Multiplicative thinking: much more than knowing multiplication facts and procedures.
Hurst, Chris ; Hurrell, Derek
What characterises multiplicative thinking?
Multiplicative thinking is accepted as a 'big idea' of
mathematics (Hurst & Hurrell, 2015; Siemon, Bleckley & Neal,
2012) that underpins important mathematical concepts such as fraction
understanding, proportional reasoning, and algebraic thinking. It is
characterised by understandings such as the multiplicative relationship
between places in the number system, basic and extended number facts,
and properties of operations and associated relationships (Hurst &
Hurrell, 2015). Siemon, Breed et al. (2006) stated that multiplicative
thinking is:
* a capacity to work flexibly and efficiently with an extended
range of numbers (and the relationships between them);
* an ability to recognise and solve a range of problems involving
multiplication and/or division including direct and indirect proportion;
and
* the means to communicate this effectively in a variety of ways
(e.g., words, diagrams, symbolic expressions, and written algorithms).
(p. 28)
Multiplicative thinking is a complex set of concepts in which are
embedded many connections and relationships. Because of such
complexities, this article attempts to consider only a few aspects of
multiplicative thinking and how they might be taught. Indeed, we suggest
that it is much more than knowing multiplication procedures and number
facts and support this with some observations from our recent and
on-going research.
The proficiency of understanding
We believe that the development of genuine multiplicative thinking
(i.e., more than remembering and recalling number facts) has been
hindered through the teaching of procedures at the expense of conceptual
understanding. Later we will provide examples of what we mean by this.
The Australian Curriculum: Mathematics (Australian Curriculum Assessment
and Reporting Authority--ACARA, 2015) has adopted four proficiencies (or
actions in and with which students should engage), one of which is
Understanding. Conceptual understanding is described by Kilpatrick,
Swafford, and Findell (2001) as being "... an integrated and
functional grasp of mathematical ideas. Students with conceptual
understanding know more than isolated facts and methods" (p. 118).
Kilpatrick et al. (2001) expressed that conceptual understanding is
indicated through a capacity to represent mathematical situations in
different ways and is related to the range and richness of the
connections made.
ACARA (2015, p. 5) describes the proficiency of Understanding in
terms of "a robust knowledge of adaptable and transferable
mathematical concepts" and where students "make connections
between related concepts" and apply their knowledge to new contexts
and situations. In short, it is about links and relationships and
knowing how ideas are connected and why processes work as they do. Where
the (procedural) Fluency proficiency requires that students choose and
use correct procedures in flexible ways, it is only when conceptual
understanding is developed that the links and connections between the
'bits' of mathematics actually allow the students to "...
see the deeper similarities between superficially unrelated
situations" (Kilpatrick, Swafford, & Findell, p. 120) and
consequently have less to 'learn'. One way of considering this
proposition is to state that learning multiplication facts and the
procedure for multiplication is the exercise of Fluency, but
multiplicative thinking is the exercise of Understanding.
Conceptual plus procedural
This is not to suggest that learning multiplication facts is not
important for the development of multiplicative thinking--the
articulation of multiplicative thinking depends to an extent on
understanding and remembering multiplication facts. Wong and Evans
(2007) made the point that the importance of automaticity of recall can
be viewed when it is absent, that is, without automaticity, learning may
stall whilst the student tries to search for the required fact.
Automaticity enables less working memory to be used on factual recall
and more on solving the problem at hand (Willingham, 2009). This gives
rise to the slightly challenging notion that although highly desirable,
conceptual understanding alone is not sufficient for mathematical
proficiency (Bratina & Krudwig, 2003) but rather that a blend of
conceptual understanding and procedural fluency is required. However, we
assert here that to maximise the effectiveness of procedural fluency, it
must be underpinned by conceptual understanding. This was supported by
Swan (2007) who argued that whilst automaticity (or rather
'recall') of number facts is highly desirable, it must be
based on conceptual understanding of number facts that is built on a
robust knowledge of links and connections between them.
Observations from the research--some results and discussion
This article reports on a small part of a current study being
conducted by the authors into children's multiplicative thinking.
For this part of the study Year 6 students participated in one-on-one
interviews to identify aspects of their multiplicative thinking.
Interviews were audio recorded and students had access to a range of
resources such as counters, bundling sticks and calculators. Due to the
limited scope of this article, we are only able to present findings
related to parts of the interview based on the theme of algorithms, the
distributive property, and linking to place value.
Algorithms, the distributive property, and linking to place value
The interview questions/tasks that specifically informed this theme
are as follows: Can you give an answer for (17 x 6)?
* Children were observed to see if they were able to calculate it
mentally or if they needed to use an algorithm.
* If they calculated mentally, they were then prompted with
"Please explain how you did it".
* If unable to arrive at an answer, the prompt was "Can you
use some of the materials (bundling sticks in sets of ten as well as
singles) to help you show what is happening in the sum?"
* If the child had difficulty they were asked if they could
demonstrate 12 x 7 for a younger child.
* If further probing was required, the same process was used with
34 x 4.
The 'crunch point'--understanding the algorithm
The interviewees were shown the card 17 x 6 and asked if they could
find an answer. (Follow-up questions were then asked as shown above.) Of
the 16 children, all but one chose to use a written vertical algorithm,
which in itself is an interesting observation about children in Year 6.
Eight children (50% of the sample) could not explain or show the written
algorithm in terms of standard place value partitioning, and they were
probed further by being asked "Can you use some of these materials
to help you show what is happening in the sum?" Bundling sticks in
groups of ten and a large number of single sticks were provided. None of
the eight children was able to use bundling materials to represent the
algorithm. Typical responses from children were to show a group of 17
sticks alongside a group of six sticks (Figure 1). There was no
depiction of the desired representation of six groups of 17 sticks
(Figure 2) which might reflect an understanding of the standard place
value partition and/or the distributive property.
[FIGURE 1 OMITTED]
[FIGURE 2 OMITTED]
Other probing questions asked of children included "What does
the 'four' mean in your sum?" One student, Angelica,
said, "That was the 7 x 6 = 42", and when asked why the four
was written at the top, she said, "Add it to the six, the six times
one". It is interesting (but perhaps not surprising) that she said
'six times one' rather than 'six times ten'. In
contrast, Tilly, who correctly used the algorithm, demonstrated a sound
understanding of the procedure by saying, "That's forty that I
carried from the first column and that I added to the sixty".
Similarly, Christie described her method as "I did 7 x 6 = 42 and
then 1 x 6, which is actually 60". She was also able to relate it
to the bundling sticks by saying "Take six groups of ten and six
groups of seven, and make the 42 into four tens and two ones".
Angelica's comment along with the vignettes that follow seem to
demonstrate the concern expressed by Young-Loveridge and Mills (2005, p.
641) that "An emphasis on procedural knowledge and rules (without
understanding), as reflected in the use of algorithmic approaches to
multiplication, may undermine conceptual understanding". Students
Tilly and Christie certainly appear to have developed a measure of
conceptual understanding while student Angelica and most of the others
whose vignettes follow have not appeared to have done so.
[ILLUSTRATION OMITTED]
Student Rhiannon
Rhiannon set out her work as three separate calculations of 17 x 2
and added the three answers of 34 to incorrectly get 106. When asked if
it was right she did not identify the '6' as being wrong but
changed each of the '3's to make 24 and added them to get 76.
She explained her reason for doing it as "I find the two times
tables very easy--you just have to double the number, I did 17 x 2 three
times and added it all up". She seems to have learned a procedure
for doubling and has stuck to that as the method for doing calculations
whenever she can.
[ILLUSTRATION OMITTED]
When probed further with the 34 x 4 example and asked if she could
show how to do it, she said, "In the right way or the wrong
[way]"? She said that knowing that the 3 represented 30 and that it
was in tens column would help her do the sum "very much".
First she did it in one line and incorrectly arrived at 34 x 4 = 128 and
explained that if you did 3 x 4 = 12 and 4 x 4 =16 and added the two to
get 28 you would be wrong. She then did it as 34 x 2 twice and added the
two result as a vertical addition algorithm (carried the 1) 68 + 68 =
136. She used the same doubling procedure as for the 17 x 6 example.
Student Jane
[ILLUSTRATION OMITTED]
Jane also used an algorithmic approach. Initially she mentally
calculated 6 x 7 = 42 and wrote this down (see left). She then
calculated 1 x 6 = 6 and wrote the digit 6 in front of the 42 to make
the number 642. This suggests some confusion about partitioning the
number 17 and she may be treating the digits 1 and 7 as single digit
numbers. Her understanding could hardly be described as robust.
Student Jacinta
[ILLUSTRATION OMITTED]
Jacinta first mentally calculated 12 x 6 to get 72, then added
another 6 each time, finger counting at each stage. She went from 72 to
78 and recorded 13 showing it to be the 13th multiple of 6, then 78 to
84 (14th multiple) then to 100 not recognising her error. When asked why
she did it that way, "That works best for me". She was then
asked, "What have you seen other people do?" Her response was,
"I don't really like to look at other people's
work". This raises some interesting observations. First, she was
easily able to recall the number fact 12 x 6 = 72 but had to count on
from there. Second, her final comment suggests that there may not be
much discussion and sharing of strategies in her mathematics classroom.
Student Letitia
Letitia used a vertical algorithm and arrived at an answer of 121.
Her method, as described by her was "6 goes into 7 once, write down
1 and carry 1 and add it to the 1 in the 17. Six times two is twelve so
the answer is 121," To probe her understanding, she was given the
example 13 x 4, This time, her method was different, She said "4
times 3 is 12, write down the 2 and carry 1". She then added it to
the 1 in the 13 to get 2 and said "4 x 2 is 8", Answer = 82.
[ILLUSTRATION OMITTED]
[ILLUSTRATION OMITTED]
She was asked to try the same example again (13 x 4), This time she
did the same working and arrived at an answer of 92, These responses
illustrate considerable confusion in the student's understanding of
the mathematics involved in the algorithm, It suggests that she is
working with algorithms without the necessary conceptual understanding,
Student Lenny
[ILLUSTRATION OMITTED]
When asked if he could do 17 x 6, his first reaction was "Not
off the top of my head", He used a vertical algorithm and described
it procedurally, He was unsure of number facts for 6 x 7; "Is it
48, no 49", He was quite confused when explaining what the 1 in the
17 was worth: "What do you mean?" [then] "It's worth
5, no it's worth 1", Asked about the carried four, he said
"That came from the 7 x 6 part; it's worth four", The
question was repeated and he still said four ones,
Lenny was shown the bundling sticks including the bundles of ten,
"You can't use sticks because it's not a multiple of
ten,,,sorry, a factor of ten",
He said he couldn't use the sticks to show the sum, He was
asked if he could to do it for 12 x 7 (to show a younger child) but said
he couldn't,
Student Holly
[ILLUSTRATION OMITTED]
Out of the sixteen students who were interviewed, Holly was unique
in this choice of method, For 17 x 6 she first worked out 20 x 6 = 120
then took away 18 which is 6 x 3 to get 102, She talked about
'crossing out the zero and then doing the sum and adding back the
zero', When asked "What happens when you put a zero onto a
number? What does it do to it?", she said "It makes it part of
the ten times table or the five times table", She was probed
further; "When you put a zero on how much bigger does it get?"
She said "Ten times bigger ... then if you added another zero it
would be 100 times",
In order to further probe her understanding, she was given 47 x 6,
"I round it to the nearest number (50) that would bring it to 50 x
6, Five times 6 is 30, add the zero and equals 300, It is 47 so you go
300 - 18 = 282",
For further probing, she was given 147 x 8, She tried to do this in
the same way and became 'lost' as the numbers she had to round
were too big, She persevered but concluded "Oh wow, this is a
harder one", It was suggested that she try the algorithm and she
did it correctly and explained the procedure very clearly, However, when
asked what the carried numbers were worth, she said they were units,
Linking to place value
The sixteen students who were interviewed seemed to have a robust
recall of multiplication facts, and where initial errors may have been
made these were self-corrected, Further, many were later able to apply
these multiplication facts in the construction and solution of a two-by
one-digit multiplication vertical algorithm, However, as is seen from
the above vignettes, this facility to recall multiplication facts was
not an indicator that the students had a conceptual understanding of
multiplication, and could therefore be considered to be multiplicative
thinkers,
It may seem that this misunderstanding is more indicative of a lack
of place value knowledge but as Major (2012) stated, there is a complex
multiplicative relationship embedded in place value, Thompson (2009)
asserted that place value development runs through three phases: unitary
value understanding, quantity value understanding, and then column value
understanding, This third understanding, column value understanding, is
an important pre-requisite for multiplicative thinking (Thomas, 2004),
It is apparent that many of the students whose thinking is described in
the vignettes lack this column value understanding and appear unable to
determine if their answers obtained through the use of the algorithm are
correct or otherwise. However, it is clear that some of the students in
the sample do have a measure of conceptual understanding and are able to
articulate the correct value of a digit in terms of its column value.
There is a circular piece of reasoning to be followed at this point
as, not only does column value understanding support multiplicative
thinking, multiplicative understanding supports column value
understanding. Further, there is an argument by Graveiimeijer and van
Galen (2003) to suggest that a combination of remembering basic
multiplication facts and a conceptual understanding of multiplication
are both required to move the students through quantity value into
column value understanding. The vignettes provided in this article
contain evidence that the mastery of the procedural elements of
multiplication does not guarantee that multiplicative thinking will be
fully developed.
Conclusions
It is apparent from the observations described here that the
students in this study have a degree of mathematical fluency with
multiplication, but thai at least half of them do not have strong
conceptual understanding of the same. Please let us state again that we
support the need for this fluency and that knowing multiplication facts
is of great value. Yet the evidence collected from the students shows
that knowing these facts alone does not provide them a suitable base for
a whole range of further mathematical understandings. Just knowing
multiplication facts does not make a student a multiplicative thinker.
However, some of the students showed that it is entirely possible to
develop a deeper understanding of the multiplicative situation. These
students were conversant with multiplication facts and also displayed a
good level of conceptual understanding of the multiplication algorithm.
Further, they were able to illustrate their understanding using concrete
materials. Some students showed an awareness of alternative methods of
computation (from the algorithm) but their understanding could hardly be
described as being robust. Significantly, it is likely that at least
some of the students involved in the interviews have been introduced to
the written multiplication algorithm without the underpinning conceptual
understanding based on place value.
References
Australian Curriculum, Assessment and Reporting Authority [ACARA].
(2015). Australian Curriculum: Mathematics, Retrieved from
http://www.australiancurriculum.edu.au/ Mathematics/Rationale
Bratina, T. A., & Krudwig, K. M. (2003). Get it right and get
it fast! Building automaticity to strengthen mathematical proficiency.
Focus On Learning Problems In Mathematics, 25(3), 47-63.
Graveiimeijer, K., & van Galen, F. (2003). Facts and algorithms
as products of student's own mathematical activity. In J.
Kilpatrick, W. G. Martin & D. Schifter (Eds.), A research companion
to principles and standards for school mathematics, 114-122. Reston, VA:
National Council of Teachers of Mathematics.
Hurst, C., & Hurrell, D. (2015). Developing the big ideas of
number. International Journal Of Educational Studies In Mathematics,
1(1), 1-17.
Kilpatrick, J., Swafford, J., & Findell, B. (2001). Adding it
up: Helping children learn mathematics, Washington, DC: National Academy
Press.
Major, K. (2012). The development of an assessment tool: Student
knowledge of the concept of place value. In J. Dindyal, L. P. Cheng
& S. F. Ng (Eds.), Mathematics education: Expanding horizons
(Proceedings of the 35th annual conference of the Mathematics Education
Research Group of Australasia). Singapore: MERGA.
Siemon, D., Bleckly, J., & Neal, D. (2012). Working with the
big ideas in number and the Australian Curriculum: Mathematics. In B.
Atweh, M. Goos, R. Jorgensen & D. Siemon, (Eds.). (2012). Engaging
the Australian National Curriculum: Mathematics--perspectives from the
field, Online Publication: Mathematics Education Research Group of
Australasia. 19-45.
Siemon, D., Breed, M., Dole, S., Izard, J., & Virgona, J.
(2006). Scaffolding numeracy in the middle years--Project findings,
materials, and resources, Final Report submitted to Victorian Department
of Education and Training and the Tasmanian Department of Education,
Retrieved from http://www.eduweb.vic.gov.au/edulibrary/public/teachlearn/student/snmy.ppt
Swan, P. (2007). Tackling tables: Using a strategies approach (2nd
ed.). Perth: A-Z Type.
Thomas, N. (2004). The development of structure in the number
system, Paper presented at the 28th Conference of the International
Group for the Psychology of Mathematics Education, Bergen, Norway.
Thompson, I. (2009). Place Value? Mathematics Teaching, (215), 4-5.
Willingham, D. (2009). Why don't students like school?
California: Jossey-Bass.
Wong, M., & Evans, D. (2007). Improving basic multiplication
fact recall for primary school students. Mathematics Education Research
Journal, 19(1), 89-106.
Young-Loveridge, J., & Mills. (2009). Teaching multi-digit
multiplication using array-based materials. In R. Hunter, B. Bicknell
& T. Burgess (Eds.), Crossing divides (proceedings of the 32nd
annual conference of the Mathematics Education Research Group of
Australasia, 635-643. Palmerston North, NZ, MERGA.
Chris Hurst
Curtin University, Australia
<C.Hurst@curtin.edu.au>
Derek Hurrell
University of Notre Dame, Australia
<derek.hurrell@nd.edu.au>