Retrospective remorse: college athletes' reported regrets from a single season.
Robbins, Jamie E. ; Madrigal, Leilani ; Stanley, Christopher T. 等
Retrospective remorse: college athletes' reported regrets from a single season.
After completing a given task, many individuals are faced with a
seemingly simple dilemma: Should I have done it differently? That is,
did certain actions or inactions unfavorably impact the outcome?
One's actions and subsequent emotional response to these questions
may stimulate feelings of regret, which can generally be described as an
individual's personal judgment of his or her errors or missed
actions (Landman, 1993). After making a choice, people tend to judge
themselves and contemplate whether another action or inaction would have
resulted in a more preferable outcome. This afterthought is termed
counterfactual thinking. In sum, if the alternative choice is deemed
more favorable, regret is experienced (Zeelenberg, 1999).
Another form of regret, termed "anticipatory regret,"
which is addressed in advance of an action or lack thereof, involves
deliberations over possible regrets, and these may contribute towards
decision-making and future actions (Gavanski & Wells, 1989). The
notion that individuals may act upon their own speculations related to
regret fits nicely into an instrumental learning theory framework.
Therefore, regrets may be a natural part of the socio-emotional milieu
that accompanies development in a variety of contexts, as regret appears
to impact behaviors, thoughts, and emotions, leading up to and following
an event.
Both experienced and anticipated regrets have been studied to
identify their prevalence, benefits and/or detriments in connection to
well-being and decision-making. While regret may be apparent in many
social contexts, very little research has been done in connection to
sport. This general scarcity of research is surprising given the
constant review processes used by athletes and coaches to detect errors
and improve future performances (Robbins & Stanley, 2013).
Additionally, the inherent probability for errors and losses in sports
likely yields numerous opportunities for regrets on a daily and seasonal
basis, as well as during transitional phases of a career. Further
examination of regret in sport appears warranted.
Action vs. Inaction
Regret may be more formally defined as a negative affective state
connected to a counterfactual thought (Connolly & Zeelenberg, 2002).
Conflicting evidence exists regarding what situations lead to the most
regret. Kruger, Wirtz, and Miller (2005) found that people regret
actions more than inactions. Whereas other researchers have found that
people regret inactions more than actions (Zeelenberg, Van Dijk, Van den
Bos, & Pieters, 2002). Roese and Summerville (2005) explained that
individuals regret areas of life they could change and have fewer
regrets in domains of life that afford little change. On the other hand,
Beike, Markman, and Karadogan (2009) found people regret lost
opportunities, explained as situations they once could have changed, but
now cannot change. This lack of control impedes closure, thus
intensifying feelings of regret. This feeling can then motivate behavior
change if reacted to quickly. In other words, if someone still has the
chance to do or undo an act, the instantaneous feeling of regret may be
beneficial (Beike et al., 2009). Basically the regret serves as a means
of teaching a person to not make the same mistake twice (Beike et al.,
2009). Patrick, Lancellotti, and Hagtvedt (2009) found this in relation
to consumers who missed an opportunity to purchase an item, experienced
regret, which then intensified their intent to change the behavior in
the future. Given our scant understanding of regrets in an athletic
population, it seems relevant to identify whether actions or inactions
are more regretful in this population, as this insight could be used by
Sport Psychology Consultants (SPCs) aiming to improve athletes'
experiences by using regrets that lead to behavior change and by
minimizing regrets that impact well-being.
Positive vs. Negative Impacts
Importantly, linkages may be drawn between regret and life
satisfaction scores in older adults (Torges, Stewart, &
Miner-Rubino, 2005). Individuals with fewer regrets scored higher on
indices of physical health when compared to their counterparts reporting
more regrets. In another study, researchers examined regrets in a sample
of Dutch females (16-81 years old) and found participants who reported
regrets also maintained lower levels of psychological and physical
well-being (Dijkstra & Bareld, 2008). Along these same lines, Roese
et al., (2009) found associations between regret and anhedonic
depression, as well as regret and anxious arousal in a sample of both
males and females. Additionally, regret and repetitive thought, termed
repetitive regret, was found to be highly predictive of general distress
in that group (Roese et al., 2009). Thus, there are possible negative
consequences to feeling regret. However a complete lack of regret may
not be possible or advantageous.
The existence of regrets may be beneficial if used to modify future
behaviors. From a neuroscience perspective, it has been argued that
learning is enhanced by intense emotional components; subsequently,
regrets may benefit the learning process (Miller, 2009). Beike et al.
(2009) suggested that hope should be increased and regrets decreased if
people perceive possibilities for change in their future. In other
words, regret may be a functional and useful aspect of a healthy
learning and developmental process, and may be utilized to emphasize
personal growth and control. Individuals' perceptions regarding
their ability to control and change an action/behavior is more likely to
prompt corrective behavior (Roese & Summerville, 2005). Therefore,
how one perceives and what one does with the counterfactual thought is
most important. If individuals are able to consider "what may
be" instead of "what might have been" they are likely to
experience fewer negative effects from their regrets (Boninger, Gleicher
& Strathman, 1994). From a SPC's perspective, it may be useful
to address these regret-related emotions and behaviors earlier in life
(or a career) when individuals have ample opportunity to take
responsibility and identify skills and resources they possess to change
future behaviors (Bauer, Wrosch & Jobin, 2008; Wrosch &
Heckhausen, 2002).
Anticipated versus Experienced Regret
Anticipated regrets have been found to be strongly influential on
behavior change (Gilbert, Morewedge, Risen, & Wilson, 2004). In
fact, Gilbert et al. (2004) found that people think they will experience
regrets more than they actually do, thus the fear of experiencing regret
can positively or negatively impact behavior as one may change a
behavior to avoid regret or not engage in a behavior for the same
reason. According to Gilbert et al. (2004) this fear of regret may not
be necessary as their studies found people experienced far less regret
than they assumed in advance. With regard to certain behaviors it may be
good to recognize that the fear of regret is stronger than the regret;
however, that fear may be advantageous when it comes to other unhealthy
behaviors. More specifically, Conner, Sandberg, McMillan, and Higgins
(2006) found that anticipated regret significantly predicted intentions
to smoke, more than attitudes, subjective norms and perceived behavioral
control. Higher levels of regret also were associated with lower
intentions to smoke.
Within a sport context, Robbins and Stanley (2012) studied regret
in two Division I (DI) female collegiate teams. They found that athletes
who read and discussed regrets at the start of the season provided fewer
weekly regrets compared to their counterparts who did not have access to
the regrets of former athletes. This showed the possible benefits of
anticipated regrets for changing behaviors during a season. However,
given the homogeneous nature of the participants it was suggested that
regrets be addressed in a larger, more heterogeneous population,
including males and females from a variety of sport teams (Robbins &
Stanley, 2013).
Given the inevitable presence of regrets in sport and the
identified consequences of such feelings, it is critical to further our
understanding about the impact and type of regret among a variety of
athletes. To date regrets have only been studied in female team sport
participants. Robbins and Stanley (2013) found that out of the 46 former
DI field hockey players who participated in the study, 40 experienced
regrets, with only three reporting a complete lack of regret. Content
analysis yielded six major regret categories, including: (a) team, (b)
priorities, (c) communication, (d) role, (e) confidence, and (f)
transition. Robbins and Stanley (2012) looked at regrets throughout a
competitive season in female DI field hockey and soccer players, again
finding the existence of regret in this population. More information is
needed to identify if there are more, fewer or different regrets among a
variety of team and individual sport athletes before attempting further
interventions.
The prevalence and impacts of regrets on behaviors and emotions is
well documented. However, the scope and nature of regret in the athletic
population is not well understood. Therefore, the purpose of the current
study was to employ a larger and more diverse sample to identify the
existence or prevalence of additional categories of regret.
Subsequently, researchers were also able to examine whether regrets
differed based on sex and/or sport, in addition to actions or inactions.
Method
Participants
The final sample consisted of 172 athletes (103 males; 68 females;
1 unspecified sex) who answered questions related to regrets based on
their season. Athletes were recruited from two universities in the
Southeast United States. Coaches were initially contacted by the primary
investigator regarding the nature of the study and requesting their team
participation. Athletes were from National Collegiate Athletic
Association (NCAA) DI or DII teams and they played on either an
individual or team-sport program. More specifically, participating teams
included: baseball, basketball, field hockey, soccer, volleyball, track
& field/cross country, and golf.
Procedures
Subsequent to receiving university institutional review board (IRB)
approval, coaches of each respective team were contacted to request team
participation. Interested coaches arranged for teams to meet the
investigators in a classroom, locker room, or another closed spaced on
their campus. Coaches were not present at the meeting where athletes
were informed about the study. Following explanation of the study
protocol and purpose, possible participants were asked to sign an
informed consent form. Those who returned the signed form were then
provided the survey and those who did not sign the consent form were
asked to stay seated until their peers were finished to avoid drawing
attention to themselves. Participants completed the survey that assessed
demographic information (i.e. age, sport, sex, and class standing). No
names were collected to ensure confidentiality. To assess regrets, a
survey was adapted from previous studies, which utilized open-ended
questions (i.e. what, if any, regrets do you have from your collegiate
athletic career; what, if any, regrets do you have as a result of this
season) and written responses (Robbins & Stanley, 2012; 2013).
Participants were asked if they had any regrets in connection to their
most recent season. These could be regrets of omission (i.e., things not
done, said, or thought) and/or regrets of action (i.e., things that were
done, said, or thought). It is also important to note that when
examining regrets over a season, behaviors outside of sport may be
influential, so participants were instructed to include non-sport
experiences that impacted their season. If participants answered
"yes" to having regrets, they were asked to elaborate in
writing regarding the regret and also what they would do or change if
given the option to do it again. This second part was included to ensure
the researchers were collecting actual regrets, which are defined as a
negative emotion associated with feelings that an action could have been
carried out more effectively if alternative choices were made
(Zeelenberg, 1999).
Data Analysis
A mixed-method approach was utilized to identify frequencies,
descriptions and comparisons between and among different groups. All
open-ended responses were initially typed verbatim by the primary
investigator into a single document per team. Within each team packet
were individual athlete responses. Each individual response was coded
with the athlete's age, academic standing, sport and sex. The raw
data from all teams yielded 190 pages of data, which were then
independently reviewed and coded by two of the investigators.
The researchers read and provided codes to best describe the raw
data. Upon completion of the independent review process, the researchers
then met to determine consistency and discrepancies between their
individual codes. Interpretation began with the finest details of each
raw data theme and moved to more generalizable themes (Thompson, 1997).
This general process was repeated several times by two of the primary
researchers independently and together until distinctive emergent themes
were identified from the data. These themes were then grouped into
relevant categories on the basis of general characteristics of theme
essence (Dutton & Dukerich, 1991).
This process resulted in a codebook containing 31 first order
themes. Each code was provided a specific definition and each raw data
theme was labeled accordingly. After consensus of all the first order
themes, the researchers identified 7 second order themes which subsumed
the original themes. These second order themes were: (a) Relationships,
(b) Sport behaviors, (c) Health, (d) Cognition, (e) Performance, (f)
Life skills, and (g) Control. Finally, all themes were condensed under
the most general themes termed Controllable and Uncontrollable, as well
as, Inaction and Action. These categories were identified based on
existing literature and applied experience. As such regrets were termed
Controllable if they related to the person's actions, behavior or
thoughts. Uncontrollable regrets related to others' behaviors, team
outcomes, or winning and losing. Inaction regrets were those that
described actions or behaviors not taken, whereas Action regrets
included all regrets related to what participants actually did.
Following complete consensus by the two original investigators, a third
researcher independently coded the raw data themes. Inter-rater
reliability was found to be 89.17%. Although this percentage seems to
indicate a high degree of agreement, it does not account for agreements
which may have occurred by chance alone. Thus, an appropriate indicator
of inter-rater reliability (i.e. kappa statistic) was calculated
([kappa] = 0.77, p < .05) which confirmed a substantial level of
agreement regarding the regret themes (Landis & Koch, 1977).
For exploratory purposes, a series of chi-square tests of
independence were employed to examine if the proportion of males and
females reporting a type of regret was consistent across individual
(e.g., cross country) and team sports (e.g., baseball, volleyball). As a
result of low numbers for several regret types (e.g. health and body,
balance, steroid use, etc.) and thus questionable reliability of the
chi-square test, the data were collapsed and analyzed according to
second order themes. Additionally, further analyses were run on the most
general themes of action versus inaction and controllable versus
uncontrollable regrets.
Results
The final sample consisted of 172 participants (see table 1),
reporting a total of 1236 regrets ([M.sub.regrets] = 7.20). The vast
majority of participants reported at least one regret related to their
previous competitive season, with only four participants reporting no
regrets. The initial series of chi-square tests of independence revealed
no significance (p > .05) between males and females or across
individual (e.g., cross country) and team sports (e.g., baseball,
volleyball). However, two significant results did emerge: Males on team
sport and females on individual sports were more likely to report
"eating habit" related regret than their female-team and
male-individual counterparts, [X.sup.2] (1, 98)= 7.78,p< .05. The
same pattern emerged for "work ethic," with males on team and
females on individual sports more likely to report such regrets,
[X.sup.2] (1, 127)= 26.04, p < .05. Overall, while in select cases
athletes (i.e., team male and individual female) reported more regrets
than anticipated, generally it does not appear regrets vary according to
sex or sport type.
After collapsing the data into second order themes, four
significant findings emerged for health, [X.sup.2] (1, 227) = 9.29, p
< .05, performance, [X.sup.2] (1, 165) = 14.01, p < .05, life
skills, [X.sup.2] (1, 180) = 7.03,p < .05, and sport behaviors,
[X.sup.2] (1, 90) = 5.15, p < .05, respectively. It was the same
pattern as before, with male-team athletes and female-individual
athletes more likely to report regret in those areas. The original raw
data were also collapsed into controllable/uncontrollable and
action/inaction categories, yielding four primary analyses. There was a
significant chi-square analysis (p < .05) for all of the coded areas.
Again, male team athletes and female individual athletes were more
likely to report all four types of regrets.
The original data were condensed in some meaningful ways. First,
almost all (more than 90%) of the regret categories would be deemed
controllable, with only two categories being primarily uncontrollable.
Additionally, although there were no significant differences with regard
to regrets of actions versus inactions, in some cases, there were mainly
inaction regrets. This was evident for: attention to health and body,
balance, engage professors, focus, leadership, openness, relationship
with coach, and work ethic. In other cases, there were mainly action
regrets. This was apparent for: control, emotional control, personal
choice, and drinking. Overall, the four most commonly cited themes
included: (a) eating, which was mentioned by 55% of athlete; (b) sleep
habits, discussed by 51% of athletes; (c) confidence, a regret of 52% of
athletes; and (d) work ethic, mentioned by 48% of participants.
More specific qualitative analysis yielded the following results
(see figures 1 and 2). Within the controllable category, six
second-order themes emerged: (a) Relationships, (b) Sport behaviors, (c)
Health, (d) Cognition, (e) Performance, and (f) Life skills. Each of the
aforementioned categories contained several first-order themes. The
higher order theme, Uncontrollable, was composed of only one
second-order termed Outcome and two first-order themes: Personal outcome
and Team outcome.
Relationships
The relationship domain was comprised of 5 first order themes all
pertaining to athletes' social interactions with others, including:
(a) openness, (b) relationship with coach, (c) relationship with
teammates, (d) social life, and (e) engaging with professors. The
first-order theme, openness, explains regrets related to one's
reluctance to: express opinions, say what needed to be said, ask for
help, or communicate their emotions effectively. Athletes also regretted
not being more sociable, not meeting more people and being "too
closed off." Although most athletes regretted a lack of openness,
there were two athletes who expressed regrets over trusting others and
opening up too much, while two other athletes regretted their closed
mindedness.
Three groups of people specifically mentioned in athletes'
regrets were teammates, coaches and professors. Some athletes regretted
not talking about issues with their coaches as a means of improving
their own or their team's performance; whereas others wished they
had expressed their feelings to coaches with hopes of changing the
coach. For example, one athlete said, I regret "not saying
something earlier to our coaches about the lack of passion."
Whereas, another athlete stated, I regret "letting my coach say the
things he did to me ... extremely rude, hurtful, unprofessional."
Athletes also regretted not being more involved with their
teammates. More specifically they expressed regret over not hanging out,
getting to know, or incorporating themselves with their team. Although
most regrets concerned not being close enough to teammates and coaches,
there were athletes who regretted being friends with teammates and
trying to have a relationship with their coach. Athletes also mentioned
regret over not talking to or meeting with their professors enough. The
final sub-theme within the relationships category was social life. Some
athletes felt they missed out on life outside sport, whereas, others
regretted partying too much, hanging out too late and enjoying it too
much.
Sport Behaviors
The theme sport behavior consists of regrets termed: (a)
leadership, (b) team player, and (c) role. Compared to the
aforementioned relationship theme, sport behaviors included interactions
only in sport contexts. For instance, leadership was the most common
regret in this category. Interestingly, athletes ranging from freshmen
to fifth year seniors regretted not being effective leaders or not
accepting a stronger leadership role. They regretted, "not stepping
up," "not taking the reins," "not being a vocal
leader," "not setting a better example," and "not
pushing people hard enough." With regard to the team player theme,
most athletes regretted not being a team player, primarily described as
putting themselves before the team. For example, "I regret getting
upset when we were winning because of personal performance." And, I
regret "my attitude after games I did not get playing time I
wanted." Two athletes regretted not being effective team players
because they did not or could not contribute to the team's outcome
resulting from performance issues or injury.
Additionally, athletes expressed regrets over the size of their
role, not understanding their role, not committing to their role, or not
fulfilling their role. As well, one athlete claimed, "I regret that
I was known more [as] the loving fun girl on the team instead of one you
admire because of the work I put in." Taken together these regrets
reveal athletes' frustrations with their own approach to team
related issues. Generally athletes regretted not doing what they should
have done or being the athlete they could have been.
Health
The health theme was composed of the following first-order themes:
(a) drinking, (b) eating, (c) attention to health and body, (d) sleep
habits, and (e) steroids. The two most common regrets surrounded
athletes' ineffective eating and sleeping habits. Athletes
regretted not eating healthy or eating unhealthy and not getting enough
sleep. Drinking behaviors were mentioned by several athletes and others
had more all-encompassing regrets over not taking care of their bodies
and health. These athletes mentioned regrets concerning their quick
return to play following an injury. Although the majority of regrets in
this category had to do with not being healthy enough or taking care of
oneself, two athletes did regret their decision not to take steroids. As
a whole these regrets raise concerns over the health related behaviors
of athletes.
Cognitive
Subsumed under the cognitive theme are regrets labeled: (a)
approach, (b) attitude,
(c) confidence, (d) focus, (e) control, and (f) emotional control.
The most common regret concerned not believing in oneself. This was
another regret mentioned by a range of athletes from freshmen through
fifth year seniors. The athletes regretted a lack of, or loss of,
confidence throughout the season. However, while most athletes explained
not being confident, two athletes (1 male/1 female) regretted being too
cocky.
Athletes also regretted not controlling their emotions, focus, or
attention; in addition to allowing outside factors and people to impact
their focus, attitudes and behaviors. They regretted allowing mental
distractions to impact their performance and attitude, and their
inability to let go of negative thoughts. Specifically, they explained
regretting "giving up on certain plays in practice over
frustration," "stressing to balance athletics, academics, and
college life," and "letting my emotions turn me into someone
I'm not." Others regretted, "getting mad at myself when I
play bad," "not being able to move past negative things,"
and "getting caught up in small drama, being easily annoyed or
judgmental." Within these themes, athletes explained numerous
regrets relating to their inability to let go of a thought or change a
negative emotion.
In addition to the intense emotions already discussed, athletes
also regretted not taking their sport or school seriously enough, not
being positive, not showing more energy and enthusiasm, being too laid
back, disrespectful, or just not caring in both the classroom and their
athletic venue. They mentioned regrets over "giving up,"
"not loving what they were doing," or "doing just enough
to get by." They regretted "not buying into the program,"
"not being in tune with my sport," and "not being
competitive enough." Interestingly, athletes from every team
regretted having a negative attitude.
Performance
Performance regrets included those related to: (a) work ethic, (b)
inconsistent performance, and (c) ineffective performance and training.
The most common regret overall was not working hard enough in either
school or sport. They regretted not studying enough, going to class or
putting sufficient effort into their schoolwork. As well, athletes
regretted not doing extra, putting in more time or really give 100% to
athletics. Fundamentally, most athletes felt they could have done more
and should have done more.
Athletes cited regrets over: "not playing or performing
well," "stupid decisions on the field," and
"completing some workouts, which led to injury." Others
regretted performing inconsistently throughout the year. They regretted
a lack of organization with regard to schoolwork and a lack of
commitment related to physical training. Others explained, "not
preparing mentally for every game," "being lazy in
transition," and "not going to do more 1-1 with coaches
outside of practice." Although most athletes regretted their
inactions, there was one participant who regretted studying too much.
This again provides evidence that both actions and inactions incite
feelings of regret.
Life Skills
The final controllable theme of life skills contained the following
seven first-order themes: (a) priorities, (b) procrastination, (c)
personal choices, (d) balance, (e) handling transition, (f) time
management, and (g) school and course selection. The majority of regrets
under life skills pertained to time management, procrastination,
priorities and personal choices. These regrets concern perceived
"wasted time" in a variety of contexts. For example, one
athlete said, "I regret pulling all-nighters because of
procrastination." Many athletes regretted what they actually
prioritized during the year, although they differed with regard to what
was later deemed most important, with the majority identifying school
and sport. More specifically they said, "I regret going out too
much and not focusing on school work," and "I regret putting
school to the bottom of the list." Whereas other said, "I
regret "allowing distractions to take precedence over ball"
and "running became my last [priority]. Academics was a clear
first."
With regard to life outside of sport, there were various regrets.
Whereas some athletes regretted a lack of balance in their lives (i.e.
engaging in activities outside of sport), others regretted their
decisions when they were not playing. More specifically one athlete
said, "1 regret not taking a step back and trying to do other
things to make me happy." Another athlete claimed, "I regret
that I wasted opportunities to really enjoy the cultural/social college
life." While others regretted their "stupid decisions,"
which included: "having unprotected sex," "doing
something with this one guy," "getting back together with an
ex-boyfriend," "sleeping with my head in the freezer,"
and "smoking marijuana, fights, punching a steel door, and breaking
my hand." Additional life skill regrets were much more specific to
their personal life choices, with athletes regretting University
selection decisions. Although only a few athletes mentioned transfer
regrets, there was one who regretted the timing of her transfer to the
school, whereas others regretted not transferring out when they had the
chance.
Taken together, the majority of aforementioned regrets leave room
for change and growth. Athletes can learn to control their attitudes and
modify their actions and inactions in future seasons. The regrets, if
addressed early and often, could serve as reminders of what to do and
what not to do in the future, thus leaving athletes with fewer
experienced regrets. Although they cannot undo what was done in the
past; all the behaviors, thoughts and inactions were within their
control at the time they made their decision.
Outcome
Regrets related to outcome consisted of both personal outcomes and
team outcomes (see figure 2). These regrets specifically refer to
athletes' dissatisfaction with their win/ loss record, final
grades, ineligibility, injury or any other unchangeable end result.
Personal outcomes included not getting good grades, getting in trouble,
making errors, getting injured, gaining weight, or not scoring more.
Team outcomes related to the team's ultimate performance, teammates
not getting along, or other team related behaviors or attitudes. As a
whole these regrets are all uncontrollable because they refer
specifically to an end result or other person. Whereas the previous
regrets were caused by behaviors or attitudes that could ultimately be
changed, these are situations involving an external variable. Still,
athletes may try to change the controllable behaviors and thoughts to
put themselves in the best position to gain a more desirable outcome and
fewer regrets overall.
Discussion
As evidenced by the sheer number of regrets reported in this study,
it appears regrets may be rather pervasive in sport. In addition to the
quantity of regrets expressed by athletes, there also was a wide variety
of factors that influenced regret feelings, thus expanding domains of
regret derived from previous studies (Robbins & Stanley, 2012;
2013). Athletes in the current study revealed both controllable and
uncontrollable regrets. The majority appeared controllable in nature,
which is of critical importance when addressing the possibility for
change. That is, regrets that appear controllable may be employed in
behavior modification efforts.
The current study aimed to further our understanding of regrets and
identify if differences exist with regard to expressed regrets of a
diverse group of athletes. No significant differences were found with
regard to sex or team and individual sport, instead, commonalities were
found in experienced regrets. The most common regrets included: (a)
eating, (b) sleep habits, (c) confidence, and (d) work ethic, with
approximately 50% of participants mentioning these categories. As such,
the researchers concluded these to be important regrets requiring
attention with a collegiate athlete population and topics SPCs could
discuss with athletes to incite behavior modifications aimed at
minimizing future regrets.
The number of athletes who regretted not eating healthy or eating
unhealthy confirms the relevance of this topic, yet addressing and
assessing nutritional needs of athletes can be complicated. In addition
to a general lack of understanding concerning what is needed for each
individual, comes the lack of willingness on the part of the athlete to
take care of his or her diet. Shriver, Betts, and Wollenberg (2013)
found in female collegiate athletes that energy and carbohydrate intakes
were below the minimum recommendations and 75% of participants consumed
less than the required carbohydrates to support their training.
Additionally, only a few of the participants monitored their hydration,
which could significantly impact athletic performance.
The current study found that both male and female athletes are
aware of their poor dietary behaviors and they regret not taking more
care to eat properly. This is clearly a behavior that is in
athletes' control, thus it is necessary to identify barriers that
impede positive eating habits and initiate programs or plans to improve
eating choices. SPCs could use the findings from this study to stimulate
discussion regarding eating habits among their athletes. Given the
sensitive nature of the topic, it is imperative that one find a
delicate, yet effective way of introducing the conversation. Stating
that 55% of athletes in a previous study regretted their eating habits
may be a powerful and convincing foundation for individual and or team
behavior changes relating to eating habits.
The results regarding sleep related regrets are also relevant, as
lack of sleep has been found to be responsible for major disasters and
loss of productivity in the general population (Bonnet, 2005). In fact,
insufficient sleep can lead to fatigue, irritability, concentration
difficulties, and health related problems (Bonnet, 2005; Wells &
Vaughn, 2012). It has been said that athletes, likely more than the
average person, require sleep to perform optimally (Davenne, 2009).
There are intense daily demands on student-athletes' minds and
bodies to perform in the classroom and on the field. This increases the
importance of effective focus and memory if athletes hope to achieve
success. However, both focus and memory are negatively influenced by a
lack of sleep (Diekelmann, Wilhelm, & Born, 2009; Ficca &
Salzarulo, 2004). As such, recent studies have identified a lack of
sleep as a contributing factor towards poor academic performance in
undergraduate students (Gomes, Tavares, & Azevedo, 2011).
Out of the 176 participants in the current study, 95 regretted not
getting enough sleep or staying up too late. Given the possible
detriments this behavior could have on both athletic and academic
performance, in addition to, overall mood and focus, it seems obvious
that this behavior requires greater attention. Although many athletes
expressed they would change this behavior by getting to sleep earlier,
doing work in advance, and wasting less time, it is critical that these
athletes create realistic plans for enacting this behavior change
provided their busy schedules, numerous obligations, and possible
distractions. Sleep is a complicated phenomenon not easily changed and
not the same for everyone, as numerous factors impair sleep including,
but not limited to: temperature, inconsistent schedules, and stress
(Venter, 2012). As such, SPCs could use these findings to encourage
discussion and planning among their athletes to help them avoid this
regret. Rather than lecturing athletes on the need for sleep, they can
explain the regrets of the athletes in this study and then help their
athletes identify reasons for their current sleep schedule and realistic
modifications to their daily schedules to ensure more effective sleep
habits throughout their season.
Another regret identified by more than half of the participants was
a lack of confidence. Confidence is a common term in athletics, viewed
by many as a critical component of optimal performance. According to
Vealey, Hayashi, Gamer-Homan, and Giacobbi (1998) athletes use one or
more of the following nine sources for confidence: (a) mastery, (b)
demonstration of ability, (c) physical/mental preparation, (d) physical
self-presentation, (e) social support, (f) coaches' leadership, (g)
vicarious experience, (h) environmental comfort, and (i) situational
favorableness. Although all of these sources have been utilized by
athletes, ideal sources of confidence are those that are within
athletes' control. Sport psychology practitioners may employ a
"regret aversion" approach in building self-confidence through
mental skills training.
If athletes from the current study are representative of collegiate
athletes nationally, then SPCs can be a great asset as many of their
techniques are aimed at helping athletes increase confidence. They can
help athletes shift their focus from what they cannot or did not do, to
what they can do and will do, by helping them identify and be proud of
their daily efforts to achieve their outcome rather than merely focusing
on the outcome. Throughout the season SPCs can help athletes minimize
regrets by shifting their attitude and focus, which may ultimately
impact their work-ethic as well.
Nearly half of the athletes in the current study regretted not
working hard enough. Work ethic has been described as one of the
"intangibles" coaches look for when recruiting athletes
(Flett, Gould, Paule, & Schneider, 2010). Additionally, work ethic
has been identified as a source of expectancy information coaches use to
evaluate athletes (Solomon & Rhea, 2008). As such, athletes likely
hear about their work ethic throughout the season, possibly stimulating
their ultimate regrets once season ends. Participants in the current
study explained regrets resulting from a lack of effort in both school
and sport. Given the likelihood that they were reprimanded by coaches
and earning lower than expected grades throughout the season if they
truly were not working hard, it begs the question; why did they not
experience regrets earlier? Additionally, are these regrets
substantiated or unrealistic? Are athletes looking back and judging
themselves on a true lack of effort or are they just unsatisfied with
the outcome and hoping they can do more as a means for self-enhancement.
Feeney, Gardiner, Johnston, Jones, and McEvoy (2005) explain that
revealing regrets regarding inactions can make people feel better as
they can still look back and say, if I had done more, the results would
have been different. Regardless of why athletes revealed their regrets,
the ultimate challenge is still getting them to modify their actions,
thoughts and behaviors to ensure fewer regrets in their future. Again, a
SPC can work with athletes on identifying and measuring their work-ethic
throughout the season to ensure regret is not experienced only after the
possibility for change is lost.
Taken together, the current study identified numerous regrets
athletes expressed at the culmination of their season. It is naive to
assume we could avoid all regrets, but quite realistic to believe that
some regrets could be bypassed. People will always make some mistakes
and wonder whether an alternate decision would have led to a better
outcome; however, we may be able to encourage people to think about
their actions and inactions more fully in the present to avoid
regretting as many actions and inactions in the future. Many decisions
that lead to later regrets are made without appropriate focus, planning
or attention, which can all be addressed with the aims of improving
behaviors including, but not limited to diet, sleep, work ethic, and
confidence.
Limitations and Future Research
While the current study offered a wealth of information related to
regret in sport, as a whole, research on athletes and regret is still in
its infancy. More work is needed with larger and more diverse
populations to understand regrets athletes possess over a season, if
they change throughout a season, and if they differ based on specific
athlete characteristics. Additionally, further examination of behaviors
or actions currently being used by athletes to minimize or combat their
own regret feelings would help to reveal existing effective or
ineffective methods.
The current study was able to identify areas of importance based on
a fairly large number of athletes; yet the sample was skewed toward male
responses, with 103 male participants compared to only 68 female
participants. Additionally, baseball and track and cross-country were
more represented than other sports, and still no data has been collected
on regrets in sports such as football, tennis, swimming or ice hockey.
Themes identified through this study did corroborate findings of
previous studies (Robbins & Stanley, 2012, 2013); still, future
studies should address this topic among different cultures, age groups,
levels of sport, in addition to a greater variety of sport types. As
well, more in-depth studies on smaller population can be collected to
gain a more complete understanding of these regret feelings, and
athletes' desires and willingness to change behaviors with the goal
of minimizing regrets in their future. It also would be important to
identify if athletes recognize their omissions or actions during the
season itself or only after reflection upon season's end.
Although there were limitations, the current findings still provide
a useful foundation for future studies and for applied interventions
with current athletes. Coaches and SPCs could introduce the categories
of regret mentioned by these athletes at the start of the season and
throughout the season to instigate conversation regarding possible
regrets and encourage their athletes to contemplate their decisions more
fully in an attempt at minimizing their own future regrets. The goal for
future researchers and practitioners should be aimed at helping athletes
utilize anticipated regrets and minimize experienced regrets to enhance
their well-being, enjoyment and ultimate athletic performance.
References
Bauer, I., Wrosch, C., & Jobin, J. (2008). I'm better off
than most other people: The role of social comparisons for coping with
regret in young adulthood and old age. Psychology and Aging, 23,
800-811. doi: 10.1037/a0014180
Beike, D., Markman, K., & Karadogan, F. (2009). What we regret
most are lost opportunities: A theory of regret intensity. Personality
and Social Psychology Bulletin, 35, 385-397. doi: 10.1177/014
Boninger, D., Gleicher, F., & Strathman, A. (1994).
Counterfactual thinking: From what might have been to what may be.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 67, 297-307.
Bonnet, M. (2005). Sleep deprivation. In Kryger, M.H., Roth, &
W. Dement (Eds.), Principles and Practice of Sleep Medicine.
Philadelphia, PA: Elsevier Saunders.
Conner, M., Sandberg, T., McMillan, B., & Higgins, A. (2006).
Role of anticipated regret, intentions and intention stability in
adolescent smoking initiation. British Journal of Health Psychology, 11,
85-101. doi:10.1348/135910705X40997
Connolly, T. & Zeelenberg, M. (2002). Regret in decision
making. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 77, 212-216.
Davenne, D. (2009). Sleep of athletes- problems and possible
solutions. Biological Rhythm Research, 40, 45-52. doi:
10.1080/09291010802067023
Diekelmann, S., Wilhelm, I., Born, J. (2009). The whats and whens
of sleep-dependent memory consolidation. Sleep Medicine Review, 13,
309-321.
Dijkstra, P. & Barelds, D. (2008). What Dutch women of
different ages regret most. Journal of Research in Personality, 42,
1663-1667. doi:10.1016/j.jrp.2008.07.007
Dutton, J. E., & Dukerich, J. M. (1991). Keeping an eye on the
mirror: Image and identity in organizational adaptation. Academy of
Management Journal, 34, 517-554. doi: 10.2307/256405
Feeney, A., Gardiner, D., Johnston, K., Jones, E., & McEvoy, R.
(2005). Is regret for inaction relatively self-enhancing? Applied
Cognitive Psychology, 19, 761-777. doi: 10.1002/acp.1113
Ficca, G., & Salzarulo, P. (2004). What in sleep is for memory.
Sleep Medicine, 5, 225-230.
Flett, R., Gould, D., Paule, A., & Schneider, R. (2010). How
and why university coaches define, identify and recruit
'intangibles.' International Journal of Coaching Science, 4,
15-35.
Gavanski, I. & Wells, G. L. (1989). Counterfactual processing
of normal and exceptional events. Journal of Experimental Social
Psychology, 25, 314-325.
Gilbert, D., Morewedge, C., Risen, J., & Wilson, T. (2004).
Looking forward to looking backward: The misprediction of regret.
Psychological Science, 15, 346-350. doi: 10.1111/ j.0956-7976.2004.00681
.x
Gomes, A., Tavares, J., & Azevedo, M. (2011). Sleep and
academic performance in undergraduates: A multi-measure, multi-predictor
approach. Chronobiology International, 28, 786-801. doi:
10.3109/07420528.2011.606518
Kruger, J., Wirtz, D., & Miller, D. T. (2005). Counterfactual
thinking and the first instinct fallacy. Attitudes and Social Cognition,
88, 725-735. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.88.5.725
Landis, J.R., & Koch, G.G. (1977). The measurement of observer
agreement for categorical data .Biometrics, 33, 159-174.
Landman, J. (1993). Regret: The persistence of the possible.
Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.
Miller, M. (2009). Go ahead, have regrets. Han'ard Business
Review, 87, 28-28.
Patrick, V., Lancellotti, M., & Hagtvedt, H. (2009). Getting a
second chance: The role of imagery in the influence of inaction regret
on behavioral intent. Journal of the Academy of Marketing Science, 37,
181-190. doi: 10.1007/s 11747-008-0119-8
Robbins, J. & Stanley, C. (2013). Exploring regret in former
athletes: Emotional ramifications of sport-specific decisions. Applied
Research in Coaching and Athletics Annual, 28, 1-33.
Robbins, J. & Stanley, C. (2012). Minimizing regrets in college
athletes: An intervention approach. International Journal of Sport and
Exercise Psychology, 10, 186-197. doi:10.1 080/1612197x.2012.672010
Roese, N., Epstude, K., Fessel, F., Morrison, M., Smallman, R.,
Summerville, A. Galinsky, A., & Segerskom, S. (2009). Repetitive
regret, depression, and anxiety: Findings from a nationally
representative survey. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 28,
671-688.
Roese, N. & Summerville, A. (2005). What we regret most ... and
why. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 31, 1273-1285. doi:
10.1177/0146167205274693
Shriver, L., Betts, N., & Wollenberg, G. (2013). Dietary
intakes and eating habits of college athletes: Are female college
athletes following the current sports nutrition standards? Journal of
American College Health, 61, 10-16. doi:10.1080/07448481.2012.747526
Solomon, G. & Rhea, D. (2008). Sources of expectancy
information among college coaches: A qualitative test of expectancy
theory. International Journal of Sports Science and Coaching, 3,
251-268.
Thompson, C. J. (1997). Interpreting consumers: A hermeneutical
framework for deriving marketing insights from the texts of
consumers' consumption stories. Journal of Marketing Research, 34,
438-455. doi: 10.2307/3151963
Torges, C.M., Stewart, A.J., & Miner-Rubino, K. (2005).
Personality after the prime of life: Men and women coming to terms with
regrets. Journal of Research in Personality, 39, 148-165.
doi:10.1016/j.jrp.2004.09.005
Vealey, R., Hayashi, S., Gamer-Homan, M., & Giacobbi, P.
(1998). Sources of sport-confidence: Conceptualization and instrument
development. Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology, 20, 54-80.
Venter, R. (2012). Role of sleep in performance and recovery of
athletes; A review article. South African Journal for Research in Sport,
Physical Education and Recreation, 34, 167-184.
Wells, M. & Vaughn, B. (2012). Poor sleep challenging the
health of a nation. Neurodiagnostics Journal, 52, 233-249.
Zeelenberg, M. (1999). Anticipated regret, expected feedback and
behavioral decision making. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 12,
93-106.
Zeelenberg, M., Van Dijk, E, Van den Bos, K., & Pieters, R.
(2002). The inaction effect in the psychology of regret. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 82, 314-327. doi:
10.1037//0022-3514.82.3.314
Jamie E. Robbins
West Chester University of Pennsylvania
Leilani Madrigal
University of Nebraska
Christopher T. Stanley
Florida Gulf Coast University
Address correspondence to: Jamie Robbins, Department of
Kinesiology, West Chester University of Pennsylvania, West Chester, PA
19383. E-mail: jrobbins@wcupa.edu
Table 1
Demographic Information for Participants
Sport Division Sex Class Standing
RSF Fr So Ju S 5th
Baseball I M 5 6 2 7 6 2
Baseball II M 1 8 7 8 5 2
Basketball I M 0 9 2 1 0 0
Basketball II M 0 2 1 3 0 0
Track/CC I M 2 6 3 9 5 0
Track/CC I F 2 8 4 7 0 1
F. Hockey I F 0 6 2 5 0 0
Volleyball II F 0 2 3 1 0 0
Golf I F 1 2 3 2 1 0
Soccer I F 1 3 4 8 1 1
Sport Ethnicity/Race
C AA H/L Multi
Baseball 24 1 1 2
Baseball 21 6 2 2
Basketball 4 7 0 1
Basketball 0 6 0 0
Track/CC 22 2 0 0
Track/CC 16 4 1 1
F. Hockey 13 0 0 0
Volleyball 1 4 1 0
Golf 8 1 0 0
Soccer 17 1 0 0
Note. Three athletes did not provide demographic data
related to sex, class or ethnicity. RSF=red-shirt freshman,
Fr=freshman, So=sophomore, Ju=junior, S=senior, 5th=5th year
senior, C=Caucasian,
AA=African American, H/L=Hispanic/Latino(a), Multi=multiple
ethnicities.
COPYRIGHT 2015 University of South Alabama
No portion of this article can be reproduced without the express written permission from the copyright holder.
Copyright 2015 Gale, Cengage Learning. All rights reserved.