Sports mental toughness questionnaire: evaluation for use in South African competitive tennis.
Cowden, Richard G. ; Meyer-Weitz, Anna
Sports mental toughness questionnaire: evaluation for use in South African competitive tennis.
The consensus that mental toughness (MT), as a psychological
construct, is an indispensible factor for athletic achievement, success,
and excellence (Clough, Earle, & Sewell, 2002; Crust, 2007; Jones,
Hanton, & Connaughton, 2007; Loehr, 1986) has increased interest
towards not only developing a comprehensive understanding of the
construct, but developing appropriate instruments to assess MT. Accurate
MT appraisals have prospective implications for examining areas
warranting improvement as well as assessments of MT following
intervention efforts, particularly due to the developmental trajectory
of MT over time and through experiences (Connaughton, Hanton, &
Jones, 2010) as well as through direct psychological training and
intervention efforts (Bell, Hardy, & Beattie, 2013).
Although several general (e.g., Mental Toughness Questionnaire 48
[MTQ48]; Clough et al., 2002) and sport-specific MT inventories (e.g.,
Cricket Mental Toughness Inventory [CMTI]; Gucciardi & Gordon, 2009)
have been developed, some (e.g., Psychological Performance Inventory
[PPI]; Loehr, 1986) have demonstrated inadequate psychometric properties
and have been devoid of a theoretical basis for item inclusion
(Middleton et al., 2004). In addition, there are suggestions that MT
develops differently according to sport-type involvement (Gucciardi,
2009) and has distinct requirements based on particular types of sports
or selected periods of sport participation (Bull, Shambrook, James,
& Brooks, 2005). This has resulted in the development of
sport-specific MT inventories to adequately appraise MT according to
sport-type MT manifestations and demands.
One sport that has received insufficient attention and for which a
sport-specific MT instrument has yet to be developed is tennis.
Considering the conditions, nature of participation, and demands
associated with tennis differ markedly from other sports, it is
important that an instrument be examined and validated for use with
tennis athletes. In this study, the decision to select an inventory that
has been afforded prior psychometric support is based on two reasons.
The first is that instruments that have been validated using athletes
from a variety of sporting backgrounds may not exhibit the same factor
structure or item loadings among a group of athletes from one type of
sport, especially if MT differs according to sport type. The second is
that although the instrument that has been selected for use in this
study (i.e., Sport Mental Toughness Questionnaire [SMTQ]; Sheard et al.,
2009) is a multidimensional measure of MT and has undergone extensive
psychometric validation and support, subsequent examinations have
resulted in the suggestion of further psychometric evaluation (Crust
& Swann, 2011). Therefore, accurate sport-specific appraisals of
athlete MT require the assessment and validation of individual
instruments for appropriate adaptation and use in such contexts. Hence,
the purpose of the present study is to examine the psychometric
properties of a previously validated MT instrument for prospective
adaptation and use amid competitive tennis players.
Method
Participants
A total of 365 competitive tennis athletes participated in the
study. The participant demographics are outlined in Table 1. The
participants were required to have engaged in some form of tennis
competition within the last two weeks and must have engaged in tennis
participation for a minimum of five years prior to their participation
in the study. These two criteria were considered necessary in order to
operationally define the term competitive, which would more likely
enable the evaluation of a specific sub-group of tennis participants and
maintain congruence among all participants. Additionally, the criteria
were considered important for reducing the potential for confounds to
influence the results and subsequent interpretation of the findings. For
instance, if the requirement to include athletes currently competing was
omitted, the degree to which participant scores on the various measures
were influenced by the act of engaging in competition or absence of
competitive engagement could not be determined.
Participants were recruited through local and national tennis
organisations, universities, high schools, professional coaches,
tournaments, and county clubs. University ethical approval from the
University of Kwazulu-Natal's Institutional Review Board was
obtained before conducting the study. All participants were provided and
completed an informed consent document prior to their participation.
Materials
The Sports Mental Toughness Questionnaire (SMTQ; Sheard et al.,
2009) was used to assess participants' MT. The SMTQ is a
multidimensional measurement of MT that is based on the qualitative
constituents of MT consistently identified in the MT literature (Sheard,
2013). The primary reason for selecting the SMTQ was based on the
demonstrated construct validity of the inventory resulting from
exploratory and confirmatory factor analyses, adequate internal
reliability, divergent validity, and discriminative ability findings. In
fact, Sheard (2013) asserts that the SMTQ is only one of two MT
instruments (the other being the PPI-A) that have been extensively
evaluated using EFA and CFA and acquired support from the analyses.
The SMTQ is comprised of three subscales (confidence, constancy,
and control) that combine for a general measure of MT. Sheard et al.
(2009), in their CFA, evidenced strong support for the hierarchical
three-factor model, with a goodness-of-fit index (GFI) of .95 suggesting
good model fit. The coefficients between the higher-order factor of
total MT and second-order factors of confidence (r = .72), constancy (r
= .71), and control (r = .66) were considered acceptable. Correlations
between confidence and control, confidence and constancy, and constancy
and control were reportedly .28, .31, and .31, respectively, all of
which were statistically significant (Sheard et al., 2009). Internal
consistency for global MT on the SMTQ was strong ([alpha] = .81).
Additionally, internal consistency for the subfactors was considered
moderate to strong (confidence = .79, constancy = .76, control = .72).
Providing evidence for the divergent validity of the measure,
correlations between the SMTQ and the subscales on the Life Orientation
Test, Personal View Survey II1-R, and the Positive and Negative Affect
Schedule were moderate and ranged from .23-.38,. 14-.33, and .12-.49,
respectively (Sheard et al., 2009). The researchers also reported the
discriminative power of the SMTQ based on statistically meaningful
differences between athletes of dissimilar competitive levels, age, and
gender, with higher scores for more advanced competitive levels, for
older athletes, and males.
The SMTQ contains 14-items rated on a Likert-type scale, anchored
between 1 (not at all true) and 4 (very true). Sample items on the three
subfactors include "I interpret potential threats as positive
opportunities" (confidence), "I give up in difficult
situations" (constancy), and "I am overcome by
self-doubt" (control).
Procedure
Permission letters were obtained from various tennis organisations
to access the database of tennis athletes or the contact information of
the athletes affiliated with such organisations. The informed consent
document and questionnaire were placed into an online survey format for
completion and relevant local, provincial, and national organisations
were provided the survey link and requested to forward the link to
appropriate competitive tennis players attached to such organisations.
The online survey link initially delineated informed consent
elements and requested the participants' agreement to the informed
consent elements prior to proceeding with the questionnaire items. The
participants were provided details of the study along with ethical
obligations and procedures during the informed consent process, and,
once informed consent agreement was attained, the participants'
proceeded through the questionnaire items that mirrored the hard copy
completion process.
Appropriate tennis tournaments were also identified and approached
in order to obtain additional participants. Permission was obtained to
attend the tournaments to engage the tennis participants and obtain the
required data, with the questionnaires administered to the players in
groups of convenient sizes based on their availability (approximately 5
to 10 players at a time). Informed consent was discussed and obtained
from the participants, following which the self-administered
questionnaire was distributed to the participants. This questionnaire
completion process required approximately 15 to 20 minutes from the
participants.
Results
Principal component analysis (PCA) was selected to evaluate the
factor structure of the SMTQ. The scree plot indicated the presence of
three components prior to the flattening of the line across the points.
These three points also evidenced eigenvalues of greater than one. As a
result, the number of factors that were specified for extraction in the
subsequent factor structure analysis was fixed to three.
PCA with a factor extraction limit of three (using a Direct Oblimin
rotation) was conducted with the original 14 questionnaire items. The
pattern matrix was examined to determine items that did not possess
clear or adequate loadings onto specific factors. One of the more common
approaches to assessing the appropriateness of item-factor loadings is
to apply loading cut-off criteria to the rotated factor solution
results. That is, in order for an item to be retained in the factor
structure, the highest item-factor loading should fall above the
established criterion. According to Matsunaga (2010), a loading
criterion of an absolute value of .4 is a liberal yet appropriate
cut-off value, which was selected for application in this study,
primarily because the questionnaire had undergone prior validation and
is an established instrument. The pattern matrix with item loadings is
displayed in Table 2. Although a number of items evidenced factor
loadings close to an absolute value of .4, the application of this
cutolf criterion resulted in the retention of each item. The retention
of all the SMTQ items was followed by further examination of the PCA
results.
The Barlett's Test of Sphericity was statistically
significant, [[chi square].sub.(91)] = 1010.796, p < .001. This
rejection of the null hypothesis suggests the presence of correlations
based on the items included in the analysis, and, as a result, denotes
the appropriateness of the data for proceeding with PCA. In addition,
the Kaiser-Meyer-Olkin (KMO) value was calculated to evaluate the
sampling adequacy of the dataset. In this instance, KMO was .796, which,
according to the general rules outlined by Hutcheson and Sofroniou
(1999), is within an acceptable range for proceeding with PCA.
The total variance explained by Factor 1, Factor 2, and Factor 3
were 26.05%, 13.21%, and 8.21%, respectively, with a total variance of
47.48% explained by the three factors. Examining the pattern matrix that
converged with 18 iterations, six items loaded on Factor 1, six items
loaded onto Factor 2, and two items loaded onto Factor 3 (see Table 2).
The PCA item-factor loadings for each scale and subscale were used to
generate the revised measures, with the items that loaded onto each
factor summated for subscale totals. The three subfactors were labelled
confidence/self-efficacy, emotional/cognitive control, and positive
perspective, respectively. Cronbach's alpha for total MT was .771
(all 14 items), with internal consistency estimates (and mean inter-item
correlations) for confidence/self-efficacy (6 items), emotional/
cognitive control (6 items), and positive perspective (2 items) .720
(.299), .739 (.324), and .420 (.266), respectively.
Pearson correlation results between the total MT scale subscales
are outlined in Table 3. In particular, total MT exhibited moderate to
strong positive and statistically significant correlations (r = .41 to
.82) with each of the revised MT subscales. In addition, there were
moderate positive and statistically correlations (r = .19 to .34)
between each of the MT subscales.
Discussion
The purpose of the present study was to examine the factor
structure and psychometric properties of a previously validated MT
instrument for adaptation and use amongst competitive tennis players in
South Africa. Although the SMTQ has been developed and validated
previously (Sheard et al., 2009), recent studies involving the SMTQ have
recommended further validation efforts towards the refinement of the
instrument (e.g., Crust & Swann, 2011). In this study, the SMTQ PCA
analysis results confirmed the original three factor structure of the
instrument (Sheard et al., 2009). However, the item-factor loadings in
this study indicated that the items included in each of the subfactors
diverge to some extent from the original SMTQ. Specifically, the results
revealed that 6 items loaded onto confidence/self-efficacy, 6 items
loaded onto emotional/cognitive control, and 2 items onto positive
perspective. This contrasts the original SMTQ item-factor loadings of
six, four, and four items on the confidence, constancy, and control
subscales, respectively.
Compared to the original SMTQ items included on each factor, the
present results differ in several ways. Two of the items that previously
loaded onto the confidence subscale ("I interpret potential threats
as positive opportunities" and "I have an unshakeable
confidence in my ability") loaded onto positive perspective, two of
the items that previously loaded onto the constancy subscale ("I am
committed to completing the tasks I have to do" and "I take
responsibility for setting myself challenging targets") loaded onto
confidence/self-efficacy, and two of the items that previously loaded
onto the constancy subscale ("I give up in difficult
situations" and "I get distracted easily and lose my
concentration") loaded onto emotional/ cognitive control.
The SMTQ was previously validated using samples of competitive
athletes from a range of sports (26 different sports), whereas this
study solely included competitive tennis players. The divergence between
the present and past findings may also be influenced, in part, by the
geographic and cultural context in this study, as the original factor
structure investigation did not involve South African tennis players.
Prior studies have evidenced discrepancies between athletes'
perceptions of item relevance and importance in the context of their
particular sport. For instance, elite tennis athletes reported "not
giving up in difficult situations" as the most important MT facet
(Cowden, 2012), whereas "a winning mentality and desire" was
considered the most important component in soccer (Coulter, Mallett,
& Gucciardi, 2010). The context, participation factors, and demands
associated with particular sports may emphasise certain MT components as
opposed to others, which may explain perceptual differences and the
item-factor loading differences in this study.
The multicultural, multiracial, and broad range of tennis players
included in the current study may also provide some explanation for the
results obtained. It is recognised that certain psychological
constructs, such as intelligence, are conceptualised differently between
African and Western societies (Serpell & Haynes, 2001). Perhaps
these distinctions also apply to MT, with the possibility that certain
aspects of MT, as it applies to South Africa, are not included on the
SMTQ, and, as a result, were not examined in this study. Prior studies
have suggested cultural differences in MT among athletes involved in the
same sports, such as Bull et al.'s (2005) report of a number of
components associated with MT in English cricketers (e.g., willingness
to take risks) that were not outlined in Gucciardi and Gordon's
(2009) study involving Australian cricketers. Although MT studies in
tennis are limited, it is conceivable to suggest that tennis culture
factors, along with social influences, have an impact on the MT
manifested within South African tennis players.
Another reason for the current results may be based on the factor
structure evaluation method that was selected. That is, Sheard et al.
(2009) used CFA to evaluate the EFA identified factor structure of the
SMTQ, whereas PCA was used in this study. In addition, Crust and Swann
(2011) suggested that the some of the SMTQ subscale internal consistency
scores denoted that certain subscales were assessing more than one
construct of MT. With the potential influence of cultural perceptions
and interpretations in the current sample, the apparent heterogeneity in
the item content of selected SMTQ subscales may signify that, at least
in the sample of tennis players in this study, some of the SMTQ items
may more appropriately load onto the factors delineated.
In addition, the discrepancy between the original and current
findings may relate to the common underlying factor of MT and overall
similarities among the items, denoting that the items are all measuring
a similar underlying construct. This is supported by the strong
subscale-global factor correlations in the present study (see Table 3).
These findings compare favourably with Sheard et al.'s (2009)
report of strong subscale-global factor correlations.
Although certain items loaded differently in this study as compared
to the original SMTQ subscales, a number of the items were retained on
their original scales. Specifically, four items from the original
confidence and control subscales, respectively, loaded significantly
onto the same factors in this study, possibly denoting that particular
aspects of MT are central to the construct, regardless of culture or
sport type. Indeed, Gucciardi, Gordon, and Dimmock's (2009a)
definition of MT connotes the relevance of inherent facets as well as
the experientially developmental nature of MT, with a core MT
component/s that is relatively stable throughout situations and sport
involvement. Perhaps the core MT component/s extends cultures, genders,
and sport type involvement, with non-core MT components more likely to
fluctuate based on background, experiences, and sporting requirements.
This perspective coincides with Horsburgh, Schermer, Veselka, and
Vernon's (2009) report that MT is dependent on both biological
influences and environmental experiences, with the supposition that some
aspects of MT may be more amenable to adaptation than others.
Determining which aspects of MT are more heritable or adaptable than
others, however, is an area that needs to be explored.
As denoted, the subscale terms exhibit conceptual overlap with the
original SMTQ subscales, except for the subscale of constancy, which was
not adjudicated as an appropriate label for any of the factors revealed
in this study. In a prior study, Crust and Swann (2011) suggested that
the constancy subscale items appear to assess more than one construct
(e.g., personal responsibility and concentration), which was posited as
a potential reason for the lower internal consistency estimate for the
subscale that was found in their study. This may afford some explanation
for some of the items included on the original constancy subscale
demonstrating superior loadings with other factors.
Although the third factor, positive perspective, has limited
similarity with the original SMTQ subscales, it does possess some degree
of congruence with Clough et al.'s (2002) MTQ48 instrument, in
which a subscale--challenge--is included as a component of MT. Based on
the findings in this study, positive perspective may be likened to the
MTQ48 challenge subscale because of common thread of possessing an
optimistic outlook and interpretation of the self and events or
situations. These similarities may suggest that, despite the findings of
different SMTQ factor structure contents in this study, the components
the SMTQ appears to be measuring overlap with other measures as well as
the components of MT identified in the literature.
Contextualising the positive perspective component amid the other
MT components outlined in the study, it would appear that this
optimistic and hopeful attitude has important implications for high
self-efficacious thoughts and behaviours, such as self-perceived
confidence, the ability to control thoughts, emotions, and behaviours,
to overcome adversity, and to perceive difficulties as challenges rather
than threats. Therefore, it would appear that possessing a positive
perspective is an indispensible characteristic for being mentally tough.
Providing support for this, Nicholls, Polman, Levy, and Backhouse (2008)
evidenced greater levels of optimism and lower levels of pessimism at
higher levels of athlete MT. Due to the capacity for optimism to be
augmented (Seligman, 1990), regardless of whether optimism is a
component of MT or simply a characteristic mentally tough individuals
are more likely to possess, interventions aimed at enhancing optimism
amongst athletes may provide benefits for improving the MT of athletes
(Nicholls et al., 2008).
The internal consistency for the total SMTQ scale computed in this
study ([alpha] = .77) compares favourably with previous research
([alpha] = .70 to .81--Crust & Swann, 2011; Sheard et al., 2009).
The internal consistency the confidence/self-efficacy,
emotional/cognitive control, and positive perspective subscales were
.72, .74, and .42, respectively. Although confidence/ self-efficacy and
emotional/cognitive control exhibited acceptable internal consistency,
Cronbach's alpha for positive perspective was low. With indications
that, for scales with low item quantities, mean inter-item correlations
be reported and fall between .2 and .4 for reliability to be considered
acceptable (Briggs & Cheek, 1986), the positive perspective subscale
mean inter-item correlation was .266, which resulted in the decision to
retain the subscale.
Based on the divergence between the present results and the
original SMTQ findings reported by Sheard et al. (2009), coupled with
the lower internal consistency for the positive perspective subscale,
further SMTQ validation investigations are warranted to provide
additional evidence of the factor structure, item loadings, and support
for the use of the SMTQ amongst athletes in general and sport-specific
domains. Perhaps, Bull et al.'s (2005) supposition that different
sports require or emphasise different types of MT may have particular
relevance to the distinctions between the original SMTQ factor loadings
and those found in the present study. Indeed, researchers have begun to
examine and develop sport-specific MT instruments (e.g., Australian
rules Football [AfMTI]; Gucciardi, Gordon, & Dimmock, 2009b),
positing that MT may be best understood and measured within particular
sporting domains as opposed to more generally. The AfMTI assesses four
components of MT, including tough attitude, desire success, sport
awareness, and thrive through challenge. The CMTI, on the other hand,
assesses affective intelligence, resilience, attentional control, desire
to achieve, and self-belief.
Though there is apparent distinction between the facets of MT
examined through each of the sport-specific instruments, there are
similarities between them, such as the need or desire for success. In
addition, there are some commonalities between these instruments and
more general measures of MT, such as control (subscales on the MTQ48 and
SMTQ) and self-belief (similar to confidence on the MTQ48 and SMTQ). The
factor structure outlined in this study demonstrated analogies between
the composition of MT in competitive tennis players and the
sport-specific MT measures that have been developed. For instance, the
confidence/self-efficacy and emotional/cognitive control subscales are
similar to the self-belief and attentional control subscales included in
the CMTI, whereas the positive perspective subscale may be likened to
the thrive through challenge subscale included on the AfMTI. This may
further support the notion of a core set of MT components across all
spheres and subtle differences based on sport-specific requirements or
experiences. As such, it may be important to determine the applicability
of the SMTQ as a measure of MT within specific or general athletic
populations, or whether instruments should be developed for particular
use with a single sporting group. This is one area of the MT literature
that requires attention and clarification. Nonetheless, regardless of
the proposed direction for measuring MT, the current findings suggest
that the SMTQ is an appropriate measure of MT, but that greater caution
should be used when interpreting results based on the subfactors as
compared to global MT scores.
Limitations and Future Research
Though extending current MT instrument validation efforts, there
are several limitations associated with the present study. In
particular, the sample included South African tennis players, making it
challenging to generalise the findings to tennis players from other
countries. In addition, despite including tennis players across a range
of competitive standards, ethnic groups, and age ranges, the sample
included considerably greater quantities of University Team or League
players, White tennis players, and younger tennis players. It is
indeterminable as to whether these demographics are representative of
the target population in general, and these factors should be considered
when using the revised SMTQ outlined in this study.
Notwithstanding the selected limitations associated with the
present study, the findings provide preliminary psychometric evidence
for the applicability and use of the SMTQ amongst competitive tennis
players. The findings further the suggestions that MT applications and
requirements may differ according to sport type (e.g., Bull et al.,
2005), with supplementary efforts encouraged to evaluate the relevance
and appropriate use of the SMTQ and other MT instruments amid athletes
from a variety of sporting domains. It is recommended that researchers
examine the nature and manifestation of MT based on sport type and
develop sport-specific MT instruments.
References
Bell, J., Hardy, L., & Beattie, S. (2013). Enhancing mental
toughness and performance under pressure in elite young cricketers: A
2-year longitudinal intervention. Sport, Exercise, and Performance
Psychology, 2(4), 281-297. doi: 10.1037/a0033129doi: 10.1037/a0033129
Briggs, S., & Cheek, J. (1986). The role of factor analysis in
the evaluation of personality scales .Journal of Personality, 54,
106-148. doi: 10.1111/j. 1467-6494.1986.tb00391.x
Bull, S., Shambrook, C., James, W., & Brooks, J. (2005).
Towards an understanding of mental toughness in elite English
cricketers. Journal of Applied Sport Psychology, 17, 209-227. doi:
10.1080/10413200591010085
Clough, R, Earle, K., & Sewell, D. (2002). Mental toughness:
The concept and its measurement. In I. Cockerill (Ed.), Solutions in
sport psychology (pp. 32-45). London, England: Thomson.
Connaughton, D., Hanton, S., & Jones, G. (2010). The
development and maintenance of mental toughness in the world's best
performers. The Sport Psychologist, 24, 168-193.
Coulter, T., Mallett, C., & Gucciardi, D. (2010). Understanding
mental toughness in Australian soccer: Perceptions of players, parents,
and coaches. Journal of Sports Sciences, 28, 699-716.
doi:10.1080/02640411003734085
Cowden, R. (2012). Differences between college athletes and their
coaches' appraisal of the athletes' degree of mental toughness
in competitive tennis (Unpublished Master's thesis). Middle
Tennessee State University, Murfreesboro.
Crust, L. (2007). Mental toughness in sport: A review.
International Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology, 5, 270-290.
doi:10.1080/1612197X.2007.9671836
Crust, L., & Swann, C. (2011). Comparing two measures of mental
toughness. Personality and Individual Differences, 50, 217-221.
doi:10.1016/j.paid.2010.09.032
Gucciardi, D. (2009). Do developmental differences in mental
toughness exist between specialized and invested Australian footballers?
Personality and Individual Differences, 47, 985-989. doi:
10.1016/j.paid.2009.08.001
Gucciardi, D., & Gordon, S. (2009). Development and preliminary
validation of the Cricket Mental Toughness Inventory (CMTI). Journal of
Sports Sciences, 27, 1293-1310. doi: 10.1080/02640410903242306
Gucciardi, D., Gordon, S., & Dimmock, J. (2009a). Advancing
mental toughness research and theory using personal construct
psychology. International Review of Sport and Exercise Psychology, 2,
54-72. doi: 10.1080/17509840802705938
Gucciardi, D., Gordon, S., & Dimmock, J. (2009b). Development
and preliminary validation of a mental toughness inventory for
Australian football. Psychology of Sport and Exercise, 10, 201-209.
doi:10.1016/j.psychsport.2008.07.011
Horsburgh, V., Schermer, J., Veselka, L., & Vernon, P. (2009).
A behavioural genetic study of mental toughness and personality.
Personality and Individual Differences, 46, 100-105. doi:
10.1016/j.paid.2008.09.009
Hutcheson, G., & Sofroniou, N. (1999). The multivariate social
scientist: Introductory statistics using generalized linear models.
London, England: Sage Publication.
Jones, G., Hanton, S., & Connaughton, D. (2007). A framework of
mental toughness in the world's best performers. The Sport
Psychologist, 21, 243-264.
Loehr, J. (1986). Mental toughness training for sports: Achieving
athletic excellence. Lexington, MA: Stephen Greene Press.
Matsunaga, M. (2010). How to factor-analyze your data right:
Do's, don'ts, and how-to's. International Journal of
Psychological Research, 3, 97-110.
Middleton, S., Marsh, H., Martin, A., Richards, G., Savis, J.,
Perry, C., & Brown, R. (2004). The Psychological Performance
Inventory: Is the mental toughness test tough enough? International
Journal of Sport Psychology, 35, 91-108.
Nicholls, A., Polman, R., Levy, A., & Backhouse, S. (2008).
Mental toughness, optimism, pessimism, and coping among athletes.
Personality and Individual Differences, 44, 11821192. doi: 10.1016/j
paid.2007.11.011
Seligman, M. (1990). Learned optimism. New York, NY: Knopf.
Serpell, R., & Haynes, B. (2004). The cultural practice of
intelligence testing: Problems of international export. In R. Sternberg
& E. Grigorenko (Eds.), Culture and competence: Contexts of life
success (pp. 163-185). Washington, DC: American Psychological
Association.
Sheard, M. (2013). Mental toughness: The mindset behind sporting
achievement (2nd Ed.). New York, NY: Routledge/Taylor & Francis
Group.
Sheard, M., Golby, J., & van Wersch, A. (2009). Progress toward
construct validation of the Sports Mental Toughness Questionnaire.
European Journal of Psychological Assessment, 25, 186-193. doi:
10.1027/1015-5759.25.3.186
Richard G. Cowden
North-West University
Anna Meyer-Weitz
University of Kwazulu-Natal
Address correspondence to: Richard G. Cowden, Email:
richardgregorycowden@gmail.com; Phone: (+27) 018 299 1724, Institute of
Psychology and Wellbeing, Potchefstroom Campus, North-West University,
Private Bag X6001,Potchefstroom 2520, South Africa
Table 1
Participant Demographics
Age Years of Participation
Variable n M SD n M SD
Gender 365 28.80 13.68 365 16.75 12.03
Male 191 31.32 15.28 191 19.33 13.59
Female 174 26.03 11.08 174 13.91 9.29
Ethnicity 365 -- -- 365 -- --
Black 80 23.46 6.29 80 9.84 5.27
Coloured 18 25.28 8.18 18 10.56 3.29
Indian 9 24.44 7.52 9 11.11 2.67
White 258 30.85 15.24 258 19.52 13.01
Participation Type 365 -- -- 365 -- --
County Club 63 41.21 14.61 63 24.37 14.07
Local County 24 30.96 12.80 24 18.21 12.58
Tournaments
National Tournaments 83 32.58 15.70 83 20.04 13.68
International 35 29.69 17.11 35 19.06 16.54
Tournaments
University Team / 160 21.44 2.81 160 11.32 4.05
League
Note, n = Number of participants,
M = Mean, SD = Standard Deviation.
Table 2
PCA Pattern Matrix of SMTQ Item Factor Loadings
Item Factor 1 Factor 2 Factor 3
3 .793 .052 -.403
12 .712 .086 -.024
6 .609 -.050 .233
14 .558 -.121 .319
11 .493 -.022 .145
1 .442 -.112 .220
2 -.179 -.712 -.017
9 -.059 -.691 .100
4 .047 -.657 .200
7 -.073 -.651 .015
10 .181 -.632 -.201
8 .339 -.554 -.156
13 .032 .014 .780
5 .237 -.044 .496
Note. The item numbers correspond with the items
included on the original SMTQ validation study.
Table 3
Pearson Correlations between the Mental
Toughness Scale and Subscales
Variable (1) (2) (3) (4)
(1) Total Mental Toughness -- .12 ** .82 ** .41 **
(2) Confidence/Self-Efficacy -- -- .34 ** .26 **
(3) Emotional/Cognitive Control -- -- -- .19 *
(4) Positive Perspective -- -- --
M 41.22 18.94 16.53 5.67
SD 4.67 2.50 3.11 0.85
Note. * p = .001 (two-tailed); ** p < .001 (two-tailed).
COPYRIGHT 2016 University of South Alabama
No portion of this article can be reproduced without the express written permission from the copyright holder.
Copyright 2016 Gale, Cengage Learning. All rights reserved.