Perfectionism and Perceptions of Social Loafing in Competitive Youth Soccer.
Vaartstra, Matthew ; Dunn, John G.H. ; Dunn, Janice Causgrove 等
Perfectionism and Perceptions of Social Loafing in Competitive Youth Soccer.
Social loafing refers to the reduction in effort that occurs when
an individual works collectively on a task compared to when working
alone on a task (Karau & Williams, 1993; Latane, Williams, &
Harkins, 1979). This reduction in individual effort has been shown to
result in reduced (or sub-optimal) performance/productivity levels in a
wide variety of achievement settings including (but not limited to)
business/workplace teams, classroom tasks in academe, and
physical/athletic performance tasks (for reviews see Hardy, 1990; Karau
& Williams, 1993; Simms & Nichols, 2014). Understanding the
impact of social loafing upon performance, the reasons why social
loafing occurs, and methods by which social loafing can be reduced is
particularly relevant in the context of interactive team sports (e.g.,
soccer, hockey, or basketball) where athletes trust and rely upon
teammates to give maximum effort towards the achievement of team goals
(both in training and competition). Even a small amount of social
loafing in interactive team sports, especially at elite levels of
competition, has the potential to jeopardize a team's chances of
attaining competitive success (see Hardy, 1990; Hoigaard, Fuglestad,
Peters, De Cuyper, De Backer, & Boen, 2010).
Numerous studies have examined social-loafing effects in physical
(athletic) performance tasks. The majority of these studies have been
conducted in coactive team/group contexts where the overall performance
of teams is dependent upon the summation of individual performances (but
where no specific interaction between team members is necessary: e.g., a
swimming relay race). Coactive activities that have been used to study
social-loafing effects in physical (athletic) performance tasks have
included running (Swain, 1996), rowing (Anshel, 1995; Hardy & Crace,
1991), swimming (Williams, Nida, Baca, & Latane, 1989), and cycling
(Haugen, Reinboth, Hetlelid, Peters, & Hoigaard, 2016). In these
studies, effort/performance is typically assessed through objective
measures of time/speed, distance travelled, and/or power output. The
results of these studies have revealed drops in individual performance
levels (e.g., reduced speed, reduced distance travelled, reduced power
output) when the identifiability of each individual participant's
performance was reduced (e.g., participants were informed that their
individual performance was to be subsumed within an overall measure of
group/team performance) in comparison to when the identifiability of
each participant's performance was increased (e.g., participants
were informed that their individual performance was to be directly
recorded and publicly evaluated).
Although the aforementioned studies highlight the debilitating
effects that social loafing can have upon individual- and
team-performance in athletic tasks, social-loafing research in
interactive team-sport settings is scarce. The lack of research in this
area is likely due to the inherent difficulties associated with
objectively measuring individual output (i.e., performance or effort) in
interactive team sports (see Hoigaard et al., 2010). No published
studies have directly measured the impact of social loafing on
effort/performance in interactive team sports, however, researchers have
assessed perceived social loafing (see Hoigaard, Safvenbom, &
Tonnessen, 2006) in these contexts (where higher levels of perceived
social loafing reflect a higher belief that teammates engage in social
loafing). The limited body of research that has examined perceived
social loafing in interactive team sports has shown perceived social
loafing to be positively correlated with self-reported levels of social
loafing among elite female team-handball players (Iloigaard et al.,
2010), negatively correlated with perceptions of task- and
social-cohesion among male youth soccer teams (Hoigaard, Safvenbom, et
al., 2006), and negatively correlated with team identification (i.e.,
the degree to which athletes personally and/or socially identify with
the achievements of their team) among female basketball, soccer, and
volleyball players (De Backer, Boen, De Cuyper, Hoigaard, & Vande
Broek, 2014).
Many reasons have been proposed to explain why people engage in
social-loafing behaviors in sport- (see Gilson, 2014; Hardy, 1990) and
non-sport settings (see Karau & Williams, 1993; Simms & Nichols,
2014). A detailed overview of these reasons is beyond the scope of this
paper, nevertheless, some of the more frequently cited reasons include
(a) a desire to avoid personal blame when group failure occurs (i.e.,
people 'hide in the crowd' by reducing personal
identifiability within the group), (b) a belief that other people are
not working hard so the individual will not be the 'sucker'
who carries the workload for the rest of the team/group, and (c) a
desire to save energy for situations when maximal individual benefit
(e.g., praise or social recognition) can be obtained (see Hardy, 1990;
Karau & Williams, 1993).
Although a number of situational conditions that influence the
degree to which social loafing occurs in various performance settings
have been identified in the literature--with increases in 'public
evaluation' and 'individual identifiability' being among
the key deterrents of social loafing (see Williams, Harkins, &
Latane, 1981)--personality or dispositional characteristics of
performers have also been linked to social loafing. For example, Tan and
Tan (2008) reported that conscientiousness was negatively correlated
with social loafing in a sample of undergraduate students, and Smrt and
Karau (2011) also found that protestant work ethic--defined as "an
orientation toward work which emphasizes dedication to hard work,
deferment of immediate rewards, conservation of resources, the saving of
surplus wealth, and the avoidance of idleness and waste in any
form" (Smrt & Karau, 2011, p. 267)--was negatively correlated
with social loafing (as measured by the frequency of ideas generated in
an academic task) among undergraduate students.
Dispositional characteristics of performers have also been linked
to social loafing in athletic tasks. For example, results of a study
conducted by Swain (1996) with male high school students (who were
exposed to different running relay tasks in a gymnasium) revealed that
individuals who had high ego-orientation combined with low
task-orientation (see Nicholls, 1989) socially loafed (i.e., ran slower)
in a low-identifiability situation, whereas individuals who had high
task-orientation combined with low ego-orientation did not socially loaf
(i.e., did not run slower) in the same low-identifiability condition. In
a more recent study involving male high school students (who
participated in competitive or recreational sports), individuals with
higher levels of mental toughness did not engage in social loafing in a
cycling time-trial task (i.e., where performance/effort was measured by
distance travelled on a cycling ergometer) whereas athletes with lower
levels of mental toughness did engage in social loafing on the same task
(see Haugen et al., 2016). Haugen et al. (2016) concluded that their
results "confirm the importance of taking individual-difference
characteristics into account when working with social loafing [in
athletes]" (p. 309). One such characteristic that may play a role
in the social loafing process in sport is perfectionism (Hoigaardet al.,
2010).
Perfectionism in sport can be conceptualized as a domain-specific
multidimensional achievement-motivation disposition that reflects the
degree to which individuals (a) strive for the attainment of very high
performance standards (in sport) and (b) are concerned about failing to
meet these high standards (Dunn et al., 2016; Stoeber, 2011). Although
many sub-dimensions (or facets) of perfectionism in sport have been
proposed in the literature (for a recent review see Stoeber &
Madigan, 2016), when analyzed together with multivariate techniques such
as factor analysis, two higher-order dimensions--labelled
perfectionistic strivings ma perfectionistic concerns--typically emerge
(see Dunn et al., 2016; Stoeber & Madigan, 2016). Perfectionistic
strivings in sport reflect "aspects of perfectionism associated
with a self-oriented striving for perfection and the setting of very
high personal performance standards" (Gotwals, Stoeber, Dunn, &
Stoll, 2012, p. 264). In contrast, perfectionistic concerns in sport
reflect "aspects of perfectionism associated with concerns over
making mistakes, fear of negative social evaluation, feelings of
discrepancy between one's expectations and performance, and
negative reactions to imperfection" (Gotwals et al., 2012, p. 264).
As noted by Stoeber and Madigan (2016), "the differentiation
of perfectionistic strivings and perfectionistic concerns is central to
understanding multidimensional perfectionism" (p. 32) because
perfectionistic concerns are typically associated with maladaptive,
dysfunctional, or unhealthy processes and outcomes in sport, whereas
perfectionistic strivings are more often associated with adaptive,
functional, or healthy processes and outcomes in sport (particularly
when perfectionistic concerns are low or when the overlap with
perfectionistic concerns is controlled: see Gotwals et al., 2012;
Jowett, Mallinson, & Hill, 2016). For example, athlete profiles of
perfectionism involving the combination of high perfection-istic
strivings with low perfectionistic concerns have been positively
associated with task orientation (Dunn, Causgrove Dunn, & Syrotuik,
2002), healthy attitudinal body image (Dunn, Craft, Causgrove Dunn,
& Gotwals, 2011), and confidence/optimism (Lizmore, Dunn, &
Causgrove Dunn, 2016) in sport. In contrast, athlete profiles of
perfectionism with the combination of high perfectionistic strivings
with high perfectionistic concerns have been positively associated with
ego orientation (Dunn et al., 2002) and negatively associated with
healthy attitudinal body image (Dunn et al., 2011) and
confidence/optimism (Lizmore et al., 2016) in sport.
Given that different profiles of perfectionism show different
relationships with a variety of adaptive/functional and
maladaptive/dysfunctional processes and outcomes in sport, it seems
important to determine if perfectionism in sport may play a role in the
social loafing process for athletes (see H0igaard et al., 2010). As
noted previously, athletes with high perfectionistic strivings are
typically driven by a desire to achieve very high standards of personal
performance in sport. Consequently, engaging in social loafing would be
considered antithetical to such a motivational orientation; giving less
effort in training or competition (when maximal effort is expected or
required) would mean that the highest personal performance standards
were not being met. It is conceivable that athletes in interactive team
sports who have high perfectionistic strivings might also be inclined to
expect maximal effort from teammates because teammate
effort/contribution is often necessary for the high
perfectionistic-strivings athlete to achieve his/her own personal
performance goals. For example, a soccer player who is striving to make
the maximum contribution to his/her team cannot do so if teammates do
not work hard to win the ball or make themselves available to receive
the ball in order for the 'desired' play to occur.
A key aspect of heightened perfectionistic concerns in sport is the
fear of failure (Gotwals et al., 2012; Stoeber, 2011). Perfectionism
theorists have long suggested that the 'avoidance of failure'
may be a stronger motivator than the 'desire to succeed' for
individuals who have high perfectionistic concerns (see Blatt, 1995).
Consequently, 'hiding in the crowd' to avoid the possibility
of receiving personal blame for individual/team failure (see Karau &
Williams, 1993) may be a particularly attractive social-loafing strategy
for interactive team-sport athletes who are high in perfectionistic
concerns. In addition, given that individuals with heightened
perfectionistic concerns often have high levels of contingent self-worth
(see McArdle, 2010)--where self-worth is based upon the attainment of
high performance standards and the reception of positive social
evaluation from others (see Stoeber & Otto, 2006)--it is conceivable
that individuals with high perfectionistic concerns may elect to save
their effort for times when they believe that maximum social benefit
(e.g., social recognition or praise) can be attained. By engaging in
'effort management' (see Hardy, 1990), athletes with high
perfectionistic concerns may adopt a more narcissistic, self-focussed,
or self-promotional view of sport, which has been shown to correspond
with heightened social loafing (i.e., reduced effort), particularly in
conditions of low identifiability (see Woodman, Roberts, Hardy, Callow,
& Rogers, 2011).
Given the aforementioned theorized links between perfectionistic
strivings, perfectionistic concerns, and social loafing, the overarching
purpose of this study was to explore the relationships between
athletes' perfectionist orientations and perceptions of social
loafing in sport. More specifically, the study sought to explore
relationships between perfectionism (as represented by sub-dimensions of
perfectionistic strivings and perfectionistic concerns) and perceived
social loafing in a sample of competitive youth soccer players. Youth
soccer was chosen because it has been shown to provide an appropriate
(and useful) context in which perceived social loafing in athletes can
be examined (see Hoigaard, SafVenbom, et al., 2006; Hoigaard, Tofteland,
& Ommundsen, 2006), and because the structure of soccer (i.e., many
players spread across a relatively large playing area) has the potential
to provide conditions of low identifiability where social loafing can
occur. No previous research has examined links between domain-specific
perfectionism and perceived social loafing in interactive team sports.
Method
Participants
Participants were 216 (162 female) youth soccer players (from 16
teams) who competed in the top (Tier I/II) competitive levels of
under-16 age-group soccer in a Canadian province. (1) Athletes ranged in
age from 13 to 16 years (Mage = 15.25, SD = 0.63) and had an average of
6.31 years (SD = 2.54) playing experience in competitive club soccer.
The ethnic/ racial background of the participants consisted of 170
White, 9 Black, 4 Middle-Eastern, 4 Hispanic, 4 Asian, 4 First Nations,
and 21 "other".
Instruments
Participants completed four self-report inventories: (1) a
demographic questionnaire, (2) the Sport-Multidimensional Perfectionism
Scale-2 (Sport-MPS-2; Gotwals & Dunn, 2009), (3) the Perceived
Social Loafing Questionnaire (PSLQ: Hoigaard, Safvenbom et al., 2006),
and (4) a newly developed instrument named, the Social Loafing
Acceptability
Questionnaire (SLAQ).
Sport-Multidimensional Perfectionism Scale-2. The Sport-MPS-2
(Gotwals & Dunn, 2009) is a domain-specific measure of perfectionism
in sport that was originally modelled around the conceptualization of
perfectionism contained within Frost, Marten, Lahart, and
Rosenblate's (1990) Multidimensional Perfectionism Scale. The
Sport-MPS-2 contains 42 items that are divided into six subscales. The
Personal Standards (PS: 7 items) and Organization (ORG: 6 items)
subscales measure sub-dimensions of perfectionistic strivings, whereas
the Concern Over Mistakes (COM: 8 items), Perceived Parental Pressure
(PPP: 9 items), Perceived Coach Pressure (PCP: 6 items) and Doubts About
Actions (DAA: 6 items) subscales measure sub-dimensions of
perfectionistic concerns (see Dunn et al., 2016; Gotwals & Dunn,
2009). Athletes rate items on a 5-point scale (1 = strongly disagree-, 5
= strongly agree) with higher scores reflecting higher levels of
perfectionism. Extensive validity and reliability evidence supporting
the use of Sport-MPS-2 subscales as measures of perfectionism in sport
has been reported in the literature (see Dunn, Causgrove Dunn, Gotwals,
Vallance, Craft, & Syrotuik, 2006; Dunn et al., 2016; Gotwals &
Dunn, 2009; Gotwals, Dunn, Causgrove Dunn, & Gamache, 2010).
Perceived Social Loafing Questionnaire. The PSLQ is a 5-item
unidimensional instrument that measures athletes' perceptions of
the social-loafing tendencies of team members (see Hoigaard et al.,
2010; Hoigaard & Ommundsen, 2007; Hoigaard, SafVenbom, et al.,
2006). Respondents rate the extent to which they feel that teammates
engage in social loafing (e.g., "Members of my team try to
'hide behind others 'so that they don't need to try as
hard as they could') on a 5-point rating scale ranging from 1
(strongly disagree) to 5 (strongly agree). Higher scores indicate higher
perceptions of social loafing among team members. Principal components
analysis conducted upon PSLQ responses from 118 junior league (Mage =
17.7 years; SD = .98) soccer players resulted in a single-factor
solution (see Hoigaard, SafVenbom, et al., 2006). Internal consistency
levels (a) for the PSLQ have been consistently [greater than or equal
to] .68 (see De Backer et al., 2015; Hoigaard et al., 2010; Hoigaard
& Ommundsen, 2007; Hoigaard, Safvenbom, et al., 2006).
Social Loafing Acceptability Questionnaire. We were concerned that
asking athletes to respond to questions directly about their own
social-loafing tendencies in soccer may result in a floor effect for
athlete responses and reduce response variability (cf. De Backer et al.,
2015) as well as increase the potential that data may be adversely
influenced by social desirability response bias (i.e., athletes may be
unwilling to admit to using social-loafing strategies knowing that such
behaviors would typically undermine performance and be frowned upon by
coaches and teammates: see Jones, Hoigaard, & Peters, 2014).
Consequently, we elected to create an indirect measure of social loafing
that assessed the extent to which athletes viewed the social-loafing
behaviors of other athletes as being acceptable.
The SLAQ was designed with the intention of depicting
social-loafing behaviors that corresponded with two underlying reasons
why athletes might socially loaf in sport: namely, to avoid blame (i.e.,
Blame Avoidance) and to save effort for when it most benefited the
athlete (i.e., Effort Management', see Hardy, 1990; Karau &
Williams, 1993). Blame avoidance was operationally defined as a
deliberate decrease in effort to avoid or reduce blame for group- or
personal-failure. Effort management was operationally defined as a
decrease in individual effort stemming from the desire to save effort
for a future task when the individual believes that he/she will receive
maximum social benefit (e.g., recognition or praise) for a successful
performance (at the group or individual level).
Five items/scenarios were developed around blame avoidance and five
were developed around effort management. Respondents rated the extent to
which they viewed fictitious athletes' social-loafing behaviors in
training contexts as being acceptable. Each item/scenario was rated on a
7-point scale (1 = never acceptable; 7 = always acceptable) such that
higher scores reflected higher levels of acceptance (or approval)
towards other athletes' use of social-loafing behaviors in
training. Female participants responded to scenarios depicting other
female soccer players, whereas male participants responded to identical
scenarios depicting male soccer players (e.g., "During scrimmage
situations, Joe/Anna saves giving maximal effort for when he/she knows
the coach is specifically watching his/her performance").
Procedure
Phase 1: SLAQ scenario development. We adopted similar item
construction/ evaluation procedures employed by Dunn, Bouffard, and
Rogers (1999) for assessing the content relevance of the newly
constructed items, whereby a panel of expert judges (n = 13) was asked
to assess the content relevance of each item/scenario prior to its
inclusion in the SLAQ. This phase of the test construction process was
completed before the SLAQ was administered to athletes. Each judge had a
PhD in the social sciences, had published in peer-reviewed sport--or
exercise-psychology journals, and held a full-time academic appointment
in a physical education, kinesiology, or health sciences faculty at a
North American or European university.
Judges were sent a questionnaire package by email to assess the
content-relevance of the items (see Dunn et al., 1999). The package
contained three sections. Part A contained a short demographic
questionnaire. Part B contained operational definitions for social
loafing, blame avoidance, and effort management, as well as a list
containing the 10 items/scenarios that were to be included in the SLAQ.
Part C asked judges to rate the degree of fit (i.e., match) between each
of the 10 scenarios and the two theoretical social-loafing
'constructs' the inventory was being designed to measure
(i.e., blame avoidance and effort management). Ratings were made on a
5-point scale ranging from 1 (poor fit) to 5 (excellent fit). The
intended construct that each scenario was designed to measure was not
revealed to the judges, thereby ensuring that judges' ratings were
not biased by the anticipated matches that the test developer was hoping
to find (see Dunn et al., 1999).
Phase 2: Athlete data collection. Data collection sessions were
independently scheduled and conducted by the principal investigator at a
time/place that was convenient for each team (typically in a dressing
room after a team practice) during the competitive season. Prior to
completing the inventories, athletes were reminded that their
participation was voluntary, their participation had no bearing on
playing time, their individual information would remain anonymous and
confidential, and their parents/coaches would be required to leave the
room during the completion of the inventories. The presentation order of
the Sport-MPS-2, PSLQ, and SLAQ was counterbalanced across participants
to minimize any potential presentation order effects. Institutional
research ethics clearance was obtained prior to the study, with all data
collection being conducted in accordance with the ethical guidelines for
human research set forth by the American Psychological Association.
Results
Phase 1
Aiken's (1985) content validity coefficient (V) was computed
to assess the content-relevance ratings provided by the judges for each
SLAQ item/scenario on the domain/ construct that the scenario was
intended to measure. Aiken's Fis bounded by values ranging from 0
to 1 (where a value of 0 indicates that all judges gave the item the
lowest possible rating on the intended construct and a value of 1
indicates that all judges gave the item the highest possible rating on
the intended construct). A statistically significant V coefficient
indicates that the judges (as a group) rated the item as being an
appropriate (i.e., relevant) measure of the domain/construct it was
intended to measure. The F coefficients for each item on the
keyed/intended domain are reported in Table 1 (along with abbreviated
item descriptions). Each item had a statistically significant V(ps <
.05) with the exception of Item 6. However, given that the mean rating
for Item 6 (M = 3.46) indicated that the judges believed that the
scenario provided a "good fit" with blame avoidance, the item
was retained for inclusion in the SLAQ.
Phase 2
Of the 216 questionnaire packets that were completed by
participants, there were only 18 missing data points (i.e., 0.15%
missing data). Missing data were replaced using a mean item score
computed from the remaining items in the matching subscale (to which the
missing item belonged) for each individual (see Graham, Cumsille, &
Elek-Fisk, 2003).
Pre-screening for gender differences. It was considered desirable
to combine female and male responses into a single data set to maximize
the statistical power of the data analyses. To ensure the
appropriateness of collapsing the data into a single combined-gender
data set, three separate MANOVAs were conducted for the Sport-MPS-2,
PSLQ, and SLAQ responses. In each analysis, gender was entered as the
independent variable and the item-sets for the three instruments--the
Sport-MPS-2 (42 items), PSLQ (5 items), and SLAQ (10 items)--were
entered as the dependent variables. Any significant multivariate test
statistic was followed up by univariate F-tests (with Bonferroni
corrections) on the dependent variables.
Each MANOVA was preceded by a Box's M test to examine the
homogeneity of the covariance matrices for each instrument across
gender. In accordance with the recommendations of Tabachnick and Fidell
(1996), a significant Box's M statistic is indicated when p <
.001. All three Box's M tests were non-significant, indicating that
the covariance matrices for each instrument (at the item level) across
gender were homogeneous: Sport-MPS-2, Box's M = 1375.241, F (903,
31653.868) = 1.039, p = .20; PSLQ, Box's M = 12.145, F (15,
40152.292) = .780, p = .70; SLAQ, Box's M = 88.587, F(55,
33603.945) = 1.494, p < .05. The overall multivariate tests for the
MANOVAs conducted on the PSLQ (Wilks' A = .970, F [5, 210] = 1.309,
p = .26) and SLAQ (Wilks' [LAMBDA] = .918, F [10, 205] = 1.821, p =
.06) were not significant. In contrast, the multivariate test to
identify gender differences among Sport-MPS-2 items was statistically
significant (Wilks' [LAMBDA] = 0.691, F [42, 173] = 1.841, p <
.01), however, follow-up univariate F-tests for all 42 items failed to
reveal any significant differences (with corresponding effect sizes
being small [all partial [[eta].sup.2] [less than or equal to] .04]). In
light of these results, it was deemed appropriate to combine female and
male responses into a single data set (N = 216) for all remaining
analyses.
Psychometric evaluation of the SLAQ. Given that the SLAQ is a newly
constructed instrument, it was necessary to examine the
instrument's latent structure prior to creating composite subscale
scores. Therefore, the matrix of inter-item correlations for the SLAQ
was subjected to an exploratory Principal Axes factor analysis. The
number of factors to be retained was based upon parallel analysis and
scree plot results as well as factor interpretability (Gorsuch, 2003).
Contrary to our expectations, parallel analysis and scree plot results
both indicated the retention of a single-factor (with factor loadings
ranging from .35 to .60). A unidimensional structure to the SLAQ was
therefore adopted for the remainder of the study. Higher composite SLAQ
scores (based on the summation of scores for all 10 items) indicated
higher perceived acceptability for social-loafing behaviors in other
soccer players in training/practice situations.
Descriptive statistics and internal consistencies. The means,
standard deviations, and internal consistencies for all measures are
presented in Table 2. The internal consistency levels of all
instruments/subscales were deemed acceptable (i.e., all [alpha]s
[greater than or equal to] .73).
Relationships between perfectionism and perceptions of social
loafing. The purpose of this study was to determine if perfectionist
orientations were related to perceptions of social loafing in youth
soccer players. To this end, bivariate correlations (r) were calculated
between the six Sport-MPS-2 subscales and the two social-loafing
measures (i.e., PSLQ and SLAQ; see Table 2). Significant positive
correlations were found between two perfectionism subscales (i.e.,
personal standards and doubts about actions) and PSLQ scores. In other
words, as personal standards and doubts about actions increased, so too
did athletes' tendency to perceive that teammates engaged in social
loafing. In contrast, significant negative correlations were found
between two perfectionism subscales (i.e., personal standards and
organization) and perceptions of social-loafing acceptability (SLAQ). In
other words, as personal standards and organization increased, the
tendency of athletes to accept other players' social loafing in
soccer training decreased. A significant positive correlation was also
found between doubts about actions and perceptions of social-loafing
acceptability (SLAQ), indicating that increases in athletes' doubts
about actions were associated with increased acceptance of others'
social loafing in training.
Multivariate relationships between perfectionism and perceived
social loafing. The bivariate correlations provide insight into the
independent relationships between sub-dimensions of perfectionism in
sport and athletes' perceptions of social loafing. However, a more
complete understanding of how multidimensional perfectionism is related
to perceptions of social loafing may be achieved by considering the
scores across all Sport-MPS-2 subscales simultaneously. This is
important because, as discussed previously, different patterns or
combinations of scores across various sub-dimensions of perfectionistic
strivings and perfectionistic concerns have shown markedly different
relationships with cognitive, affective, and behavioral responses of
athletes in sport (e.g., Dunn et al., 2002; Lizmore et al., 2016). To
this end, we used canonical correlation analysis (CCA) to investigate
the multivariate relationship between perfectionism and athletes'
perceptions of social loafing in soccer. Not only is CCA viewed as the
"most appropriate and powerful multivariate technique" (Hair,
Anderson, Tatham, & Black, 1998, p. 444) in situations where there
are multiple independent and dependent variables (as is the case in this
study), but CCA has an added benefit (over many other multivariate
techniques) because it enables the researcher to consider scores across
all variables simultaneously (rather than separately), thereby honoring
"the ecological reality that in nature all the variables can
interact with each other [at the same time]" (Thompson, 2005, p.
193). The two social-loafing scales (i.e., the PSLQ and SLAQ) were
entered as the criterion set, and the six Sport-MPS-2 subscales were
entered as the predictor set in the analysis.
Tabachnick and Fidell (1996) caution that univariate and
multivariate outliers can "have undue impact upon canonical
analysis" (p. 199). We therefore employed procedures described by
Tabachnick and Fidell (pp. 66-68) to screen for univariate outliers by
identifying athletes who had a standardized z-score [greater than or
equal to] [absolute value of 3.30] (p < .001) on any variable. One
univariate outlier was identified (on the SLAQ) and removed from the
data set. Following the removal of the outlier, Mahalanobis distances
were computed to screen for the presence of multivariate outliers (see
Tabachnick & Fidell, 1996, p. 94). No multivariate outliers were
detected (all Mahalanobis distances < 19.47 ([chi square]
[[8].sub.critical] = 27.87, p < .001) therefore the CCA was conducted
upon data provided by 215 athletes across eight variables. The
participant-to-variable ratio (27:1) exceeded the minimum ratio (10:1)
recommended for CCA by Tabachnick and Fidell (1996, p. 198).
The full model (across both functions) was significant (Wilks'
[LAMBDA] = .802, F [12, 414] = 4.03, p < .001) and explained 19.8% of
the variance in the data. Two significant canonical functions were
extracted, with both canonical correlations ([R.sub.C1] = .35;
[R.sub.C2] = .30,ps < .005) meeting the minimum criterion value (.30)
that is typically deemed suitable for interpretation purposes (see Hair
et al., 1998; Tabachnick & Fidell, 1996). Table 3 contains the
canonical loadings (structure coefficients) for both functions. Only
variables with loadings [greater than or equal to] [absolute value of
.30] on their respective canonical variates were interpreted (see Hair
et al., 1998, Tabachnick & Fidell, 1996).
In the first canonical function, perceived social loafing in
teammates (PSLQ) and social-loafing acceptability (SLAQ) had moderate to
strong positive loadings on the social-loafing variate. Personal
standards, doubts about actions, and perceived parental pressure had
moderate to strong positive loadings on the perfectionism variate. This
combination of a positive loading for personal standards (i.e., a
sub-dimension of perfectionistic strivings) and positive loadings for
doubts about actions and perceived parental pressure (i.e.,
sub-dimensions of perfectionistic concerns) reflects characteristics
associated with a maladaptive or unhealthy profile of perfectionism in
sport (see Dunn et al., 2002; Gotwals, 2016). The positive correlation
between the two variates ([R.sub.C1] = .35) indicates that as certain
features of maladaptive/unhealthy perfectionism increase, there is a
corresponding increase in athletes' tendencies to (a) believe that
teammates engage in social loafing, and (b) be more accepting of other
athletes' social-loafing behaviors in training/practice contexts.
In the second canonical fonction, perceived social loafing in
teammates (PSLQ) had a moderate positive loading, and social-loafing
acceptability (SLAQ) had a strong negative loading on the social-loafing
variate. Personal standards and organization had moderate to strong
positive loadings on the perfectionism variate, whereas perceived
parental pressure and doubts about actions had moderate negative
loadings on the perfectionism variate. This combination of positive
loadings for personal standards and organization (i.e., sub-dimensions
of perfectionistic strivings) and negative loadings for doubts about
actions and perceived parental pressure (i.e., sub-dimesnions of
perfectioistic concerns) reflects characteristics associated with an
adaptive/healthy profile perfectionism in sport (see Dunn et al., 2002;
Gotwals, 2016; Lizmore et al., 2016). The positive correlation between
the two variates ([R.sub.c2] = .30) indicates that as certain features
of adaptive/healthy perfectionism increase, there is a corresponding
increase in athletes' tendencies to (a) believe that teammates
engage in social loafing, and (b) be less accepting of other
athletes' social-loafing behaviors in training contexts.
Discussion
The purpose of this study was to examine relationships between
athletes' perfectionist orientations and perceptions of social
loafing in youth soccer. At the bivariate level (see Table 2), three
perfectionism subscales (i.e., personal standards, organization, and
doubts about actions) were significantly correlated with perceived
social loafing in teammates (as measured by the PSLQ) and/or acceptance
of social loafing (as measured by the SLAQ). When multivariate
relationships were examined, canonical correlation results (see Table 3)
indicated that patterns of Sport-MPS-2 subscale scores that resembled
adaptive/ healthy and maladaptive/unhealthy profiles of perfectionism
(see Gotwals, 2016) showed different relationships with athletes'
perceptions of social loafing in soccer. Collectively, the bivariate and
canonical correlation results support the need to consider dispositional
(i.e., individual-difference) characteristics in the study of social
loafing in group/team settings (see Haugen et al., 2016; Smrt &
Karau, 2011; Swain, 1996; Tan & Tan, 2008), and indicate that
perfectionism may play a role in the social-loafing process in sport
(see Hoigaard et al., 2010).
At the bivariate level, the personal standards subscale of the
Sport-MPS-2 was positively correlated with PSLQ scores, indicating that
as athletes set higher standards of personal performance in sport they
were more inclined to perceive that teammates engaged in social loafing.
It is possible that higher personal standards predispose athletes to
perceive more social-loafing behaviors in teammates due to the belief
that their teammates strive for lower performance standards. We make
this conjecture on the basis that three items contained within the
personal standards subscale of the Sport-MPS-2 ask respondents to make
comparisons between their own personal standards of performance in sport
and the performance standards adopted by other athletes (i.e., "I
think I expect higher performance and greater results in my daily
sport-training than most players"; "I feel that other players
generally accept lower standards for themselves in sport than I
do"; and "I set higher achievement goals than most athletes
who play my sport"). It therefore seems reasonable to suggest that
if athletes with higher scores on the personal standards subscale feel
that they have higher performance standards than other athletes in their
sport, they may be more likely to interpret certain teammate actions
(where effort is apparently lacking) as being indicative of social
loafing (as reflected by higher PSLQ scores). Assessing the underlying
reasons why athletes with higher personal standards perceive higher
social loafing in their teammates would appear to be a valuable
direction for future research.
The personal standards and organization subscales of the
Sport-MPS-2 were both negatively correlated with the SLAQ at the
bivariate level (see Table 2), indicating that as athletes set higher
standards of personal performance and increase their use of pre- and
within-game routines/plans to achieve desired performance levels, there
was a corresponding decrease in athletes' acceptance of social
loafing in training/practice situations. Given that the personal
standards and organization subscales of the Sport-MPS-2 measure aspects
of perfectionistic strivings in sport (Dunn et al., 2016), the negative
correlations with the SLAQ appear to make theoretical sense. Heightened
perfectionistic strivings reflect a heightened tendency of athletes to
be motivated to achieve higher standards of personal performance in
sport and to pursue these standards in an organized/planned manner (see
Gotwals et al., 2010; Lizmore et al., 2016). Consequently, athletes with
heightened perfectionistic strivings would be expected to view social
loafing (in themselves or others) as being counter-productive to their
own personal quest to achieve high (or perfect) performance in sport and
therefore adopt less accepting views of such behaviors in others. This
theoretical explanation is in line with previous research conducted in
non-sport settings where significant negative relationships have been
identified between other achievement-striving dispositions (i.e.,
conscientiousness and protestant work ethic) and social loafing (see
Smrt & Karau 2011 * Tan & Tan, 2008).
As seen in Table 2, the doubts about actions (DAA) subscale of the
Sport-MPS-2 was positively correlated with PSLQ and SLAQ scores. These
positive correlations indicate that as athletes' experience higher
doubts about the adequacy of their preparations for competition (see
Gotwals & Dunn, 2009), there is an increased tendency to believe
that teammates are engaging in social loafing (as represented by higher
PSLQ scores) and to be more accepting of other athletes' social
loafing in training (as represented by higher SLAQ scores). Doubts about
actions (DAA) is a sub-dimension of perfectionistic concerns (see Dunn
et al., 2016; Gotwals et al., 2010) and higher DAA scores typically
correspond with maladaptive or dysfunctional correlates in sport (see
Gotwals et al., 2012; Lizmore et al., 2016; Stoeber, 2011). This appears
to be the case in this current study where higher DAA scores
corresponded with a greater acceptance of other athletes'
social-loafing behaviors in training. If athletes are more accepting of
others' social loafing in training, they may be more inclined
themselves to engage in social-loafing behaviors (in comparison to
athletes who are less accepting [or more disapproving] of social loafing
behaviors in the same context), thereby creating a vulnerability factor
to social loafing in sport.
The bivariate correlations provide insight into the independent
relationships between different aspects of perfectionism and perceptions
of social loafing. However, the results of the canonical correlation
analysis provide an opportunity to further examine relationships between
perfectionism and perceptions of social loafing by considering scores
across all sub-dimensions of perfectionistic strivings and
perfectionistic concerns simultaneously (see Dunn et al., 2002, 2011;
Gotwals, 2016; Gotwals et al., 2010). As noted previously, this approach
has the potential benefit of providing a more ecologically valid
assessment of how perfectionism may operate within athletes because all
aspects of athletes' perfection-istic strivings and perfectionistic
concerns coexist and presumably operate simultaneously in real-world
settings.
A combination of heightened perfectionistic strivings with
heightened perfectionistic concerns--as depicted in the first canonical
function contained in Table 3--is typically associated with maladaptive
functioning in sport (see Gotwals, 2016; Lizmore et al., 2016), whereas
a combination of heightened perfectionistic strivings with lower
perfectionistic concerns--as depicted in the second canonical
function--is typically associated with adaptive functioning in sport
(see Gotwals, 2016; Lizmore et al., 2016). Although the patterns of
canonical loadings on the perfectionism variates appear to support the
distinction between maladaptive and adaptive profiles of perfectionism
(see Lo & Abbott, 2013), it is the associations between the
perfectionism variates and the social-loafing variates that are of
particular interest.
The perfectionism variate in the first canonical function that
contained characteristics resembling maladaptive perfectionism (i.e.,
positive loadings for PS [a sub-dimension of perfectionistic strivings]
and positive loadings for DAA and PPP [sub-dimensions of perfectionistic
concerns]) was positively correlated ([R.sub.C1] = .35) with the
social-loafing variate that contained moderate to high positive loadings
for both the PSLQ and SLAQ. In other words, as maladaptive
perfectionistic tendencies increased, so too did the tendency of
athletes to (a) perceive higher levels of social loafing in teammates,
and (b) adopt more accepting views of social loafing in training
situations. In contrast, the perfectionism variate in the second
canonical function that contained characteristics resembling adaptive
perfectionism (i.e., positive loadings for PS and ORG [perfectionistic
strivings] and negative loadings for PPP and DAA [perfectionistic
concerns]) was positively correlated ([R.sub.C1] = .30) with the
social-loafing variate that contained a moderate positive loading for
the PSLQ but a strong negative loading for the SLAQ. These results
support findings of previous research illustrating that different
patterns (or combinations) of athletes' perfectionistic strivings
and perfectionistic concerns (when considered simultaneously) can have
unique relationships with various cognitive, affective, and/or
behavioral correlates in sport (e.g., Dunn et al., 2002, 2011; Gotwals
et al., 2010). The results also reinforce that perfectionism--and in
particular, perfectionistic strivings--may play a 'dual role'
in sport (Stoeber, 2011), at times being associated with
adaptive/healthy/functional outcomes or processes and at other times
being associated with maladaptive/unhealthy/dysfunctional outcomes or
processes.
The personal standards subscale of the Sport-MPS-2--considered by
many perfectionism theorists as a central indicator of perfectionistic
strivings in sport (see Stoeber & Madigan, 2016)--had moderate to
strong positive loadings on both perfectionism variates (Table 3). In
other words, heightened PS scores were important in defining both
adaptive and maladaptive profiles of perfectionism. When combined with
heightened perfectionistic concerns (i.e., positive loadings for PPP and
DAA in the first canonical function), heightened PS was associated with
an increased tendency to perceive social loafing in teammates and to be
more accepting of social loafing in training situations. In contrast,
when combined with heightened organization (another aspect of
perfectionistic strivings in sport) and lower perfectionistic concerns
(i.e., negative loadings for PPP and DAA in the second canonical
function), heightened PS was again associated with an increased to
tendency to perceive social loafing in teammates but a less accepting
view of social loafing in training. Thus, while having higher personal
standards may predispose an athlete to perceive that teammates engage in
social loafing--a perception that has the potential to either enhance an
athlete's motivation to work harder [i.e., to make up for the
teammates' lack of effort or to claim the personal benefit/credit
for oneself] or cause frustration/conflict between the athlete and
teammates [i.e., the social loafing is adjudged to be undermining the
athlete's ability to reach his/her own high standards of personal
performance]: see Jones et al. (2014)--the corresponding level of
perfectionistic concerns appears to play an important role in
differentiating the extent to which athletes adopt adaptive/functional
(i.e., less accepting) or maladaptive/ dysfunctional (i.e., more
accepting) views of social loafing in sport. The canonical results also
support the views of Stoeber (2011) who has previously argued that
"perfectionistic strivings may form part of a 'healthy pursuit
of excellence' ... [but] this may only be the case when
perfectionistic strivings are not accompanied by perfectionistic
concerns" (p. 140).
Notwithstanding the valuable insight that the current results
provided with respect to identifying and understanding links between
athletes' perfectionist orientations and perceptions of social
loafing in sport, a number of limitations within the study must be
acknowledged. First, no assessment of the athletes' own
self-reported use of social-loafing behaviors in sport was conducted.
Therefore we cannot make any inferences about athletes' perfection
ism profiles and their own social-loafing tendencies in sport. Second,
all data were obtained from self-report instruments. As such, no
assessment of actual social-loafing behavior (i.e., reduced effort)
among participants was conducted; therefore, no inferences about the
frequency or extent of social loafing in the current sample can be
generated. Third, although we propose that higher levels of perceived
acceptance for social loafing in training may increase the likelihood
that athletes will engage in social loafing themselves, we currently
have no empirical evidence to support this position. Indeed, the
adaptive or maladaptive 'consequences' for athletes who
perceive that teammates engage in social loafing (PSLQ scores) or who
have heightened acceptance levels of social loafing (SLAQ scores) are
largely unknown (and it should be noted that the average perceived
acceptability rating for social loafing was quite low [M= 2.59: see
Table 2], indicating that athletes were typically not accepting of such
social-loafing behaviors in training contexts: cf. Jones et al., 2014).
Fourth, the SLAQ is clearly in the infancy of its development and
despite demonstrating adequate psychometric characteristics, more
research is required to further examine the latent structure of the
instrument (given that we had originally anticipated two dimensions and
expert judges had identified two dimensions, yet factor analytic results
supported the retention of a single-factor [unidimensional] solution).
With respect to the item content of the SLAQ, it should also be
noted that athletes were asked to rate the acceptability of others'
social-loafing behaviors in training/practice settings. Jones et al.
(2014) have suggested that social loafing is less likely to occur in
game/competition situations. Therefore, acceptability ratings may change
had participants been asked to rate the acceptability of social-loafing
behaviors in competition (rather than in training/practice). Clearly
more research is needed to examine the impact of changing the
situational context in which athletes are asked to rate the
acceptability of social-loafing behaviors in sport.
Other limitations (and cautions) extend to the generalizability of
our results, particularly as they relate to sample characteristics. For
example, caution must be taken when generalizing to athletes who
participate at different competitive levels. Research indicates that
perfectionistic strivings and perfectionistic concerns tend to increase
as athletes compete in higher levels of competition (see Rasquinha,
Dunn, & Causgrove Dunn, 2014) whereas social loafing is believed to
become less prevalent at higher levels of competition (see Hoigaard et
al., 2010). Similar generalizability cautions apply to (a) the type of
interactive team sport that is studied (e.g., the sport of basketball
has fewer athletes competing in a smaller-sized playing area which would
likely increase identifiability and decrease social loafing: see Jones
et al., 2014), (b) the age of athletes in the sample (e.g., perceived
parental pressure in sport is believed to play a decreasing role for
athletes as they move from child hood through adolescence and into
adulthood: see Dunn et al., 2016), and (c) the gender composition of the
sample (i.e., research indicates that men may be more prone to social
loafing than women: see Karau & Williams, 1993).
Lastly, it should be acknowledged that other measures of
perfectionism in sport exist (for a recent review see Stoeber &
Madigan, 2016), therefore the extent to which the current results may be
unique to the specific item-content or subscale-composition of the
Sport-MPS-2 remains unknown. For example, the Sport-MPS-2 does not
assess Other Oriented Perfectionism (see Hewitt & Flett, 1991)--an
interpersonal sub-dimension of perfectionism that focusses on the degree
to which individuals expect perfection and/or high standards from other
people--which might impact the perceptions that athletes hold for
teammates who give reduced effort in training or competitive situations.
Despite the aforementioned limitations, the current study provides
the first empirical evidence linking perfectionism and social loafing in
an interactive team-sport setting, and further highlights the need for
researchers to consider dispositional (or individual-difference)
characteristics when studying social loafing in team settings (see
Haugen et al., 2016; Hoigaard et al., 2010; Smrt & Karau, 2011;
Swain, 1996; Tan & Tan, 2008). The results also have potential
implications for coaches and sport psychology practitioners who work
with athletes in interactive team sports. For example, practitioners may
wish to design interventions (or educational mental-skills training
programs) that foster or maintain perfectionistic strivings in sport
while simultaneously reducing or inhibiting the development of
perfection-istic concerns. A profile of high perfectionistic strivings
combined with low perfectionistic concerns is most likely to be
associated with adaptive functioning in sport (Gotwals, 2016; Stoeber,
2011) and could potentially reduce the likelihood of social loafing in
athletes (although it should be acknowledged that enhancing or
maintaining high perfectionistic strivings while reducing
perfectionistic concerns may be easier said than done given the positive
correlation that typically exists between perfectionistic strivings and
perfectionistic concerns in sport: see Gotwals et al., 2012).
Ultimately, if sport practitioners have a better understanding of
variables (such as perfectionism) that may influence social loafing in
sport, they have a better chance of reducing the deleterious effects
that social loafing can have upon athletic performance. It is hoped that
the results of this study provide a step towards the accomplishment of
this goal.
Authors' Note
This research was funded by a grant from the Sport Science
Association of Alberta, and was conducted when the first author (Matthew
Vaartstra) was a graduate student at the University of Alberta.
Matthew Vaartstra
University of Idaho
John G.H. Dunn and Janice Causgrove Dunn
University of Alberta, Canada
Address correspondence to: Dr. John Dunn, Faculty of Kinesiology,
Sport, and Recreation, 3-100 University Hall, Van Vliet Complex,
University of Alberta, Edmonton, Alberta, Canada, T6G 2H9. (Phone:
780-492-2831) E-mail: john.dunn@ualberta.ca
References
Aiken, L. R. (1985). Three coefficients for analyzing the
reliability and validity of ratings. Educational and Psychological
Measurement, 45, 131-142.
Anshel, M. H. (1995). Examining social loafing among elite female
rowers as a function of task duration and mood. Journal of Sport
Behavior, 18, 39-49.
Blatt, S. J. (1995). The destructiveness of perfectionism:
Implications for the treatment of depression. American Psychologist, 50,
1003-1020.
De Backer, M., Boen, F., De Cuyper, B., Hoigaard, R., & Vande
Broek, G. (2015). A team fares well with a fair coach: Predictors of
social loafing in interactive female sport teams. Scandinavian Journal
of Medicine & Science in Sport, 25, 897-902.
Dunn, J. G. H., Bouffard, M., & Rogers, W. T. (1999). Assessing
item content-relevance in sport psychology scale-construction research:
Issues and recommendations. Measurement in Physical Education and
Exercise Science, 3, 15-36.
Dunn, J. G. H., Causgrove Dunn, J. L., Gotwals, J. K., Vallance, J.
K. H., Craft, J. M., & Syrotuik, D. G. (2006). Establishing
construct validity evidence for the Sport Multidimensional Perfectionism
Scale. Psychology of Sport and Exercise, 7, 57-79.
Dunn, J. G. H., Causgrove Dunn, J. L., & Syrotuik, D. G.
(2002). Relationship between multidimensional perfectionism and goal
orientations in sport. Journal of Sport & Exercise Psychology, 24,
376-395.
Dunn, J. G. H., Craft, J. M., Causgrove Dunn, J. L., & Gotwals,
J. K. (2011). Comparing a domain-specific and global measure of
perfectionism in competitive figure skating. Journal of Sport Behavior,
34, 25-46.
Dunn, J. G. H., Gotwals, J. K., Causgrove Dunn, J., Selzler, A.-M.,
Lizmore, M.R., Vaartstra, M., Sapieja, K.M., & Gamache, V.E. (2016).
A multi-sample investigation of the higher-order latent dimensionality
of the Sport-Multidimensional Perfectionism Scale-2. Psychology of Sport
and Exercise, 27, 150-156.
Frost, R. 0., Marten, P., Lahart, C., & Rosenblate, R. (1990).
The dimensions of perfectionism. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 14, 449
468.
Gilson, T.A. (2014). Social processing effects. In R.C. Eklund
& G. Tennenbaum (Eds.), Encyclopedia of sport and exercise
psychology (Vol. 2, pp. 702-706). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Gorsuch, R. L. (2003). Factor analysis. In I. B. Weiner (Series
Ed.), J. A. Schinka & W. F. Velicer (Vol. Eds.), Handbook of
psychology: Vol. 2. Research methods in psychology (pp. 143-164).
Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.
Gotwals, J. K. (2016). The tripartite model of perfectionism:
Evidence from research in sport and dance. In A. P. Hill (Ed.),
Perfectionism in sport, dance, and exercise (pp. 150-173). London:
Routledge.
Gotwals, J. K., & Dunn, J. G. H. (2009). A multi-method
multi-analytic approach to establishing internal construct validity
evidence: The Sport Multidimensional Perfectionism Scale 2. Measurement
in Physical Education and Exercise Science, 13, 71-92.
Gotwals, J. K., Dunn, J. G. H., Causgrove Dunn, J. L., &
Gamache, V. (2010). Establishing validity evidence for the Sport
Multidimensional Perfectionism Scale-2 in intercollegiate sport.
Psychology of Sport and Exercise, 11, 423-432.
Gotwals, J. K., Stoeber, J., Dunn, J. G. H., & Stall, O.
(2012). Are perfectionistic strivings in sport adaptive? A systematic
review of confirmatory, contradictory, and mixed evidence. Canadian
Psychology, 53, 263-279.
Graham, J. W., Cumsille, P. E., & Elek-Fisk, E. (2003). Methods
for handling missing data. In I.B. Weiner (Series Ed.), J.A. Schinka
& W.F. Velicer (Vol. Eds.), Handbook of psychology: Vol. 2. Research
methods in psychology (pp. 87-114). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.
Hair, J. F., Anderson, R. E., Tatham, R. L., & Black, W. C.
(1998). Multivariate data analysis (5th ed.). Upper Saddle River, NJ:
Prentice Hall.
Hardy, C. J. (1990). Social loafing: Motivational losses in
collective performance. International Journal of Sport Psychology, 21,
305-327.
Hardy, C. J., & Crace, R. K. (1991). The effects of task
structure and teammate competence on social loafing. Journal of Sport
& Exercise Psychology, 13, 372-381.
Haugen, T., Reinboth, M., Hetlelid, K. J., Peters, D.M., &
Hoigaard, R. (2016). Mental toughness moderates social loafing in cycle
time-trial performance. Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport, 87,
305-310.
Hewitt, P. L., & Flett, G. L. (1991). Perfectionism in the self
and social contexts: Conceptualization, assessment, and association with
psychopathology. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 60, 456
470.
Hoigaard, R., Fuglestad, S., Peters, D. M., Cuyper, B. D., Backer,
M. D., & Boen, F. (2010). Role satisfaction mediates the relation
between role ambiguity and social loafing among elite women handball
players. Journal of Applied Sport Psychology, 22, 408-419.
Hoigaard, R., & Ommundsen, Y. (2007). Perceived social loafing
and anticipated effort reduction among young football (soccer) players:
An achievement goal perspective. Psychological Reports, 100, 857-875.
Hoigaard, R., Safvenbom, R., & T0nnessen, F. E. (2006). The
relationship between group cohesion, group norms, and perceived social
loafing in soccer teams. Small Group Research, 37, 217-232.
Hoigaard, R., Tofteland, I., & Ommundsen, Y. (2006). The effect
of team cohesion on social loafing in relay teams. International Journal
of Applied Sports Sciences, 18, 59-73.
Jones, G.W., Hoigaard, R., & Peters, D.M. (2014). "just
going through the motions ...": A qualitative exploration of
athlete perceptions of social loafing in training and competition
contexts--Implications for team sport coaches. International Journal of
Sports Science and Coaching, 9, 1067-1082.
Jowett, G.E., Mallinson, S.H., & Hill, A.P. (2016). An
independent effects approach to perfectionism in sport, dance and
exercise. In A.P. Hill (Ed.), The psychology of perfectionism in sport,
dance and exercise (pp. 85-149). New York: Routledge.
Karau, S. J., & Williams, K. D. (1993). Social loafing: A
meta-analytic review and theoretical integration. Journal of Personality
and Social Psychology, 65, 681-706.
Latane, B., Williams, K. D., & Harkins, S. (1979). Many hands
make light the work: The causes and consequences of social loafing.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 37, 822-832.
Lizmore, M. R., Dunn, J. G. H., & Causgrove Dunn, J. (2016).
Reactions to mistakes as a function of perfectionism and situation
criticality in curling. International Journal of Sport Psychology, 47,
81-101.
Lo, A., & Abbott, M. J. (2013). Review of the theoretical,
empirical, and clinical status of adaptive and maladaptive
perfectionism. Behavior Change, 30, 96-116.
McArdle, S. (2010). Exploring domain-specific perfectionism.
Journal of Personality, 78, 494-508.
Nicholls, J. G. (1989). The competitive ethos and democratic
education. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Rasquinha, A., Dunn, J. G. H., & Causgrove Dunn, J. (2014).
Relationships between perfection-istic strivings, perfectionistic
concerns, and competitive sport level. Psychology of Sport and Exercise,
15, 659-667.
Simms, A., & Nichols, T. (2014). Social loafing: A review of
the literature. Journal of Management Policy and Practice, 15, 58-67.
Smrt, D. L., & Karau, S. J. (2011). Protestant work ethic
moderates social loafing. Group Dynamics: Theory, Research, and
Practice, 15, 261-21 A.
Stoeber, J. (2011). The dual nature of perfectionism in sports:
Relationships with emotion, motivation, and performance. International
Review of Sport and Exercise Psychology 4 128-146.
Stoeber, J., & Madigan, D. (2016). Measuring perfectionism in
sport, dance, and exercise. In A.R Hill (Ed.), The psychology of
perfectionism in sport, dance, and exercise (pp. 31-56). New York:
Routledge.
Stoeber, J., & Otto, K. (2006). Positive conceptions of
perfectionism: Approaches, evidence, challenges. Personality and Social
Psychology Review, 10, 295-319.
Swain, A. (1996). Social loafing and identifiability: The mediating
role of achievement goal orientations. Research Quarterly for Exercise
and Sport, 67, 337-344.
Tabachnick, B. G., & Fidell, L. S. (1996). Using multivariate
statistics (3rd ed.). New York: Harper Collins.
Tan, H. H., & Tan, M. L. (2008). Organizational citizenship
behavior and social loafing: The role of personality, motives, and
contextual factors. The Journal of Psychology, 142 89-108.
Thompson, B. (2005). Canonical correlation analysis. In B.S.
Everitt & D.C. Howell (Eds.), Encyclopedia of statistics in
behavioral science (pp. 192-196). Chichester, UK: Wiley.
Williams, K. D., Harkins, S., & Latane, B. (1981).
Identifiability as a deterrent to social loafing: two cheering
experiments. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 40 303-311.
Williams, K. D., Nida, S.A., Baca, L. D., & Latane, B. (1989).
Social loafing and swimming: Effects of identifiability on individual
and relay performance of intercollegiate swimmers. Basic and Applied
Social Psychology, 10, 73-81.
Woodman, T, Roberts, R., Hardy, L., Callow, N., & Rogers, C.H.
(2011). There is an "I" in TEAM: Narcissism and social
loafing. Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport 82 285-290.
(1) The current sample formed a sub-sample of athletes (N= 1605) in
a study conducted by Dunn et al. (2016). The perfectionism data reported
in this study were used by Dunn et al. (2016) for the sole purpose of
examining the higher-order latent dimensionality of a perfectionism
measure. None of the perceived social loafing data reported in this
study have been reported in any other publication.
Table 1
Aiken's V, Means, and Standard Deviations for
Judges' Content-Relavance Ratings of Social Loafing
(Blame Avoidance and Effort Management) Scenarios
Abbreviated item
descriptions V p
1. Kim saves giving maximal .808 < .01
effort for when she sprints
against one player believing
that the winners of the
one-on-one sprints receive
the most recognition from
the coach following each
race.
2. Whenever Rob is on the .692 < .05
captain's team at practice
and it appears that his team
will lose the scrimmage,
Rob makes less of an effort
to get actively involved in
the play believing that this
will decrease the likelihood
of drawing the captain's
attention.
3. Jan tends to save .981 < .01
giving maximal effort during
practice for offensive
situations rather than
defensive situations.
4. Tim saves giving maximal .885 < .01
effort in practice until he
knows his parents will be
watching him.
5. When the players are .788 < .01
lining up to start the drill
Bob allows his teammates to
go first because he does not
want to risk being the player
who messes up the drill
6. Tom decides to hold back .615 ms
from aggressively seeking
out the ball during practice
scrimmages to decrease the
likelihood of drawing the
coach's attention to his play.
7. Therefore, .942 < .01
during practice,
Jon saves a little effort
during offensive situations
so that he can give maximal
effort during defensive
situations.
8. Although Pam and one of .827 < .01
her teammates have an equal
opportunity to score, to
avoid the risk of missing the
shot that may cause the team
to run the wind-sprints,
Pam backs off to allow her
teammate to take the final
shot.
9. During scrimmage .942 < .01
situations, Joe saves giving
maximal effort for when
he knows the coach is
specifically watching
his performance.
10. Amy attempts to avoid .769 < .01
taking this role in the drill
because she does not want to
be responsible for causing
the drill to fail.
Judges' ratings
Blame Effort
Avoidance Management
Abbreviated item
descriptions M SD M SD
1. Kim saves giving maximal 1.23 0.44 4.23# 1.17
effort for when she sprints
against one player believing
that the winners of the
one-on-one sprints receive
the most recognition from
the coach following each
race.
2. Whenever Rob is on the 3.77# 1.24 1.31 0.63
captain's team at practice
and it appears that his team
will lose the scrimmage,
Rob makes less of an effort
to get actively involved in
the play believing that this
will decrease the likelihood
of drawing the captain's
attention.
3. Jan tends to save 1.00 0.00 4.92$ 0.28
giving maximal effort during
practice for offensive
situations rather than
defensive situations.
4. Tim saves giving maximal 1.08 0.28 4.54# 0.52
effort in practice until he
knows his parents will be
watching him.
5. When the players are 4.15# 1.14 1.38 0.51
lining up to start the drill
Bob allows his teammates to
go first because he does not
want to risk being the player
who messes up the drill
6. Tom decides to hold back 3.46# 1.39 1.85 0.99
from aggressively seeking
out the ball during practice
scrimmages to decrease the
likelihood of drawing the
coach's attention to his play.
7. Therefore, 1.00 0.00 4.77# 0.44
during practice,
Jon saves a little effort
during offensive situations
so that he can give maximal
effort during defensive
situations.
8. Although Pam and one of 4.31# 1.03 1.15 0.38
her teammates have an equal
opportunity to score, to
avoid the risk of missing the
shot that may cause the team
to run the wind-sprints,
Pam backs off to allow her
teammate to take the final
shot.
9. During scrimmage 1.15 0.38 4.77# 0.44
situations, Joe saves giving
maximal effort for when
he knows the coach is
specifically watching
his performance.
10. Amy attempts to avoid 4.08# 1.12 1.15 0.38
taking this role in the drill
because she does not want to
be responsible for causing
the drill to fail.
Note. The mean rating on the intended domain
for each item has been highlighted in bold.
Note. The mean rating on the intended domain
for each item are indicated with #.
Table 2
Subscale Means, Standard Deviations, Internal
Consistencies ([alpha]) and Bivariate Correlations (r)
Perfectionism (a)
PS COM
M SD M SD
Subscales 3.76 .65 3.17 .80
PS [alpha] = .79
COM .35 *** [alpha] = .83
PPP .15 * .47 ***
PCP .15 * .42 ***
DAA .08 .30 ***
ORG .40 *** .12
PSLQ .23 ** .06
SLAQ -.15 * .03
Perfectionism (a)
PPP PCP
M SD M SD
Subscales 2.92 .90 3.24 .75
PS
COM
PPP [alpha] = .90
PCP .30 *** [alpha] = .78
DAA .26 *** .29 ***
ORG .11 .11
PSLQ .07 .04
SLAQ .11 .04
Perfectionism (a)
DAA ORG
M SD M SD
Subscales 2.62 .68 3.48 .82
PS
COM
PPP
PCP
DAA [alpha] = .73
ORG -0.4 [alpha] = .87
PSLQ .25*** .08
SLAQ .21 ** -.19 **
Social Loafing (b)
PSLQ SLAQ
M SD M SD
Subscales 2.34 .76 2.59 .76
PS
COM
PPP
PCP
DAA
ORG
PSLQ [alpha] = .80
SLAQ .11 [alpha] = .75
Note. Internal consistency values are contained in the main
diagonal, and bivariate correlations are contained in the
lower-triangular matrix of the table. Subscale abbreviations:
PS = personal standards; COM = concern over mistakes;
PPP = perceived parental pressure; PCP = perceived coach
pressure; DAA = doubts about actions; ORG = organization;
PSLQ = perceived social loafing questionnaire;
SLAQ = social loafing acceptability questionnaire.
(a) Items were measured on a 5-point response scale.
(b) Items were measured on 7-point response scales.
* p <.05. ** p <.01. *** p <.001.
Table 3
Canonical Loadings (Structure Coefficients)
for Perfectionism and Social Loafing Variables
Canonical Loadings
Variable Function 1 Function 2
Social Loafing
Perceived social loafing questionare .95# .32#
Social loafing acceptability questionnare .42# -.91#
Perfectionism
Personal standards .48# .76#
Concern over mistakes .21 -.08
Perceived parental pressure .31 -.31#
Perceived coach pressure .16 -.11
Doubts about actions .87# -.37#
Organization .07 .62#
Note. Canonical loadings [greater than or equal to]
[absolute value of .30] are considered meaningful
and highlighted in bold.
Note. Canonical loadings [greater than or equal to]
[absolute value of .30] are considered meaningful
are indicated with #.
COPYRIGHT 2018 University of South Alabama
No portion of this article can be reproduced without the express written permission from the copyright holder.
Copyright 2018 Gale, Cengage Learning. All rights reserved.