首页    期刊浏览 2025年07月05日 星期六
登录注册

文章基本信息

  • 标题:Examining the Effects of Sport and Exercise Interventions on Body Image Among Adolescent Girls: A Systematic Review.
  • 作者:McIntosh-Dalmedo, Sharon ; Devonport, Tracey J. ; Nicholls, Wendy
  • 期刊名称:Journal of Sport Behavior
  • 印刷版ISSN:0162-7341
  • 出版年度:2018
  • 期号:September
  • 出版社:University of South Alabama
  • 摘要:Body image has been described as a person's perceptions, thoughts, and feelings about their body (Grogan, 2016). Body image is a multifaceted construct consisting of a variety of measured dimensions (Thompson, 2004; Thompson, Heinberg, Altabe, & Tantleff-Dunn, 1999). This includes perceptual, cognitive, affective and behavioral components (Bane & McAuley, 1998). Body image dissatisfaction (BID), defined as a subjective negative evaluation of one's physical appearance (Presnell, Bearman, & Stice, 2004), has been the focus of much research to date. In Western societies, BID is suggested to be so widespread, particularly among females (Salk & Engeln-Maddox, 2012), that is has been described as normative discontent (Cash & Smolak 2011; Hardit & Hannum, 2012; Rodin, Silberstein, & Striegel-Moore, 1984; Tantleff-Dunn, Barnes, & Larose, 2011).

    There is a wealth of research indicating that, across a lifespan, females are more likely to experience BID when compared to their male counterparts (Buchanan, Bluestein, Nappa, Woods, & Depatie, 2013; Elgin & Pritchard, 2006; Feingold & Mazzella, 1998; Sweeting & West, 2002). Indeed, girls as young as five years of age have been found to convey dissatisfaction with their body shape and (or) size (Davison, Markey, & Birch, 2000). As children transition from preadolescence into their next stage of maturity, adolescence becomes a significant phase in the development of body image (Fenton, Brooks, Spencer, & Morgan, 2010; Kostanski, Fisher, & Gullone, 2004). In the present study, we utilised the World Health Organisation (WHO) guidelines which describes adolescence as occurring between 10-19 years of age. Body image dissatisfaction has been reported to intensify during adolescence, most notably amongst girls, due to the bodily changes that take place during puberty (Bucchianeri, Arikian, Hannan, Eisenberg, & Neumark-Sztainer, 2013; Calzo et al., 2012; Maxwell & Cole, 2012). Furthermore, an increasing regard for the opinions of others during adolescence exacerbates the potential for BID (Reber & Reber, 2001). Despite adolescence presenting a potentially volatile time for body image, previous research has typically focused on young adults, with studies of adolescents being less common (Mellor et al., 2013; Rubin, Gluck, Knoll, Lorence, & Geliebter, 2008; Williams, Ricciardelli, McCabe, Waqa, & Bavadra, 2006).

Examining the Effects of Sport and Exercise Interventions on Body Image Among Adolescent Girls: A Systematic Review.


McIntosh-Dalmedo, Sharon ; Devonport, Tracey J. ; Nicholls, Wendy 等


Examining the Effects of Sport and Exercise Interventions on Body Image Among Adolescent Girls: A Systematic Review.

Body image has been described as a person's perceptions, thoughts, and feelings about their body (Grogan, 2016). Body image is a multifaceted construct consisting of a variety of measured dimensions (Thompson, 2004; Thompson, Heinberg, Altabe, & Tantleff-Dunn, 1999). This includes perceptual, cognitive, affective and behavioral components (Bane & McAuley, 1998). Body image dissatisfaction (BID), defined as a subjective negative evaluation of one's physical appearance (Presnell, Bearman, & Stice, 2004), has been the focus of much research to date. In Western societies, BID is suggested to be so widespread, particularly among females (Salk & Engeln-Maddox, 2012), that is has been described as normative discontent (Cash & Smolak 2011; Hardit & Hannum, 2012; Rodin, Silberstein, & Striegel-Moore, 1984; Tantleff-Dunn, Barnes, & Larose, 2011).

There is a wealth of research indicating that, across a lifespan, females are more likely to experience BID when compared to their male counterparts (Buchanan, Bluestein, Nappa, Woods, & Depatie, 2013; Elgin & Pritchard, 2006; Feingold & Mazzella, 1998; Sweeting & West, 2002). Indeed, girls as young as five years of age have been found to convey dissatisfaction with their body shape and (or) size (Davison, Markey, & Birch, 2000). As children transition from preadolescence into their next stage of maturity, adolescence becomes a significant phase in the development of body image (Fenton, Brooks, Spencer, & Morgan, 2010; Kostanski, Fisher, & Gullone, 2004). In the present study, we utilised the World Health Organisation (WHO) guidelines which describes adolescence as occurring between 10-19 years of age. Body image dissatisfaction has been reported to intensify during adolescence, most notably amongst girls, due to the bodily changes that take place during puberty (Bucchianeri, Arikian, Hannan, Eisenberg, & Neumark-Sztainer, 2013; Calzo et al., 2012; Maxwell & Cole, 2012). Furthermore, an increasing regard for the opinions of others during adolescence exacerbates the potential for BID (Reber & Reber, 2001). Despite adolescence presenting a potentially volatile time for body image, previous research has typically focused on young adults, with studies of adolescents being less common (Mellor et al., 2013; Rubin, Gluck, Knoll, Lorence, & Geliebter, 2008; Williams, Ricciardelli, McCabe, Waqa, & Bavadra, 2006).

In looking to develop and deliver body image interventions, there is growing support for the notion that participation in sport and exercise can enhance body image perceptions (Abbott & Barber, 2011; Daniels & Leaper, 2006; Fox, 2000; Hausenblas, Cook, & Chittester, 2008; Langdon & Petracca, 2010; Slater & Tiggemann, 2011; Swami & Tovee, 2009). Three meta-analyses demonstrated small (Campbell & Hausenblas, 2009; Hausenblas & Fallon, 2006) to moderate (Reel et al., 2007) effects of exercise on body image (based on 57, 121 and 35 studies respectively), whereby an exercise intervention had the observed outcome of improved body image. However, the design of any sport and/or exercise intervention must account for the complex nature of the association between body image and motivation to exercise, which may vary according to demographics. The above meta-analysis included broad demographic samples (males and females across a broad age range), and thus it is difficult to inform interventions for female adolescence with confidence. Whilst a desire to improve body image can act as a motivator to exercise in certain individuals, for others, it may present an obstacle for exercise participation (Focht & Hausenblas, 2004; Schuler et al., 2004). For example, Slater and Tiggemann (2010) noted that females (aged 13 to 15 years) frequently reported appearance-based concerns as a reason for ceasing participation in sport and exercise. In a follow up study, teasing and body image concerns appeared to contribute to reduced rates of participation in sports and other physical activities among adolescent girls (aged 12 to 16).

Within the present review, elucidating the outcomes of different sport and exercise interventions on body image may help identify effective strategies for enhancing body image. The aim of the present review is to provide a systematic evaluation of sport and (or) exercise interventions that seek to enhance body image among female adolescents. Specifically, this review will synthesize findings to address the following objectives:

(a) to examine the effects of sport and/or exercise interventions on female adoles cent body image;

(b) to critique included studies highlighting the implications for future research practice.

Method

To ensure methodological rigor, objectivity and replicability, the Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analyses guidelines (PRISMA; Higgins & Green, 2009; Petticrew & Roberts, 2005) were applied. The review was registered on PROSPERO (CRD42016037225) and all aims, inclusion criteria, data extraction, and data quality evaluation were specified at the outset.

Procedure for Search Strategy and Study Inclusion

A primary systematic search of six electronic databases through EBSCO was conducted (Medline, PsycINFO, SPORTDiscus, Child and Adolescent Studies, Education Research Complete, and Psychology and Behavioral Sciences) from inception up to and including March 2016. Search terms included "body image", "adolescence", "sport and exercise", and "intervention" (see Appendix for the complete search strategy). For inclusion, there had to be consensus that the following criteria were met:

(a) peer reviewed journal article published in the English language;

(b) data were reported from female adolescents;

(c) an exercise or sport intervention was delivered;

(d) intervention had a measured attempt to affect positive body image change;

(e) the outcomes were measured (quantitative or qualitative) with data at baseline and post-intervention;

(f) study sample should not include clinical populations or subpopulations with known differences that could obscure the direct examination of the effect of sport and exercise (e.g., illnesses or disease; physical disabilities, elite adole cent athletes, young offenders, or statemented adolescents with educational or behavioral and emotional needs).

Reference management software was used to organize citations (Endnote XI). The primary search yielded 4,210 records (see Figure 1), which following deduplication reduced to 3,073. These titles were independently screened by three reviewers to identify studies. Following title screening, 3,015 titles were excluded. Where there was disagreement, the full text manuscript was consulted, and for a paper to be included, there had to be consensus that the above inclusion criteria were met.

A full screen of the remaining 58 papers resulted in a further 52 exclusions as follows; (a) sport and/or exercise was not delivered as a body image intervention (n = 28); (b) non-target population (e.g., wrong age, clinical population, coaches; n = 17); (c) results not reported for females or male participants only (n = 4); (d) technology or computer-based interventions (n = 2) and, (e) a meta-analysis (n = 1 : Campbell & Hausenblas, 2009).

Data Extraction

Data extraction parameters were established in line with the research questions and data extraction was processed using Microsoft Excel. This facilitated the capture of pertinent information including sample, measures, intervention characteristics, body image outcomes, research limitations, and implications for future practice (see Table 1).

Data Quality

The methodological quality of the included studies was scored and assessed using the standard quality assessment for evaluating primary research papers (for details see Kmet et al., 2004). Kmet et al. (2004) provides comprehensive guidelines (pp. 14-22) to ensure that the scoring of quality within and between systematic reviews is completed to a given standard. Synthesizing data quality scores "provides a systematic, reproducible and quantitative means of simultaneously assessing the quality of research encompassing a broad range of study designs" (Kmet et al., 2004, p. 11). Studies were evaluated on 20 criteria spanning design, sampling, methodology, analysis, results and conclusions. For each criterion, benchmark statements are provided to guide scoring. Papers scored 2 (good), 1 (partial fulfilment), 0 (not fulfilled), or X (not relevant), possible score range was 0-2, with a higher score indicating better quality (Kmet et al., 2004). A mean score was calculated for each paper to give an overall rating of quality. In addition, a mean score for each of the sub-criteria was used to indicate the relative strengths and limitations across included studies, with the mean scores (SD) for individual studies presented in Table 1.

Results

Characteristics of the Included Studies

Included studies (see Table 1) originated from the USA (n = 2; Gehrman, Hovell, Sallis, & Keating, 2006; Waldron, 2007); UK (n = 2; Burgess et al., 2006; Daley & Buchanan, 1999); Canada (n = 1; Boyd & Hrycaiko, 1997); and Sweden (n = 1; Lindwall & Lindgren, 2005). One study included male and female participants but reported female data separately (Gehrman et al., 2006). The remaining studies had female only samples, with participant numbers ranging from 50 (Burgess et al., 2006) to 181 (Boyd & Hrycaiko, 1997). Mean age was reported in four studies, and ranged from 11.5 (SD = 0.96; Gehrman et al., 2006) to 16.35 years (SD = 1.56; Lindwall & Lindgren, 2005). Three studies failed to report ethnicity (Boyd & Hrycaiko, 1997; Daley & Buchanan, 1999; Lindwall & Lindgren, 2005), whilst two studies (Burgess et al., 2006; Waldron, 2007) simply reported ethnicity as predominately Caucasian. Gehrman et al. (2006) reported participants as 45% Caucasian, 38% Hispanic, 8% African-American, and 8% as "other" ethnic backgrounds. Social and economic data were presented in two studies reporting participants to be of lower socioeconomic status (Burgess et al. 2006; Lindwall & Lindgren, 2005).

Sport and exercise interventions were delivered within schools (n = 4; Boyd & Hrycaiko, 1997; Burgess et al., 2006; Daley & Buchanan, 1999; Lindwall & Lindgren, 2005) or the to the community (n = 2; Gehrman et al., 2006; Waldron, 2007). Of these, five prescribed exercise, those being weight circuits (Boyd & Hrycaiko, 1997), non-competitive games and weight-bearing activities (Gehrman et al., 2006), aerobics (Burgess et al., 2006; Daley & Buchanan, 1999) and an established 5km running training programme (Waldron, 2007). One study allowed self-selected exercise (Lindwall & Lindgren, 2005), with selected activities including: aerobics, water aerobics, step-up, spinning, dancing, yoga, badminton, kick-boxing, climbing, bowling, karate, jujitsu, and different ball-games. The duration of exercise and sport interventions ranged from five-weeks (Daley & Buchanan, 1999) to six-months (Lindwall & Lindgren, 2005). Five studies reported the session length, with interventions delivered through sessions varying from 40-minutes (Boyd & Hrycaiko, 1997) to 120-minutes (Gehrman et al., 2006), producing a mean of 72 minutes (SD = 32.71). Three studies combined taught or discussion-based components alongside exercise components (Boyd & Hrycaiko, 1997; Gehrman et al., 2006; Lindwall & Lindgren, 2005).

Six different measures of body image were utilized across included studies. Only one measure (Physical self-perception profile; Fox & Corbin, 1989) was common to two studies (Daley & Buchanan, 1999; Lindwall & Lindgren, 2005) and no further consistency in measurement of body image was observed. Other variables measured included; physiology (n = 3; Burgess et al., 2006; Gehrman et al., 2006; Lindwall & Lindgren, 2005), participation in exercise (n = 2; Burgess et al., 2006; Daley & Buchanan, 1999), and eating behavior (n = 1; Gehrman et al., 2006). Details of measures used are included in Table 1.

Narrative Synthesis of Findings and Discussion

Having first discussed data quality, the findings of this systematic evaluation are synthesized and discussed in accordance with the stated aims. These were (a) to examine the effects of sport and/or exercise interventions on body image (b) to critique included studies highlighting the implications for future research practice.

Data Quality

The possible range of scores on the quality assessment was 0-2, with a higher score indicating better quality (Kmet et al., 2004). The mean scores (SD) for individual studies are presented in Table 1. The overall mean score for data quality was 0.90 (SD = 0.22), pointing to a poor quality of research on this topic. The range was 0.67 (SD = 0.69; Boyd & Hrycaiko, 1997) to 1.16 (SD = 0.83; Lindwall & Lindgren, 2005). One of the included studies scored more than one standard deviation below the sample mean (Boyd & Hrycaiko, 1997). The mean scores (SD) for individual indicators of quality across all quantitative studies are presented in Table 2 (n = 6). Of the indicators that were assessed, studies performed particularly poorly on experimental methodology. This was due to limitations such as failure to account for or control confounding factors, randomization not being performed rigorously, nor with the appropriate blinding of participants or investigators, and a failure by all included studies to collect long-term follow up data. Studies overall performed comparatively well on use of suitable and validated predictor and outcome measures, and specifying a clear hypothesis.

The Effect of Sport and Exercise Interventions Body Image

Two studies revealed significant improvements in one or more sub measure of body image. Daley and Buchanan (1999) asked an experimental group to complete a one hour aerobics class, once a week, for five-weeks, in addition to compulsory physical education. The experimental group improved physical self-worth, sports competence, strength competence, and body attractiveness over time when compared to a comparison group (partaking in compulsory physical education one-hour per week). Changes in exercise behaviors were not reported for either the experimental or comparison group. In a six-month intervention, Lindwall and Lindgren (2005) offered 45-minutes of self-selected exercise activities and 14-minutes of discussion on healthy lifestyles twice a week. A waiting-list control group had no organized activity. After the intervention, when conducting analysis that excluded participants with missing data, the intervention significantly reduced social physique anxiety and increased scores on three subscales from the physical self-perception profile (sport competence, physical conditioning and physical self-worth). However, when undertaking more conservative intent-to-treat analysis (i.e., including all participants that were originally allocated to conditions), no effect of intervention on the physical self-perception profile was observed, but effect of the intervention on social physique anxiety (over time and between groups) was still observed, with those in the intervention having an improved score post-intervention. These positive changes were not associated with changes in physiological variables.

No intervention effect on body image was found across four studies. Boyd and Hrycaiko (1997) found no significant main effect of a six-week intervention (comprised of physical activity (training, cardiovascular and agility), education (healthy lifestyle, weight management, healthy role models), and self-report (logbooks tracking own performance) on general self-esteem when compared with a control group doing regular PE lessons. Gehrman et al. (2006) delivered an eight-week intervention, providing sessions designed to teach the importance of physical activity in overall health and non-competitive games and weight-bearing activities designed to enhance bone health. The intervention did not have a significant effect upon body dissatisfaction, drive for thinness, or weight concerns; with mean scores for body dissatisfaction following the same pattern for the intervention group when compared to the comparison group who completed training on injury prevention.

Burgess et al. (2006) utilized a cross-over design with two equivalent groups who participated in a comparison condition (conventional British physical education swimming program), and an experimental condition (aerobic dance). Both groups completed both conditions in a counterbalanced order. Within the inferential analysis, the differences between conditions were not reported, therefore for the purpose of comparing conditions, we report the means. No differences were found between the experimental and comparison condition in physical activity participation over time. Means show attractiveness was marginally higher, and disparagement and feeling fat marginally lower in the aerobic condition, when compared to the swimming condition. Waldron (2007) delivered a running intervention comprised of 90 minute sessions, twice a week for 8-12 weeks. There was no change in self-perception subscales observed post intervention. However, interviews indicated that following intervention, the girls expressed greater acceptance of the self, improved physical fitness, and enhanced knowledge of taking care of their body.

Critique of Included Atudies and Recommendations for Future Research Practice

Analysis of the data quality assessment highlighted that included studies were poor in their application of experimental method, which might have introduced bias or confounding factors. In particular, none of the included studies detailed whether the aims of the study were concealed from participants. This may mean that participants could guess the aims of the study and be unduly influenced. Likewise, investigators were not blinded as to the group's activities when analysis took place, and again this may introduce bias. In addition, key details were omitted from reporting. For example, only one study reported the level of attendance at the intervention (Lindwall & Lindgren), at a rate of 56% (SD = 19%). Where no effect is observed, poor attendance or adherence with the intervention may be an explanatory factor.

Sampling strategies used in the included studies present limitations that require consideration. The predominant strategies used were convenience samples from schools (Boyd et al., 1997; Daley & Buchanan, 1999), or the community (Waldron, 2007), or selectively sampled participants for characteristics such as low physical activity levels (Burgess et al., 2006; Gehrman et al., 2006; Lindwall & Lindgren, 2005). Two studies (Gehrman et al., 2006; Lindwall & Lindgren, 2005) randomly assigned participants to conditions. Daley and Buchanan (1999) acknowledge that random sampling is preferable, but state that this was impractical in a school setting where girls were asked to voluntarily stay behind after school for intervention activities.

Studies comprised of volunteers (e.g., Daley & Buchanan, 1999), selective sampling of participants scoring low at baseline testing on a variable of interest (e.g., body attitudes, Burgess et al., 2006), or selective sampling from a population hypothesized to be less physically active (e.g., low socioeconomic status communities; Burgess et al., 2006; Lindwall & Lindgren, 2005). Volunteer-based recruitment strategies might result in biased samples because participants might have an increased desire to take part (Lubans & Sylva, 2006; Mauriello et al., 2010). Selective sampling of participants scoring low on a variable of interest increases the likelihood that improvements on this variable may be observed over time as compared to a sample scoring higher at baseline. This was illustrated by Walters and Martin (2000) who found no significant improvement in the self-concept of school children (male and female) following a 13-week aerobic exercise intervention. The authors suggested that as pretest self-concept scores were generally high, this left limited scope for further increases (a ceiling effect). Similarly, Raglin (1990) found exercise did not decrease levels of depression in those whose initial scores fall within normal range, noting "exercise does not make normals more normal" (p. 325).

When evaluating control or comparison conditions, one study had a waiting list control group who were inactive (Lindwall & Lindgren, 2005), whilst one study employed a within subjects design (Waldron, 2007). Three studies included a comparison group participating in a different sport or exercise activity to the intervention group (Boyd & Hrycaiko, 1997; Burgess et al., 2006; Daley & Buchanan, 1999), whilst one study had a comparison group completing a non-sport activity (Gehrman et al., 2006). This presents a confounding factor, as if sport and exercise activities were to have an effect on body image, this effect should also be observed in any comparison group participating in a different sport or exercise activity to the intervention group. Comparison group activities are described in Table 1.

When choosing to utilize swimming as the comparison group activity, Burgess et al. (2006) rationalised that swimming has been reported to be one of the most disliked physical education activities for adolescent females, and may not benefit body image. Dislike for swimming is partially attributed to the tight, form-fitting, swimsuits that are associated with increases in self-consciousness and body image dissatisfaction (Reel, Petrie, SooHoo, & Anderson, 2013). Evidence for this contention was provided by Thogersen-Ntoumani, Ntoumanis, Cumming, Bartholomew, and Pearce (2011) who demonstrated that wearing tight/ revealing exercise attire heightened physically active female university students' levels of state self-objectification. Among individuals with low self-esteem, this heightened self-objectification associated with low satisfaction with body shape and size. With regards, the findings of Burgess et al. (2006), they found that attractiveness was marginally higher, and disparagement and feeling fat marginally lower in the aerobic condition, when compared to the swimming condition.

There was little consistency in the physical activity offered as an intervention across included studies. An aerobics based intervention had a positive change on body image (Daley & Buchanan, 1999), with the authors noting that female participants may have greater confidence in their capacity to participate in gender typed 'feminine' activities (Clifton & Gill, 1994; Lirgg 1991). A second study delivering an aerobics intervention (Burgess et al., 2006) did not have a significant effect on body image, however, as previously noted, the poor methodology applied in this study, and the consequent lower data quality score mean that less emphasis should be placed on this study in the context of this review. Where a competitive element was implied in the intervention delivered (e.g., training for a 5k race, Waldron, 2007), no significant effect of intervention on body image was observed. It has been suggested that for female adolescents in particular, competitive sports may increase unrealistic expectations and have a negative influence on self-concept, self-esteem and self-confidence (Cox, Schofield, & Kolt, 2010).

Three included studies provided multiple physical activities as interventions. Where this involved self-selected activities along with a discussion component (Lindwall & Lindgren, 2005), positive change in body image was found. By contrast, two studies providing multiple physical activities alongside a logbook and education (Boyd, 1997; Gehrman, et al., 2006) had no significant effect on body image. It may be that presenting the opportunity to partake in self-selected activities was the key factor in determining the positive change in body image observed following the intervention delivered by Lindwall and Lindgren (2005). Arguably self-selecting exercise activities supports self-determined behaviors, and may therefore create more opportunity to have a positive effect on body image (Thogersen-Ntoumani & Ntoumanis, 2007). In the study by Lindwall and Lindgren (2005), activities were designed to encourage self-control, pride in their body, and competence through promoting their abilities to meet their needs, and where necessary to engage other resources. Exercise enjoyment was a key aim of this study.

An important consideration when undertaking body image research is the selection of measures used, with assessment errors characterizing much of the work in this area (Thompson, 2004). Thompson (2004) notes that it is vitally important that researchers clearly identify the dimension of body image they wish to investigate, and then select a measure that assesses this specific dimension. Attention to such detail is necessary to help understand which, if any, aspect of body image is responsive to sport and exercise interventions. It is quite plausible that some components may not change (e.g., body image investment), whereas, other components (e.g., weight-specific dissatisfaction) may improve. Thus, clearly articulating and adhering to dimensions of body image underpins the evaluation and advancement of theories of body image (Thompson, 2004). Whilst the measures used (see Table 1), along with the titles of included studies, infer a focus on body image perceptions across the majority of included studies (Burgess et al., 2006; Daley & Buchanan, 1999; Lindwall & Lindgren, 2005; Waldron, 2007), none of the included studies sufficiently acknowledged the dimension of body image assessed, or justify the measure(s) of body image used.

Similarly, measuring physical activity can be a complicated endeavour, as all measures have known limitations (Baranoski, Thompson, Durant, Baranoski, & Puhl, 1993). Failing to sufficiently control basic exercise variables (frequency, duration, and intensity) in order to account for fitness improvements and heterogeneity of outcome measures was a limitation of the included studies (Campbell & Hausenblas, 2009). Physical activity (frequency, intensity and type) was commonly recorded via self-report (Boyd & Hrycaiko, 1997; Burgess et al., 2006; Daley & Buchanan, 1999). This does not provide the most accurate form of recording information, particularly in both pediatric and adolescent populations, as this subjective technique may lead to inaccuracy, falsification, or over, or underestimation of actual physical activity levels (Godin, Jobin, & Bouillon, 1986; Sallis & Saelens, 2000; Shephard, 2003; Sirard & Pate, 2001; Welk et al., 2000). None of the included studies analyzed data pertaining to the intensity of the activity participants engaged with in the intervention. Exercise intensity is positively correlated with psychological benefits in adolescents (Biddle & Asare, 2011). This is therefore a variable to consider both in the design of the interventions, and as a covariate in assessing the effect of interventions on outcome measures that are salient. Usage of accelerometers in gathering data would enable physical activity to be measures with greater precision and confidence. In particular thigh-worn accelerometers support a more sensitive and specific evaluation of exercise frequency, duration, and intensity (Montoye, Pivarnik, Mudd, Biswas, & Pfeiffer, 2016).

With respect to the treatment of data, there were examples where the analysis undertaken was inappropriate, meaning that the findings could not sufficiently address the study aims. For example, Burgess et al. (2006) employed a counterbalanced design including two groups and two conditions across three time points. Whilst this was an example of good study design, the analysis did not include a variable to describe 'condition'. Therefore, the effect of the condition on the outcome variables could not be assessed. In a further example, Boyd and Hrycaiko (1997) substituted a 'difference' score into the ANOVA, instead of using the raw scores for pre and post-intervention. Consequently, the effect of the interaction between groups over time on body image could not be assessed, and the lack of descriptive statistics makes interpretation of their findings difficult. Similarly, Gehrman et al. (2006), included post-test scores as a co-variate, so again there was no clear analysis of the effect of the intervention on body image over time.

Included studies reported a large number of analyses. The reporting was unclear in some cases, and did not allow for the number of analyses conducted to be discerned (e.g., Boyd & Hrycaiko, 1997). Conducting many analyses increases the possibility of a type one error, with only two studies controlling for this in their interpretation of findings (Daley & Buchanan, 1999; Waldron, 2007). In some cases, small sample sizes precluded the application of multivariate analysis, meaning that repeated paired t-tests (Waldron, 2007) or ANOVA (Boyd & Hrycaiko, 1997) were used. For other studies, it was unclear why some of the analyses were presented. For example, Gehrman et al. (2006) included gender as an independent variable, where it would have been more appropriate to their aims to include pre and post-intervention scores in the analyses as an independent variable, as their study aimed to examine the effect of the intervention on body image. Boyd and Hrycaiko (1997) applied a mean-split based on scores on self-concept, and used this as an independent variable in analyses, although the independent variables were described as the intervention condition versus control condition and the age of participants. Only one study reported a strategy for the imputation of missing variables (Lindwall & Lindgren, 2005). The same study also reported intent-to-treat analyses, which include all participants who are randomized to a treatment (including drop-outs) and are therefore more conservative than analyses of only those who do not drop out.

Conclusion

The present review evidences two interventions which had a positive effect on body image. These two studies indicate that the use of gender-aligned, or self-selected sport and exercise activities, undertaken in conjunction with careful discussion around empowering adolescent females, may have positive outcomes for body image. However, an ability to draw conclusions regarding the potential for sport and exercise interventions to help promote positive body image among female adolescents is presently limited by the overall poor quality of research in this area. There is a need to undertake future research with greater methodological rigor as detailed by the present systematic review. This includes more careful attention to considerations such as participant sampling, control conditions/groups, measurement of key variables, intervention features, and analysis of data.

Appendix A

Example search strategy

1. Body?image

2. Self?image

3. Physical self?efficacy

4. Body?satisfaction

5. Body?shame

6. Physical self concept

7. Physical self-concept

8. Adolescen*

9. Young?adult

10. Teen*

11. Child*

12. Physical activity

13. Sport*

14. Exercis*

15. Interv*

16. Train*

17. 1 or 2 or 3 or 4 or 5 or 6 or 7

18. 8 or 9 or 10 or 11

19. 12 or 13 or 14

20. 15 or 16

21. 17 and 18 and 19 and 20

Sharon McIntosh-Dalmedo, Tracey J. Devonport, Wendy Nicholls, and Andrew P. Friesen

University of Wolverhampton

Address correspondence to: Tracey Devonport, Institute of Sport and Human Science, University of Wolverhampton, UK. Email: T.Devonport@wlv.ac.uk

References

Aaron, D. J., Kriska, A. M., Dearwater, S. R., Cauley, J. A., Metz, K. F., & LaPorte, R. E. (1995). Reproducibility and validity of an epidemiologic questionnaire to assess past year physical activity in adolescents. American Journal of Epidemiology, 142(2), 191-201. doi: 10.1093/oxfordjournals.aje.al 17618

Abbott, B. D., & Barber, B. L. (2011). Differences in functional and aesthetic body image between sedentary girls and girls involved in sports and physical activity: Does sport type make a difference? Psychology of Sport and Exercise, 12, 333-342. doi: 10.1016/j. psychsport.2010.10.005

Bane, S., & McAuley, E. (1998). Body image and exercise. In J. L. Duda (Ed.), Advances in sport and exercise psychology measurement (pp. 311-324). Morgantown, WV: Fitness Information Technology.

Baranowski, T., Thompson, W. O., Durant, R. H., Baranowski, J, & Puhl, J. (1993). Observations on physical activity in physical locations: Age, gender, ethnicity, and month effects. Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport, 64, 127-133. doi: 10.1080/02701367.1 993.10608789

Ben-Tovim, D. I., & Walker, M. K. (1991). The development of the Ben-Tovim Walker Body Attitudes Questionnaire (BAQ), a new measure of women's attitudes towards their own bodies. Psychological Medicine, 27(3), 775-784. doi:10.1017/S0033291700022406

Biddle, S. J., & Asare, M. (2011). Physical activity and mental health in children and adolescents: A review of reviews. British Journal of Sports Medicine, 45. 886-895. doi: 10.1136/bjsports-2011-090185

Boyd, K. R., & Hrycaiko, D. W. (1997). The effect of a physical activity intervention package on the self-esteem of pre-adolescent and adolescent females. Adolescence, 32, 693-708.

Bucchianeri, M. M., Arikian, A. J., Hannan, P. J., Eisenberg, M. E., & Neumark-Sztainer, D. (2013). Body dissatisfaction from adolescence to young adulthood: Findings from a 10year longitudinal study. Body Image, 10, 1-7. doi:10.1016/j.bodyim.2012.09.001

Buchanan, N. T., Bluestein, B. M., Nappa, A. C., Woods, K. C., & Depatie, M. M. (2013). Exploring gender differences in body image, eating pathology, and sexual harassment. Body Image, 10, 352-360. doi:10.1016/j.bodyim.2013.03.004

Burgess, G., Grogan, S., & Burwitz, L. (2006). Effects of a 6-week aerobic dance intervention on body image and physical self-perceptions in adolescent girls. Body Image, 3(1), 57-66. doi: 10.1016/j.bodyim.2005.10.005

Calzo, J. P., Sonneville, K. R., Haines, J., Blood, E. A., Field, A. E., & Austin, S. B. (2012). The development of associations among body mass index, body dissatisfaction, and weight and shape concern in adolescent boys and girls. Journal of Adolescent Health, 51, 517-523. doi: 10.1016/j.jadohealth.2012.02.021

Campbell, A., & Hausenblas, H. A. (2009). Effects of exercise interventions on body image: A meta-analysis. Journal of Health Psychology, 14, 780-793. doi: 10.1177/1359105309338977

Cash, T. F., & Smolak, L. (Eds.). (2011). Body image: A handbook of science, practice, and prevention. New York: Guilford Press.

Clifton. R T., & Gill, D. L. (1994). Gender differences in self-confidence on a feminine-typed task. Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology, 16, 150-160. doi : 10.1123/ jsep. 16.2.150

Cox, M., Schofield, G., & Kolt, G. S. (2010). Responsibility for children's physical activity: Parental, child, and teacher perspectives. Journal of Science and Medicine in Sport, 13, 46-52. doi: 10.1016/j.jsams.2009.02.006

Daley, A. J., & Buchanan, J. (1999). Aerobic dance and physical self-perceptions in female adolescents: some implications for physical education. Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport, 70, 196-200. doi: 10.1080/02701367.1999.10608037

Daley, A. J., & Parfitt, G. (1996). Good health--Is it worth it? Mood states, physical well-being, job satisfaction and absenteeism in members and non-members of a British corporate health and fitness club. Journal of Occupational and Organizational Psychology, 69(2), 121-134.

Daniels, E., & Leaper, C. (2006). A longitudinal investigation of sport participation, peer acceptance, and self-esteem among adolescent girls and boys. Sex Roles, 55, 875-880. doi: 10.1007/s 11199-006-9138-4

Davison, K. K., Markey, C. N., & Birch, L. (2000). Etiology of body dissatisfaction and weight concerns among 5-year-old girls. Appetite, 35, 143-151. doi: 10.1006/ appe.2000.0349

Elgin, J., & Pritchard, M. (2006). Gender differences in disordered eating and its correlates. Eating and Weight Disorders, 11, 96-101. doi:10.1007/BF03327565

Feingold, A., & Mazzella, R. (1998). Gender differences in body image are increasing. Psychological Sciences, 9, 190-195. doi: 10.1111/1467-9280.00036

Fenton, C., Brooks, F., Spencer, N. H., & Morgan, A. (2010). Sustaining a positive body image in adolescence: An assets-based analysis. Health & Social Care in the Community, 18, 189-198. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2524.2009.00888.x

Focht, B. C., & Hausenblas, H. A. (2004). Perceived evaluative threat and state anxiety during exercise in women with social physique anxiety. Journal of Applied Sport Psychology, 16, 361-368. doi: 10.1080/10413200490517968

Fox, K. R. (2000). The effects of exercise on self-perceptions and self-esteem. In S. J. H. Biddle, K. R. Fox, & S. H. Boutcher (Eds.), Physical activity and psychological well-being (pp. 88-117). London: Routledge.

Fox, K. R., & Corbin, C. B. (1989). The physical self-perception profile: Development and preliminary validation. Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology, 11(4), 408-430.

Garner, D. M. (1991). Eating disorder inventory-2. Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources.

Gehrman, C. A., Hovell, M. F., Sallis, J. F., & Keating, K. (2006). The effects of a physical activity and nutrition intervention on body dissatisfaction, drive for thinness, and weight concerns in pre-adolescents. Body Image, 3(4), 345-351. doi: 10.1016/j.bodyim.2006.07.006

Godin, G., Jobin, J., & Bouillon, J. (1986). Assessment of leisure time exercise behavior by self-report: A concurrent validity study. Canadian Journal of Public Health, 77, 359-362.

Reel, J. J., Greenleaf, C., Baker, W. K., Aragon, S., Bishop, D., Cachaper, C., ... & Hattie, J. (2007). Relations of body concerns and exercise behavior: A meta-analysis. Psychological reports, 101(3), 927-942. doi:10.2466/pr0.101.3.927-942

Grogan, S. (2016). Body image: Understanding body dissatisfaction in men, women and children. London, Routledge.

Hardit, S. K., & Hannum, J. W. (2012). Attachment, the tripartite influence model, and the development of body dissatisfaction. Body Image, 9, 469-475. doi: 10.1016/j.body im.2012.06.003

Hart, E. A., Leary, M. R., & Rejeski, W. J. (1989). The Measurement of Social Physique Anxiety. Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology, 11(1), 94-104.

Harter, S. (1988). Manual for the self-perception profile for adolescents. Denver, CO: University of Denver.

Hausenblas, H. A., Cook, B. J., & Chittester, N. I. (2008). Can exercise treat eating disorders? Exercise and Sport Sciences Reviews, 36, 43-47. doi: 10.1097/jes.ObO13e31815e4040

Hausenblas, H. A., & Fallon, E. A. (2006). Exercise and body image: A meta-analysis. Psychology and Health, 21, 33-47. doi: 10.1080/14768320500105270

Higgins, J., & Green, S. (2009). Cochrane handbook for systematic reviews of interventions. Oxford: Wiley & Sons Ltd.

Killen, J. D., Taylor, C. B., Hayward, C., Wilson, D. M., Haydel, K. F., Hammer, L., ... Kraemer, H. (1994). Pursuit of thinness and onset of eating disorders symptoms in a community sample of adolescent girls: A three-year prospective analysis. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 16, 227-238. doi.org/10.1002/1098-108X(199411)16:3<227::AIDEAT2260160303>3.0.CO;2-L

Kmet, L. M., Lee, R. C., & Cook, L. S. (2004). Standard quality assessment criteria for evaluating primary research papers from a variety of fields. The Alberta Heritage Foundation for Medical Research. Edmonton, Canada, https://www.ihe.ca/publications/ standard-quality-assessment-criteria-for-evaluating-primary-research-papers-from-a-variety-of-fields

Kostanski, M., Fisher, A., & Gullone, E. (2004). Current conceptualisation of body image dissatisfaction: Have we got it wrong? Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 45, 1317-1325. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2004.00315.x

Kriemler, S., Meyer, U., Martin, E., Van Sluijs, E. M. F., Andersen, L. B., & Martin, B. W. (2011). Effect of school-based interventions on physical activity and fitness in children and adolescents: A review of reviews and systematic update. British Journal of Sports Medicine, 45, 923-930. doi: 10.1136/bjsports-2011-090186.

Langdon, S. W., & Petracca, G. (2010). Tiny dancer: Body image and dancer identity in female modern dancers. Body Image, 7, 360-363. doi:10.1016/j.bodyim.2010.06.005

Liggett, G., Grogan, S., & Burwitz, L. (2003, September). The effectiveness of a six-week aerobic dance intervention on body image dissatisfaction among adolescent females. Paper presented at the meeting of British Psychological Society, Division of Health Psychology Conference, Stafford, UK.

Lindwall, M., & Lindgren, E. C. (2005). The effects of a 6-month exercise intervention programme on physical self-perceptions and social physique anxiety in non-physically active adolescent Swedish girls. Psychology of Sport and Exercise, 6, 643-658. doi: 10.1016/j.psychsport.2005.03.003

Lirgg, C. D. (1991). Gender differences in self-confidence in physical activity: A meta-analysis of recent studies. Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology, 13, 294-310. doi:10.1123/jsep.13.3.294

Lubans D., & Sylva K. (2006). Controlled evaluation of a physical activity intervention for senior school students: Effects of the lifetime activity program. Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology, 28, 252-268. doi:10.1123/jsep.28.3.252

Marsh, H. (1998). The self-description questionnaire: SDQ manual and research monograph. San Antonio: Psychological Corporation.

Mauriello, L. M., Ciavatta, M. M. H., Paiva, A. L., Sherman, K. J., Castle, P. H., Johnson, J. L., & Prochaska, J. M. (2010). Results of a multi-media multiple behavior obesity prevention program for adolescents. Preventive Medicine, 51, 451-456. doi: 10.1016/j.ypmed.2010.08.004

Maxwell, M. A., & Cole, D. A. (2012). Development and initial validation of the adolescent responses to body dissatisfaction measure. Psychological Assessment, 24, 721-737. doi: 10.1037/a0026785

Mellor, D., Waterhouse, M., Mamat, N. H. B., Xu, X., Cochrane, J., McCabe, M., & Ricciardelli, L. (2013). Which body features are associated with female adolescents' body dissatisfaction? A cross-cultural study in Australia, China and Malaysia. Body Image, 10, 54-61. doi: 10.1016/j.bodyim.2012.10.002

Montoye, A. H., Pivarnik, J. M., Mudd, L. M., Biswas, S., & Pfeiffer, K. A. (2016). Validation and comparison of accelerometers worn on the hip, thigh, and wrists for measuring physical activity and sedentary behavior. AIMS Public Health, 3(2), 298-312. doi: 10.3934/publichealth.2016.2.298

Morris, N. M., & Udry, J. R. (1980). Validation of a self-administered instrument to assess stage of adolescent development. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 9(3), 271-280.

Patel, V., Kieling, C., Maulik, P. K., & Divan, G. (2013). Improving access to care for children with mental disorders: a global perspective. Archives of Disease in Childhood, 98, 323-327. doi: 10.1136/archdischild-2012-302079

Petticrew, M., & Roberts, H. (2005). Systematic reviews in the social sciences: A practical guide. Oxford: Wiley-Blackwell Ltd.

Presnell, K., Bearman, S. K., & Stice, E. (2004). Risk factors for body dissatisfaction in adolescent boys and girls: A prospective study. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 36, 389-401. doi:10.1002/eat.20045

Raglin, J. S. (1990). Exercise and mental health: Beneficial and detrimental effects. Sports Medicine, 9, 323-329. doi: 10.2165/00007256-199009060-00001

Reber, A. S., & Reber, E. S. (2001). The penguin dictionary of psychology (3rd ed.). New York: Penguin Books.

Reel, J. J., Greenleaf, C., Baker, W. K., Aragon, S., Bishop, D., Cachaper, C., ... & Hattie, J. (2007). Relations of body concerns and exercise behavior: A meta-analysis. Psychological Reports, 101, 927-942. doi: 10.2466/pro.101.3.927-942.

Reel, J. J., Petrie, T. A., SooHoo, S., & Anderson, C. M. (2013). Weight pressures in sport: Examining the factor structure and incremental validity of the weight pressures in sport-females. Eating Behaviors, 14, 137-144. doi: 10.1016/j.eatbeh.2013.01.003

Rodin, J., Silberstein, L., & Striegel-Moore, R. (1984). Women and weight: a normative discontent. In T. B. Sondregger (Ed.), Nebraska symposium on motivation 1984: Psychology and Gender (pp. 267-307). Lincoln, NE: University of Nebraska Press.

Rubin, B., Gluck, M. E., Knoll, C. M., Lorence, M., & Geliebter, A. (2008). Comparison of eating disorders and body image disturbances between eastern and western countries. Eating and Weight Disorders, 13, 73-80. doi:10.1007/BF03327606

Salk, R. H., & Engeln-Maddox, R. (2012). Fat talk among college women is both contagious and harmful. Sex Roles, 66, 636-645. doi: 10.1007/s 11199-011-0050-1.

Sallis, J. F., & Saelens, B. E. (2000). Assessment of physical activity by self-report: status, limitations, and future directions. Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport, 71, 1-14. doi: 10.1080/02701367.2000.11082780

Schilder, P. (1950). The image and appearance of the human body. New York: International Universities Press.

Schuler, P. B., Broxon-Hutcherson, A., Philipp, S. F., Ryan, S., Isosaari, R. M., & Robinson, D. (2004). Body-shape perceptions in older adults and motivations for exercise. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 98, 1251-1260. doi.org/10.2466/pms.98.3c. 1251-1260

Shephard, R. J. (2003). Limits to the measurement of habitual physical activity by questionnaires. British Journal of Sports Medicine, 37, 197-206. doi: 10.1136/bjsm.37.3.197

Sirard, J. R., & Pate, R. R. (2001). Physical activity assessment in children and adolescents. Sports Medicine, 31, 439-454. doi: 10.2165/00007256-200131060-00004

Slater, A., & Tiggemann, M. (2011). Gender differences in adolescent sport participation, teasing, self-objectification and body image concerns. Journal of Adolescence, 34, 455-463. doi: 10.1016/j.adolescence.2010.06.007

Slater, A., & Tiggemann, M. (2010). "Uncool to do sport": A focus group study of adolescent girls' reasons for withdrawing from physical activity. Psychology of Sport and Exercise, 11(6), 619-626. doi:10.1016/j.psychsport.2010.07.006

Swami, V., & Tovee, M. J. (2009). A comparison of actual-ideal weight discrepancy, body appreciation, and media influence between street-dancers and non-dancers. Body Image, 6, 304-307. doi: 10.1016/j.bodyim.2009.07.006

Sweeting, H., & West, P. (2002). Gender differences in weight related concerns in early to late adolescence. Journal of Epidemiology and Community Health, 56, 701-702. doi: 10.1136/jech.56.9.700

Tantleff-Dunn, S., Barnes, R. D., & Larose, J. G. (2011). It's not just a "woman thing:" The current state ofnormative discontent. Eating Disorders, 19, 392-402. doi: 10.1080/10640266.2011.609088

Thogersen-Ntoumani, C., & Ntoumanis, N. (2007). A self-determination theory approach to the study of body image concerns, self-presentation and self-perceptions in a sample of aerobic instructors. Journal of Health Psychology, 12(2), 301-315. doi: 10.1177/1359105307074267

Thogersen-Ntoumani, C., Ntoumanis, N., Cumming, J., Bartholomew, K. J., & Pearce, G. (2011). Can self-esteem protect against the deleterious consequences of self-objectification for mood and body satisfaction in physically active female university students? Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology, 33, 289-307. doi: 10.1123/jsep.33.2.289

Thompson, J. K. (2004). The (mis)measurement of body image: Ten strategies to improve assessment for applied and research purposes. Body Image, 1, 7-14. doi:10.1016/S17401445(03)00004-4

Thompson, J. K., Hemberg, L. J., Altabe, M., & Tantleff-Dunn, S. (1999). Exacting beauty. Theory, assessment, and treatment of body image disturbance. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association,

van Sluijs, E. M. F., McMinn, A. M., & Griffin, S. J. (2007). Effectiveness of interventions to promote physical activity in children and adolescents: Systematic review of controlled trials. British Medical Journal, 335, 703-707. doi:10.1136/bmj.39320.843947

Waldron, J. J. (2007). Influence of involvement in the girls on track program on early adolescent girls' self-perceptions. Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport, 78, 520-530. doi: 10.1080/02701367.2007.10599451

Walters, S. T., & Martin, J. E. (2000). Does aerobic exercise really enhance self-esteem in children? A prospective evaluation in 3rd-5th graders. Journal of Sport Behavior, 23, 51-60.

Caption: Figure 1. Selection of Studies for Inclusion in Review
Table 1
Characteristics of included Studie

                    Mean
Author & Year   Age (a) (sd)         Design

Boyd &          NR             Experimental
Hrycaiko,       (range         design
1997            9-16)          Comparing
                               age group
                               (Pre, early,
                               mid adolescence)
                               and comparing
                               intervention
                               with a
                               comparison
                               condition (c).
                               Measures
                               taken pre
                               and post
                               intervention.

Burgess,        13.5           Experimental
Grogan, &       (0.3)          design
Burwitz,                       comparing
2006                           intervention and
                               comparison
                               groups.
                               Counterbalanced
                               intervention
                               delivery.
                               Measures
                               pre, mid,
                               post-intervention.
                               and 12-week
                               follow up.

Daley &         NR             Experimental
Buchanan,       (range         design
1999            15-16)         comparing
                               intervention
                               (n = 43) and
                               comparison
                               condition
                               (n = 70). Measures
                               taken pre and
                               post-intervention.

Gehrman,        11.5           Experimental
Hovell,         (0.96) (b)     design. Random
Sallis, &                      allocation
Keating,                       to intervention
2006                           (n = 49) or
                               comparison
                               condition
                               (n = 35). Measures
                               at pre and
                               post-intervention.

Lindwall        16.35          Experimental
& Lindgren,     (1.56)         design with random
2005                           assignment to
                               intervention
                               group (n = 27,
                               or waiting list
                               control group
                               (n = 35).
                               Measures
                               at pre and
                               post-intervention.

Waldron,        11.51          Mixed design.
2007            (0.37)         with no
                               control group.
                               Measures at
                               first and final
                               week of the
                               course.

                                     Intervention-sport/
Author & Year         Sample            exercise type

Boyd &          A non-random         Three components;
Hrycaiko,       convenience          (1) physical
1997            sample of 181        activity
                school girls,        (strength
                described as         training, CV and
                pre, early.          agility--precise
                and mid              activities were
                adolescence.         tailored to
                Subjects             the group),
                scoring in           (2) education
                the 50th             (healthy life
                percentile           style, weight
                or lower on          management,
                the SDQ were         healthy role
                assigned to the      models), (3)
                low self-esteem      self-report
                group.               (log books tracking
                                     own performance).

Burgess,        50 British           Aerobic
Grogan, &       school girls         dance--warm up,
Burwitz,        from a               dance workout,
2006            specialist           cool down &
                sports college;      stretch.
                selective            Control group
                sampling of          partook in
                participants         swimming
                with low             lessons.
                physical
                self-perception
                and high
                body image
                dissatisfaction.

Daley &         113 British school   Aerobics
Buchanan,       girls (from a        (warm up,
1999            single-sex school)   aerobic workout.
                                     resistance.
                                     cool down and
                                     stretches).

Gehrman,        84 American          Parent groups
Hovell,         children             taught behavior
Sallis, &       (females             modification
Keating,        n = 52, males        techniques
2006            n = 32).             to increase
                Participants         activity and
                were eligible        change dietary
                if their BM1         habits in children.
                <32, and             Children
                they were            participated in
                not currently        fitness sessions
                participating        and provided
                in organized         information
                sports for           regarding nutrition.
                three or             Children
                more days            completed
                per week             activity logs.
                                     The comparison
                                     group completed
                                     family sessions
                                     on the subject of
                                     injury nrevention.

Lindwall        Non-physically       Self-selected
& Lindgren,     active Swedish       exercise activities
2005            adolescent           and discussion
                girls recruited      on healthy
                from schools.        lifestyles.
                Eligible if they     Control group
                were active          had no organized
                for less than        activity.
                20 minutes
                per day,
                exercise less
                than once a
                week, and
                medically
                healthy.

Waldron,        An                   Structured
2007            American             coaching
                community            programme
                sample of 34         based on
                schoolgirls.         Harter's model
                                     of competence
                                     motivation
                                     culminating in
                                     a 5km running
                                     race.

                   Frequency
Author & Year     and Duration           Measures

Boyd &          40-minute          Selt-Description
Hrycaiko,       sessions           Questionnaire
1997            over six           I and II
                weeks.             (Marsh, 1988)
                Pre-adolescent     subscales;
                participants       general
                had nine           self-esteem,
                sessions.          physical
                Early and          self-concept,
                mid-adolescent     physical abilities,
                groups had 12.     physical
                                   appearance.

Burgess,        Fifty-minute       Body attitude
Grogan, &       classes, twice     questionnaire
Burwitz,        a week for         (Ben-Tovim
2006            six-weeks.         & Walker 1991)
                                   Children and
                                   Youth Physical
                                   Self-perception
                                   profile
                                   (Fox & Corbin, 1989)
                                   Leisure time
                                   physical
                                   activity
                                   questionnaire
                                   (Aaron, Kriska, &
                                   Dearwater, 1995).
                                   Body Mass Index.

Daley &         One hour           Physical
Buchanan,       of physical        self-perception
1999            education          profile (Fox &
                plus one           Corbin, 1989)
                hour of            Participation
                aerobics for       in physical
                five-weeks.        activity
                Control            questionnaire
                group              (Daley &
                partook in         Parfitt, 1996).
                one hour
                of physical
                education
                per week.

Gehrman,        An eightweek       Eating Disorders
Hovell,         physical           Inventory-2
Sallis, &       activity and       (Garner, 1991)
Keating,        nutrition          Weight concerns
2006            intervention.      scale (Killen
                Frequency and      et al., 1994),
                duration of        physical
                sessions not       maturity--Tanner
                reported.          Scale Drawings
                Control            (Morris & Uldry,
                condition          1980), Body
                was two-hour       Mass Index,
                family sessions,   Parental Body
                delivered weekly   Mass Index.
                for eightweeks.

Lindwall        45-min             Physical
& Lindgren,     exercise           self-perception
2005            sessions           profile
                followed           (Fox & Corbin, 1989).
                by 15-min          Social physique
                healthy            anxiety scale
                lifestyle          (Hart, Leary, &
                discussion         Rejeski 1989)
                delivered          Body mass
                twice a            index and
                week for           submaximal
                six-months.        oxygen uptake.

Waldron,        Ninety             Selt-perception
2007            minute             profile for
                Girls on           adolescents
                Track              (subscales;
                running            physical
                sessions,          competence,
                twice a            physical
                week for           appearance
                eight-12           competence,
                weeks.             self-worth;
                                   Harter, 1988).
                                   Semi-structured
                                   interviews.

                                        Mean data
Author & Year      Main findings       quality (sd)

Boyd &          No significant         0.67
Hrycaiko,       main effect of         (0.69)
1997            intervention
                on self-esteem
                (or the physical
                appearance
                sub-scale).

Burgess,        Means show             1.12
Grogan, &       attractiveness         (0.78)
Burwitz,        was marginally
2006            higher in
                the aerobic
                condition, and
                disparagement,
                feeling
                fat and were
                marginally
                lower when
                compared to
                the swimming
                group. No
                significant
                differences in
                pre-to-mid,
                mid-to-post
                or pre-to-post
                BMI scores
                for both
                groups.

Daley &         The experimental       0.72
Buchanan,       group improved         (0.67)
1999            physical selfworth,
                sports competence,
                strength competence,
                and body
                attractiveness
                over time when
                compared to
                the comparison
                group.

Gehrman,        Mean scores            1 (0.77)
Hovell,         for females
Sallis, &       on body
Keating,        dissatisfaction
2006            followed the same
                pattern for the
                intervention
                group when
                compared to
                the control
                group.

Lindwall        No significant         1.16
& Lindgren,     difference             (0.83)
2005            in physical
                self-perception
                observed
                between
                groups,
                both groups
                improved
                significantly
                over time.
                Improvement
                in social
                physique anxiety
                scale over
                time for
                intervention
                group compared
                to control group.

Waldron,        No change in           0.72
2007            self-perception        (0.57)
                subscales
                observed at
                follow up.
                Interviews
                indicated that
                following
                intervention,
                the girls
                expressed
                greater
                self-acceptance,
                improved
                fitness, and
                enhanced
                knowledge of
                body care.

Notes.

(a) Where mean age is not reported, age range is provided.

(b) Mean age is reported from the whole sample, separate data were not
available fro females.

(c) Group sizes NR for each condition.

Table 2
Quality review scores

Item   Indicator of Quality                               Mean   (SD)

13     Quality of measures                                1.67   0.52
1      Hypothesis                                         1.67   0.52
3      Predictor and outcome measures                     1.50   0.84
2      Study design                                       1.33   0.52
7      Describing those lost to follow up                 1.20   0.84
6      Sample descriptions                                1.17   0.98
14     Methods of analysis                                1.17   0.41
5      Selection is unbiased                              1.00   0.63
19     Description of findings                            1.00   0.00
10     Description of trial                               0.83   0.41
8      Sample size                                        0.83   0.41
20     Conclusions                                        0.67   0.52
15     Inclusion of effect sizes                          0.50   0.55
11     Concealment of randomization from participants     0.67   1.03
4      Follow up                                          0.33   0.52
9      Randomization                                      0.50   0.84
16     Control for confounding variables                  0.17   0.41
17     Concealment of randomization from investigators    0.00   0.00

Note. Item 18 referred to adjustments in analysis made
for the differences in timing of follow up data collection.
Since none of the studies collected follow up data, this
item has been omitted here. Item 12 was omitted as it
was not relevant to the included studies.
COPYRIGHT 2018 University of South Alabama
No portion of this article can be reproduced without the express written permission from the copyright holder.
Copyright 2018 Gale, Cengage Learning. All rights reserved.

联系我们|关于我们|网站声明
国家哲学社会科学文献中心版权所有