cog, Pass & van Merrienboer, 2006). For
example, a student's attempt to justify a solution or
"self-explain" the difficulty in solving a problem contributes
to germane load. Such processing activities demand that students search
their LTM and construct chains of reasoning. In so doing students are
encouraged to extend existing schemas that would help them learn or
solve problems in a meaningful manner. Because students are encouraged
to engage in multidirectional knowledge search, germane activities are
effective in helping students construct powerful domain-specific
schemas. From a mathematics teaching perspective, it is important that
teachers explore ways of fostering germane load. One approach could be
by asking students to present alternative ways of solving a problem and
establishing similarities and differences between the different
approaches.
Worked examples and schema development
Worked examples provide a step-by-step demonstration of how to
solve a given problem. Although students could be given directions in
problem-solving processes through a series of instructions, exercises
and feedback, it is usually the case that students benefit from examples
to understand concepts and procedures. Such behaviour is manifested in
their explicit mentioning of examples when they solve problems (Chi, et
al, 1989). Students who studied worked examples were, in general, found
to be better problem-solvers compared to those who engage in
conventional problem solving. Zhu and Simon (1987) found that the use of
worked examples could act as an effective alternative to conventional
classroom instruction.
When students are asked to solve problems, a great deal of their
mental effort is directed towards understanding the new problem which
involves high levels of extraneous load. From a cognitive processing
perspective, problem-solving consumes a high proportion of the limited
working memory capacity leaving few resources for constructing schemas.
In comparison to problem-solving, studying an appropriately
structured worked example of a problem is less demanding and involves
less extraneous load. As a consequence, limited working memory resources
can be directed towards germane load activities such as understanding
the structure of the problem. During future attempts to solve an
analogous problem, students are able to understand the problems rapidly
and allocate more working memory to the more difficult aspects of a
given problem, and transferring knowledge to new situations.
The use of worked examples in the course of a lesson is not
uncommon in Australian mathematics classrooms. What we are suggesting
here is that both students and teachers need to understand better the
cognitive complexities involved during these activities so that
students' involvement becomes more purposeful and meaningful.
To summarise, mathematics educators need to be mindful about how
they approach teaching when information is high in complexity. During
teaching, teachers need to ensure that they engage students in
activities that are germane in nature that will lead to schema
development and automation. Extraneous load activities that are not
directly related to learning need to be minimised or eliminated during
the learning process. The use of properly structured, worked examples
should be supported as this instructional strategy assists students to
make more efficient use of their working memory and develop powerful
schemas. Instruction needs to be supported by activities that utilise
worked examples that would constrain students' attention to aspects
of the solution in which available cognitive resources assist them to
deconstruct and reconstruct more refined and powerful representations of
problems.
However, when students' experience with a particular topic of
mathematics increases, they develop a rich body of domain-specific
schemas in that topic, and thus, for this group of students, the use of
worked examples as an instructional strategy may be counterproductive.
Processing a worked example that fully describes the solution path does
not improve the existing schemas in any significant manner. Thus, once
learners develop a degree of expertise then conventional problem-solving
activity becomes a more powerful form of mathematical instruction.
References
Baddeley, A. D. & Hitch, G. J. (2000). Development of working
memory: Should the Pascual-Leone and the Baddeley and Hitch models be
merged? Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 77, 128-137.
Barlett, F. (1932). Remembering: A study in experimental and social
psychology. New York: Cambridge University Press.
Board of Studies. (2002). Mathematics K-6, 7-12. Sydney: Board of
Studies NSW.
Chi, M. T. H., Bassock, M., Lewis, R., Reimann, P., & Glaser,
R. (1989). Self explanations: How students study and use examples in
learning to solve problems. Cognitive Science, 13, 145-182.
Chinnappan, M. (1998). Schemas and mental models in geometry
problem solving. Educational Studies in Mathematics, 36(3), 201-217.
Cooper, G., Tindall-Ford, S., Chandler, P., & Sweller, J.
(2001). Learning by imagining procedures and concepts. Journal of
Experimental Psychology: Applied, 7, 68-82.
Kalyuga, S. (2006). Instructing and testing advanced learners: A
cognitive load approach. New York: Nova Science Publishers.
Kirschner, P. A. (2002). Cognitive load theory: implications of
cognitive load theory on the design of learning. Learning and
Instruction, 12(1), 1-10.
Marshall, S. P. (1995). Schemas in problem solving. NY: Cambridge
University Press.
Mayer, R. E. & Chandler, P. (2001). When learning is just a
click away: Does simple user interaction foster deeper understanding of
multimedia messages? Journal of Educational Psychology, 93, 390-397.
Miller, G. A, (1956). The magical number seven, plus or minus two:
Some limits on our capacity for processing. Psychological Review, 63,
81-97.
Newell, A. & Simon, H. (1972). Human problem solving. New
Jersey: Prentice-Hall.
Sweller, J. (1994). Cognitive load theory, learning difficulty and
instructional design. Learning and Instruction, 4, 295-312.
van Gog, T., Paas, F., & van Merrienboer, J. J. G. (2006).
Effects of process-oriented worked examples on troubleshooting transfer
performance. Learning and Instruction, 16,154-164.
Mohan Chinnappan & Paul Chandler
University of Wollongong
<mohan@uow.edu.au>