Promoting reasoning through the magic V task.
Bragg, Leicha A. ; Loong, Esther Yook-Kin ; Widjaja, Wanty 等
Reasoning in mathematics plays a critical role in developing
mathematical understandings. In this article, Bragg, Loong, Widjaja,
Vale & Herbert explore an adaptation of the Magic V Task and how it
was used in several classrooms to promote and develop reasoning skills.
The importance of reasoning
Mathematical reasoning is the foundation of deep understanding
(Bragg, et al., 2013). Adaptive reasoning is viewed as "the glue
that holds everything together, the lodestar that guides learning"
(National Research Council, 2001, p. 129). The importance of reasoning
is noticeable in its inclusion as an explicit learning requirement of
many nations' curriculum documents (Loong, Vale, Bragg &
Herbert, 2013) including the Australian Curriculum: Mathematics
(Australian Curriculum, Assessment and Reporting Authority [ACARA],
2013) where it is one of four designated proficiency strands defined as:
Students develop an increasingly sophisticated
capacity for logical thought and actions,
such as analysing, proving, evaluating,
explaining, inferring, justifying and generalising.
Students are reasoning mathematically
when they explain their thinking, when
they deduce and justify strategies used and
conclusions reached, when they adapt the
known to the unknown, when they transfer
learning from one context to another, when
they prove that something is true or false and
when they compare and contrast related ideas
and explain their choices. (p. 5).
Opportunities to reason should commence at the earliest opportunity
for children and as they progress, their reasoning should become more
sophisticated when supported by teachers through a systematic approach
(Stacey, 2013). The Mathematics Reasoning Research Group at Deakin
University developed the Mathematical Reasoning Professional Learning
Research Program [MRPLRP] to support and further teachers'
knowledge of reasoning to foster the critical engagement of their
students in mathematical reasoning. In this article, we describe our
adaptation of the Magic V task (http://nrich. maths.org/6274), in the
second of two reasoning lessons demonstrated in three Victorian primary
schools and one Canadian elementary school in our project to assist
primary teachers to promote and support mathematical reasoning in middle
and upper primary classes. See Bragg, et al. (2013) for a full
description of the first reasoning lesson called "What else
belongs?"
The Magic V task
The Magic V is a task which affords children an opportunity to
develop and test conjectures and form generalisations (Widjaja, 2014).
The Magic V explores mathematical reasoning giving children the
opportunity to, "Investigate and use the properties of odd and even
numbers (ACMNA071)" (ACARA, 2013, p. 30). Specific learning
objectives addressing reasoning for this demonstration lesson within the
MRPLRP included but were not exclusive to: use oral language for
equivalence and equivalent number sentences to record, explain and
justify solutions; compare and contrast to generalise and develop ideas
(conjectures); test ideas (justifying and proving); trial to form
conjectures (inductive reasoning); develop a logical argument based on
an understanding of equivalence and properties of odd and even numbers
(deductive reasoning). There was also an emphasis on developing
mathematical language, such as "equals" and "does not
equal", as the children explored and explained properties of odd
and even numbers. Skills associated with problem-solving, such as
applying systematic trial and error to seek solutions, were also
supported in this lesson.
The lesson commences with the teacher referring to the two Vs on
the board (see Figure 1a and 1b) and inviting the children to share what
is the same about the Vs.
[FIGURE 1 OMITTED]
Typical responses are "Number 1 to 5 are used in both
Vs", "5, 3 and 2 are in the same spots" and "All the
numbers add up to 15." Once the sameness of the two Vs is
exhausted, the teacher invites the children to share what is different
about the two Vs. The typical initial response is, "4 and 2 are in
different spots", and is often followed by a student noticing,
"The arms on one V don't add to the same as the arms on the
other V." Such a response helps draws the children's focus to
the essence of what makes one V magical and the other non-magical, that
is, a Magic V is when the numbers on one arm add up to the same as the
numbers on the other arm. For example in Figure 1a, the arm 4 + 2 + 3 is
equal to the sum of the arm 5 + 1 + 3; both arms are equal to 9. Whereas
in Figure 1b, the arm 1 + 2 + 3 is not equal to the sum of the arm 5 + 4
+ 3.
If the students have failed to notice the symmetry of the arms in
the Magic V, the teacher can offer enabling prompts such as, "I
think there is something interesting going on with these Vs. We have
been thinking about the numbers just by themselves. Can you think about
ways we might add these numbers? [Pause] Can you add the numbers in the
arms? What do you notice?" Once the total of the arms have been
explored the teacher points to the Magic V and explains this is the
preferred V, and the Magic V will be the focus of the lesson and removes
the non-magical V from the board. This discussion establishes the rule
for testing or verifying whether a V is magic or not and links to
specific actions in the Australian Curriculum: Mathematics definition of
reasoning, such as, "analysing, proving, evaluating,
explaining" (ACARA, 2013, p. 5)
The teacher poses the challenge to the children, "I wonder if
we can make more Magic Vs with the numbers 1 to 5?" In pairs, the
children are given a set of numbers, a Magic V mat (see Figure 2) and a
record sheet to record all the Magic Vs they discover. The teacher
reminds the children of the classroom norms for working together, such
as sharing materials; the task of recording; explaining their thinking
to each other; and checking and convincing each other that they have
found a Magic V before they commence finding the next one.
The teacher records the various strategies adopted by the children
to collate the Magic Vs. Key observational points for the teacher to
note is if the children are specialising, that is, trying out particular
cases.
[FIGURE 2 OMITTED]
Students will initially explore possibilities with placing the
numbers into the circles, checking if the two totals are equal, then
moving specific numbers around to ensure the two arms balance. The
children may begin to notice relationships and consider switching
positions of the numbers to change the order but keep the same
combination; that is, the children may realise the commutative property
for addition as they explore all possible Magic Vs with the same number
at the vertex. Conversely, the children may consider different
combinations and begin to generalise from the specific examples thus
employing inductive reasoning (Holton, Stacey, & FitzSimons, 2012),
that is, notice common attributes in the combinations or placement of
numbers in the vertex. For example, that their Magic Vs have an odd
number at the vertex. With a systematic approach children can find 24
different Magic Vs from the set of numbers 1 to 5. You can create eight
Magic Vs with the number 1 at the vertex and the arms equalling 8, eight
Magic Vs with the number 3 at the vertex and the arms equalling 9, and
eight Magic Vs with the number 5 at the vertex and the arms equalling
10.
[FIGURE 3 OMITTED]
After ten minutes has elapsed the teacher makes an announcement,
"I am going to give you a large blank V sheet [the same style as
Figure 1a but without the numbers] and I would like you to record one of
your Magic Vs on the sheet with a coloured marker and place it on the
board. Check that you have a different Magic V to one already
displayed." Once each pair has added their Magic V to the board
(see Figure 3) the whole class come together to share their experiences
and what they have noticed about the Magic V.
The teacher asks a pair of students, "Point to the Magic V you
have placed on the board. How do you know this is a Magic V?"
Depending on the response of the children, the teacher either notes on
the board examples of number sentences with or without the vertex
included; such as 3 + 4 = 5 + 2 or 5 + 1 = 4 + 2 without the vertex, or
alternatively 3 + 4 + 1 = 5 + 2 + 1 or 5 + 1 + 3 = 4 + 2 + 3 with the
vertex included. The absence of the vertex in the first number sentences
format emphasises that the number in the vertex could be ignored,
thereby drawing the focus of the children's noticing to the arms.
The teacher asks children to share their process for selecting
numbers in particular spaces. "Share with us. How did you find your
Magic V?" Anticipated responses from the children include, "We
mixed the numbers around until we found one", or "We switched
the numbers in order like this ...". The second comment denotes a
systematic approach to the discovering and recording process. The
teacher now shifts the children's attention to the board to examine
the Magic Vs created by the class, "Look at all these different
Magic Vs. What do you notice about the Magic Vs we found?" The
teacher records the children's observations and conjectures on the
board. Responses include: "These two Magic Vs are the same but with
the numbers swapped," "All the numbers in the vertex [bottom/
corner/ point] are 1, 3 or 5", "All the numbers in the vertex
are odd numbers," "It's impossible to have 2 [or 4] in
the vertex," "It's impossible to have an even number in
the vertex." Noticing what is the same about the Magic Vs is a
necessary step for the children to form conjectures about the properties
of Magic Vs as illustrated in these statements. The development of
students' capacity to form and test conjectures is a central
component of reasoning (Carpenter, Franke, & Levi, 2003). When a
child notices that the vertex only has 1, 3 or 5 (odd numbers), the
teacher moves the children's examples on the board to collate those
with the same number in the vertex [1, 3 or 5] together thus forming
three groups.
The teacher challenges the children to consider this conjecture,
"[Child's name] noticed that all the numbers at the vertex are
odd. I wonder why all our examples are odd? I wonder if they could be
even?" The teacher shares the following questions on a worksheet
with the children, "Sam said "It is impossible to make a Magic
V with an even number at the bottom with the set of numbers 1 to
5". Is Sam right? Explain why or why not? [You can use sentences,
number sentences and drawings in your explanation.]"
The children return to their desks to test Sam's conjecture
while the teacher roves to observe the children's reasoning and to
provide enabling prompts if required. Enabling prompts may consist of
the following comments and actions. "What happened when you tried 2
or 4 at the bottom of the V? I wonder why 2 and 4 does not work?"
The teacher covers the number at the vertex of the V to focus students
on the 4 other numbers and asks "What do you notice about these
numbers in the arms of the V?" [pause] "What do the other 4
numbers add up to?" [pause] "How is this different when it is
an odd number at the bottom/vertex?" The teacher may remove the
numbers in the arms off the sheet, leaving a number at the vertex, and
place the four remaining numbers in a row. "What do you notice
about the total when you add these numbers together: 1, 2, 4 and 5; 2,
3, 4 and 5; 1, 2, 3 and 4?" [pause] "How is this different for
1, 3, 4 and 5; and 1, 2, 3 and 5?"
During this roving time the teacher records observations such as:
How are students testing the conjecture? Are they exploring combinations
of odd and even numbers? Are they trialling different combinations with
even numbers in the vertex? Are they recording number sentences? Are
they exploring expressions for equivalence? Do they ignore the number in
the vertex? Are they thinking about the four numbers that could be
placed in the arms? Are they thinking about the total of all numbers?
What explanations are students developing? What language are they using
when forming a conclusion?
After approximately 10 minutes of time for students to explore
Sam's conjecture the teacher brings the children back to the floor
to share findings in a final discussion. The teacher asks the following
question, "Thumbs up if you agreed with Sam? Thumbs down if you
disagreed with Sam? Thumbs sideways if you are not sure or not
convinced?" and quickly and effectively gauges the general feeling
of the class. Of the children who are not sure or not convinced the
teacher invites them to share why. Typically these unconvinced children
seek more proof and respond with, "I need to test more
combinations."
On the board the teacher places movable numbers from 1 to 5 (seen
in Figure 2 and backed with blu-tac) and two blank Vs on the board, one
with a 2 at the vertex and the other with a 4. The children are invited
to use this support material to demonstrate their justifications. The
teacher selects students who have a range of different approaches, e.g.,
trial by exhausting all possibilities, students who notice the arms
maybe odd on one side and even on other side, or students who notice you
need to divide the remaining numbers by two. Figure 4 illustrates one
pair's justification of the Sam conjecture.
[FIGURE 4 OMITTED]
The teacher invites the first selected pair who have the least
complex reason for agreeing with Sam, using trial and error, up to the
board to share their reason why even numbers do not work for this set of
numbers. The teacher records all responses on the board. The teacher
opens an invitation to the class, "Can anyone else add to this
explanation?" Through inviting children to "add to this
explanation" the work from the previous pair is not diminished but
rather each pair's offering is viewed as collaborating to building
a shared understanding together.
The teacher targets children who have a more complex reason than
trial and error, "Why did you think it was not possible?" The
teacher continues to select pairs whose complexity towards advanced
thinking would progress as follows:
1. Trial and error. Some or all of the number sentences are listed.
2. Noticing one arm is 1 number different to the other arm, e.g., 8
vs 9.
3. Noticing even numbers cannot make a Magic V because they make an
even and an odd number on each arm.
4. Identifies total of four numbers is odd when an even number is
at the vertex; whereas total of four numbers is even when an odd number
is at the vertex.
5. Generalises that when an even number is at the vertex, you
cannot divide the total of the remaining numbers in two evenly and
therefore Sam is correct.
The teacher continues until the reasons are exhausted or the
justification is complete (see Widjaja, 2014, for an analysis of the
students' justifications), that is, the total of the four numbers
must equal an even number so that they can divide evenly by two leaving
a whole number. Therefore, the vertex must be an odd number, leaving two
odd and two even numbers on the arms.
At the conclusion of the lesson, the children are invited to
consolidate and reflect upon their reasoning in their mathematics
journal and share any of their own questions for further exploration.
Conclusion
The Magic V lesson tasks affords children with opportunities to
reason through carefully selected prompts and models. The teacher uses
the final shared discussion time to emphasise the importance of
comparing and contrasting for forming conjectures; encouraging children
to develop skills in testing, proving, and justifying conjectures, and
building and consolidating children's understanding of why an odd
number is at the vertex of the Magic V for this set of numbers. Each
discussion provides further opportunity for children to learn from their
peers' reasoning, examples, conjectures, explanations and to use
and connect mathematical ideas. The children's noticing of testing,
justifying, proving and generalisations emerges from looking for the
differences and similarities within the solutions, and noticing and
sharing these solutions with each other. For further tasks to support
reasoning, please review the Australian Association of Mathematics
Teachers Top Drawer Teachers site (2013).
Leicha A. Bragg
Deakin University
<leicha.bragg@deakm.edu.au>
Esther Yook-Kin Loong
Deakin University
Wanty Widjaja
Deakin University
Colleen Vale
Deakin University
Sandra Herbert
Deakin University
References
Australian Association of Mathematics Teachers [AAMT] (2013).
Reasoning. Retrieved July 31, 2014 from http://
topdrawer.aamt.edu.au/Reasoning
Australian Curriculum and Assessment Authority (2013). Australian
Curriculum: Mathematics. Retrieved July 31, 2014 from
http://www.australiancurriculum.edu.au/ Mathematics/Rationale
Bragg, L. A., Vale, C., Herbert, S., Loong, E., Widjaja, W.,
Williams, G. & Mousley, J. (2013). Promoting awareness of reasoning
in the primary mathematics classroom. In A. McDonough, A. Downton, &
L. A. Bragg, (Eds.), Mathematics of the planet earth: Proceedings of the
MAV50th Annual Conference, pp. 23-30, Mathematical Association of
Victoria, Melbourne, Vic.
Carpenter, T. P., Franke, M. L., & Levi, L. (2003). Thinking
mathematically: Integrating arithmetic and algebra in elementary school.
Portsmouth: Heinamann.
Holton, D., Stacey, K., & FitzSimons, G. (2012). Reasoning a
dog's tale. Australian Mathematics Teacher, 68(3), 22-26.
Loong, E., Vale, C., Bragg, L. A. & Herbert, S. (2013). Primary
school teachers' perceptions of mathematical reasoning. In V.
Steinle, L. Ball & C. Bardini, (Eds.), Mathematics education:
Yesterday, today and tomorrow. Proceedings of the 36th annual conference
of Mathematics Education Research Group of Australiasia pp. 466-473.
Melbourne: MERGA.
National Research Council (2001). Adding it up: Helping children
learn mathematics. Washington, D.C.: National Academy Press.
Stacey, K. (2012). Why reasoning? Australian Primary Mathematics
Classroom, 17(2), 16-17.
Widjaja, W. (2014). Year 3/4 children's forms of
justification. In J. Anderson, M. Cavanagh & A. Prescott (Eds.),
Curriculum in focus: Research guided practice. Proceedings of the 37th
annual conference of the Mathematics Education Research Group of
Australasia. pp. 694-697. Sydney: MERGA.