Differential effects of motives and points of attachment on conative loyalty of Formula 1 U.S. Grand Prix attendees.
Ballouli, Khalid ; Trail, Galen T. ; Koesters, Todd C. 等
Differential effects of motives and points of attachment on conative loyalty of Formula 1 U.S. Grand Prix attendees.
Introduction
Formula 1 racing has millions of fans worldwide, generating
millions in revenue. For the Formula 1 U.S. Grand Prix hosted by Circuit
of The Americas (COTA) in Austin, Texas, more than 235,000 spectators
attended the three-day event in 2014, but attendance has dropped
successively over the years since COTA first hosted the event in 2012
(DiZinno, 2013; Theis, 2014). Though the Formula 1 World Championship is
one of the world's most watched international mega sport
events--roughly 425 million people watched the Formula 1 World
Championship in 2014 (Sylt, 2015)--COTA relies on revenue generated from
ticket sales to accrue any kind of profit margin due to the fact revenue
generated from television rights and track sponsorships for all Formula
1 events go directly back to ownership and management (Gutman, 2012). A
steady decline in race attendance in recent years coupled with limited
potential for revenue generation has led several researchers to question
whether a U.S. niche market like Austin can sustain hosting the Formula
1 race (e.g., Jenson, Cobbs, & Groza, 2014), particularly given how
much bringing Formula 1 to the US cost COTA and the state of Texas
(Sylt, 2014).
Making matters worse for host race facilities like COTA is the fact
Formula 1 has faced criticism recently regarding the increasing
financial commitments demanded of the racing teams and the subsequent
withdrawal of racing teams in Formula 1's most popular events.
According to Weaver (2013), more than half of the sport's $1.5
billion income goes directly to a private equity firm with a controlling
stake in Formula 1, while racing teams spend upwards of $250 million to
be involved in Formula 1. Such a lack of revenue sharing and increased
investment spending has recently forced two racing teams to file for
bankruptcy (Weaver, 2014), and may perhaps explain why a decade-low 18
cars competed in the Formula 1 U.S. Grand Prix last year. Thus, a
growing concern for Formula 1 becomes whether the costly nature of the
sport for spectators, participants, and organizers might potentially
damage future loyalty to the brand (i.e., conative loyalty).
Many researchers have examined conative loyalty (behavioral
intentions supporting the brand or team; see Shapiro, Ridinger, &
Trail, 2013; Trail, Fink, & Anderson, 2003), or related constructs
(e.g., group-supportive behaviors; Fisher & Wakefield, 1998), in the
context of sport. For example, Trail, Kwon, and Anderson (2009) found
advertising mitigated the effects of losing on conative loyalty for
low-involvement consumers of an intercollegiate men's basketball
team. Trail, Anderson, and Fink (2005) found attachment was correlated
with conative loyalty, while Harrolle, Trail, Rodriguez, and Jordan
(2010) showed an indirect relationship, but did not test or report a
direct correlation. Gwinner and Swanson (2003) found highly identified
fans exhibited higher conative loyalty toward sponsors of the team
(e.g., purposely look for sponsors, denote a stronger purchase intent),
while Matsuoka, Chelladurai, and Harada (2003) determined high fan
identification was related to strong intentions to attend future
professional soccer games. Amidst a plethora of research on conative
loyalty in the sport marketing literature, however, there exists a
relative shortage of research on predictors of conative loyalty among
sport consumers.
Based on identity theory and extant literature on identification
and spectator consumption behavior, we sought to investigate the
differential effects of motives and points of attachment on conative
loyalty of attendees of the Formula 1 U.S. Grand Prix. Although
researchers in the sport marketing literature have examined motives and
points of attachment (e.g., Funk & James, 2006; Robinson, Trail,
& Kwon, 2004; Trail, Robinson, Dick, & Gillentine, 2003; Woo,
Trail, Kwon, & Anderson, 2009), there is a relative dearth of
research devoted to investigating the effects of motives and points of
attachment on conative loyalty (e.g., Harrolle et al., 2010). Further,
while studies on Formula 1 have grown rapidly in recent years, a
comprehensive model of motives and points of attachment predicting
conative loyalty has not yet been proposed. The current research
provides some meaningful insights and implications in these regards.
Theoretical Framework
Motivation
Motivation has been defined as "the driving force within
individuals that impels them to action" (Schiffman & Kanuk,
2004, p. 87), and "the energizing force that activates behavior and
provides purpose and direction for that behavior" (Hawkins, Best,
& Coney, 2004, p. 354). There are many theories of motivation
throughout various disciplines; however, this study concerns the
motivation for consumption, predominately spectator sport consumption
(e.g., event attendance). While there is still much to learn about
motives in sport consumption, there is a well-established stream of
sport marketing research dedicated to the topic. Trail and James (2001)
claim there are more than 40 different motives that have been proposed
by scholars, all of which are considered to predict being a
spectator/fan and predict sport consumption behavior or behavioral
intentions, including value development (e.g., Milne & McDonald,
1999), eustress (e.g., Gantz & Wenner, 1995), and self-esteem
enhancement (e.g., Branscombe & Wann, 1991), to name a few.
Wann's (1995, 1997) Sport Fan Motivation Scale (SFMS) measures
eight motives of sport consumption, and includes eustress, self-esteem,
escape, entertainment, economic (gambling), aesthetic, group
affiliation, and family as factors driving sport spectating. Trail and
James (2001) developed the Motivation Scale for Sport Consumption (MSSC)
that identifies nine motivations: achievement, acquisition of knowledge,
aesthetics, drama, escape, family, skill, physical attraction, and
social interaction. Funk, Mahony, Nakazawa, and Hirakawa's (2001)
Sport Interest Inventory (SII) has also contributed a broad range of
motivational factors for sport consumption.
Not only have previous scholars established an inventory of general
sport consumption motives, but also they have gone further in suggesting
that motivations differ by type of sport (Robinson & Trail, 2005;
Wann, Grieve, Zapalac, & Pease, 2008; Wann, Schrader, & Wilson,
1999). For example, work by Wann et al. (1999) showed consumers who
preferred individual (versus team) sport were motivated more by
aesthetics than by any other motive in the SFMS. Consumers who preferred
team sport were motivated by eustress and self-esteem more than any
other motives in the SFMS. In addition, consumers who preferred
aggressive to non-aggressive sport were most strongly motivated by
economic (gambling) factors, and those consumers who preferred
non-aggressive sport were most strongly motivated by aesthetics (Wann et
al., 1999). To this end, Wann et al. (2008) categorized different sports
into three dichotomous pairings--individual/team,
aggressive/non-aggressive, and stylistic/non-stylistic--and found
motivations for the various sports depended on how they were classified
among these dichotomies.
Based on Sloan's (1989) argument that there are distinct
differences between observers and fans, and that motivations may differ
between the two, Trail, Robinson et al. (2003) proposed several models
that categorized motives by those specific to fans, those specific to
spectators, and those that applied to both. They suggested that
spectator motives were those related to the skill of the athletes,
aesthetics of the game, drama, and knowledge acquisition. The only
motive that they suggested was specific solely to fans was vicarious
achievement. However, they also proposed that there were overarching
motives that would be applicable to both spectators and fans--these
motives were comprised of escape and social interaction. These
categorizations were supported.
Woo et al. (2009) reassessed Trail, Robinson et al.'s (2003)
models and extended them, but tested competing models in which they
moved social interaction to be a part of fan motives out of the
overarching motives, and left only escape as the sole overarching
motive. Woo et al. chose this competing model for several reasons.
However, there were no significant differences between the fit of the
competing models, so either model could have been selected based solely
on fit.
Similar to Trail, Robinson et al. (2003) and Woo et al. (2009), we
chose the MSSC to base our selection of motives for this research. The
motives included social interaction, drama, physical skill, aesthetics,
success (vicarious achievement), and acquisition of knowledge. In
addition, we attempted to generate sport-specific motives due to the
research from Wann and colleagues, who suggest sports differ on
motivation (Wann et al., 1999, 2008). To that end, we had a fair amount
of input from a pre-study Formula 1 focus group and we generated ideas
from the popular press, as well. Furthermore, consistent with Trail,
Robinson et al. (2003) and Woo et al. (2009), we hypothesized categories
of motives (e.g., spectator motives).
Through this process, we added a motive of nostalgia to the
spectator motives second order construct based on input from our
pre-study focus group. We also moved social interaction to the spectator
motives construct based on the input that true fans do not go to Formula
1 events for the social aspects--these individuals are referred to as
"superficial" fans. We removed aesthetics from this second
order construct, as well, as we explain later. Therefore, the spectator
motives construct included acquisition of knowledge, appreciation of the
drivers' skill, drama, nostalgia, and social interaction, leaving
vicarious achievement (team success) as the only fan motive and escape
as the only overarching motive.
However, in addition to our review of sport motivation literature
and our focus group with longtime Formula 1 fans, we assessed there
might be supplementary motives for following Formula 1 or being a
Formula 1 fan that were specific to the sport and not included in any of
the prior measures of motives. One of the recent changes that Formula 1
has implemented has been a new look and sound for the cars, including a
new engine that is considerably quieter than the old engine (Johnson,
2014). The longtime fans in the focus group were not in favor of these
changes at all.
However, some of the popular press articles noted that some people
liked the new look and sound because it was being marketed as more
eco-friendly (e.g., Golson, 2014). Thus, we created items to measure
whether the new look, sound, and engine of cars were motives for people.
Also, one of the enhancements to the new cars was an upgrade in
technology that reportedly helped the drivers perform more efficiently.
However, there was disagreement in the focus group as to whether this
was a good thing or not. As such, we created items to determine if this
was a motive. Finally, the U.S. Grand Prix has a relatively new track
that is supposed to be a very aesthetically pleasing track (Alberstadt,
2014). Therefore, we modified the aesthetics motive scale to be track
specific and included it in the category that we call product motives.
In the end, we had five new motive subscales that were specific to the
product (Formula 1 racing), which created a fourth dimension. Based on
the above information we created our first hypothesis:
Hypothesis 1: The four separate motive dimensions (i.e., the
spectator motive dimension, the product motive dimension, team
success--a first order latent variable that by itself represents fan
motives--and an overarching motive of escape) will be correlated with
each other (see Figure 1).
Points of Attachment
Within identity theory, Stryker and Burke (2000) suggest motives
impact identification, or role identities. Specific to sport, several
researchers have examined the relationships between motives and team
identification, or more recently points of attachment. Much of the
extant work on sport consumption has been based on the framework of
identity theory, which integrates both motives and identification, as
previously noted. Stryker and Burke (2000) define identity as
"parts of a self, composed of the meanings that persons attach to
the multiple roles they typically play in highly differentiated
contemporary societies" (p. 284). A role is an identity standard,
which is a cognitive representation (belief) about what the particular
role entails (Stryker & Burke, 2000). A person's identity
standard interacts with perceived situational meanings and causes a
cognitive comparison between the ideal role and the situationally
impacted role. The cognitive comparison, if positive, results in
positive emotions and eventually behaviors (Stryker & Burke, 2000).
Fink, Trail, and Anderson (2002) defined identification within the
context of sport consumption as the "orientation of the self in
regard to other objects (the team) that results in feelings or
sentiments of close attachment" (p. 198). This definition
distinguishes between the cognitive "orientation of self' and
the affective results of feelings or sentiments regarding close
attachment. Based on identity theory and research of Trail et al. (2000)
and Trail, Robinson et al. (2003), Robinson and Trail (2005) determined
there are several points of attachment associated with the connection to
a team or sport organization (e.g., player, coach, community). From the
viewpoint of identity theory, different roles may be formed based on
different points of attachment and the commitment to those roles. For
example, a person can be a fan of the Los Angeles Dodgers (an identity
role), where the point of attachment is the team (Dodgers). However, the
same person might also be a baseball fan (another role). The point of
attachment in this case is the sport. In addition, perhaps that
individual is also a fan of Clayton Kershaw, the Dodgers' pitcher
(player attachment). Each of these identities could be salient at the
same time if Kershaw was pitching for the Dodgers on a particular night.
However, the player-attachment role might not be salient on nights when
he is not pitching. The identity of being a Dodger fan might also not be
relevant if the person is watching the World Series game between the
Kansas City Royals and New York Mets, but the baseball-fan identity
would be. Roles also have a hierarchy of importance (Stryker &
Burke, 2000). Being a Dodger fan might be more important to a person
than being a Kershaw fan. If Kershaw was traded to the Tampa Bay Rays,
perhaps the individual might no longer be a Kershaw fan. On the other
hand, if the individual's commitment to the player is strong
enough, then a new role might be formed during the trade, and the person
now becomes a Rays fan.
Robinson and Trail (2005) suggested that there were at least seven
points of attachment (i.e., player, team, coach, community, type of
sport, university, and level of sport). Other studies found additional
points of attachment as well (e.g., golf tour and charities; Robinson et
al., 2004; athletic department and general sport fan; Shapiro et al.,
2013). Trail, Robinson et al. (2003) proposed and tested a model in
which attachment to the team, coach, community, university, and player
all comprised a higher order construct called organizational
identification (attachment). Trail, Robinson et al. (2003) distinguished
organizational identification from sport identification--the latter
included attachment to the level of sport and attachment to the sport
itself. They found that organizational identification was distinct from
sport identification. Woo et al. (2009) also found similar results,
though community attachment was not included in the organizational
identification dimension.
Similar to motives, we had to modify some of the attachment
subscales and eliminate others due to the unique aspects of Formula 1
and racing in general. We modified attachment to player to attachment to
driver, which made sense since the driver is really the only
"player," but oftentimes is also the face of the organization.
We did not need to modify the attachment to team subscale at all, nor
the attachment to community subscale; however, we re-termed it
attachment to place and made its focus the entire state of Texas where
the U.S. Grand Prix is hosted. Further, based on information we gathered
from popular press surrounding the U.S. Grand Prix, we added another
attachment subscale for attachment to the track facility (COTA). We kept
the attachment to sport (auto racing) and attachment to type of sport
(Formula 1 racing). Based on research from Trail, Robinson et al. (2003)
and Woo et al. (2009), we kept the idea of attachment categories and
included organizational attachment (team and driver), sport attachment
(sport and type of sport), and place attachment (Texas and COTA).
However, rather than create second order latent variables like Woo et
al. (2009), we kept the first order latent variables for two reasons.
First, based on what we gathered from the focus group, the popular
press, and track managers, they seemed to feel the order of some of the
relationships between some of the points of attachment were important.
For example, those who were attached to Texas would be more likely to be
attached to the track itself (COTA) since a lot of state taxes were put
into funding it. Further, the longtime fans in our focus group were
adamant that the drivers were the face of most teams and drove the
connection to the team, unlike in other sports where the team was
paramount. Hence, we proposed our second and third hypotheses:
Hypothesis 2: Attachment to Texas will lead to attachment to COTA.
Hypothesis 3: Attachment to the driver will lead to attachment to
the team.
The relationship between attachment to the sport of racing and
attachment to Formula 1 was not clear among those involved in our focus
group. Most of the focus group felt attachment to Formula 1 would lead
to some degree of attachment to racing in general, but others disagreed.
The popular press and track managers gave the impression that being a
racing fan leads to being a Formula 1 fan. Trail, Robinson et al. (2003)
and Woo et al. (2009) demonstrate the type of sport and the specific
sport are correlated but no directional relationships have been
suggested or tested. With little to guide us, we followed the rationale
of the aforesaid parties and posited the following:
Hypothesis 4: Attachment to the sport of racing will be related to
attachment to Formula 1 racing.
The second reason that we did not create second order constructs
with the two first order latent variables is solely statistical.
Structural equation modeling (SEM) programs often show that when there
are only two first order latent variables loading on a second-order
latent variable, one first-order variable will load perfectly, creating
a boundary parameter violation (Harrolle et al., 2010). To circumvent
this issue and to test the hypothesized order of the points of
attachment, we tested the model as depicted in Figure 1. Further, based
on theory and previous research that has shown motives impact fan
identification and points of attachment, we believed that motives would
vary across points of attachment, which we will discuss later.
Motivation and Points of Attachment
In regards to identity theory, Stryker and Burke (2000) suggested
motives influence the interaction between the identity standard and the
situational meanings, resulting in the cognitive comparison. Within the
sport context, the relationship between motives and team identification
has long been evaluated (e.g., Trail & James, 2001; Wann, 1995; Wann
& Branscombe, 1993). For instance, Trail, Robinson et al. (2003)
found that motives explained 70% of the variance in team identification,
whereas Fink et al. (2002) showed the motive of vicarious achievement
explained vastly more variance in fan identification than did other
motives (e.g., drama/excitement, escape, and acquisition of knowledge).
Relatedly, Wann and Branscombe (1993) found that individuals who were
highly identified made more ego-enhancing attributions for their
teams' success than did those with low to moderate fan
identification. Furthermore, Wann, Ensor, and Bilyeu (2001) found
intrinsic motivations for originally following a team were strongest for
those individuals with high identification.
Based on identity theory and previous sport marketing literature
(Fink et al., 2002; Kwon, Trail, & Anderson, 2005; Robinson &
Trail, 2005; Robinson et al., 2004; Trail et al., 2003), Woo et al.
(2009) proposed and compared multiple models that examined relationships
among fan and spectator motivations and points of attachment. Woo et al.
(2009) and Trail et al. (2003) showed spectator motives predicted sport
identification--approximately 31% of the variance in the former research
and a little over 50% in the latter. Woo et al. (2009) also found fan
motives explained 77% of the variance in organizational identification,
whereas Trail, Robinson et al. (2003) showed that vicarious achievement
(sole fan motive) explained about 73% of the variance in organizational
identification. As such, we also posited those relationships in our
model. With our newly created product/place motive category, we believed
motives would tie in most closely with attachment to place. We thus
included that relationship in the model, creating the following three
hypotheses:
Hypothesis 5: Fan motives (success of the team) will be related to
attachment to the team indirectly through attachment to the driver.
Hypothesis 6: Spectator motives will be directly related to
attachment to the sport and indirectly related to attachment to the type
of sport (Formula 1) through attachment to the sport.
Hypothesis 7: Product/place motives will be directly related to
attachment to the place (Texas) and attachment to the venue (COTA)
indirectly through the former.
Conative Loyalty
Team identification has been associated with conative loyalty many
times in the sport marketing literature, but the relationships among the
remaining points of attachment to conative loyalty have not been
investigated much at all. Conative loyalty is defined by Oliver (1999)
as a behavioral intention to repurchase or a "deeply held
commitment to buy" the brand in the future (p. 35). Though Eagly
and Chaiken (1993) mentioned intentions (conative loyalty), they did not
make a distinction between behavioral loyalty and conative loyalty. Dick
and Basu (1994) gave some insights into the idea of a conative
disposition; however, it included sunk costs, switching costs, and
expectations. To this end, the expectations aspect of conation
postulated by Dick and Basu (1994) was somewhat near to Oliver's
(1999) idea of conative loyalty. Crosby and Taylor (1983) indicated that
conations include intentions and were impacted by cognitive and
affective constructs, which in turn led to purchase behavior. We use
Oliver's (1999) definition and make the distinction that conative
loyalty is the intention or likelihood of acting in a loyal manner in
the future, and is different from cognitive, affective, and behavioral
loyalty (as per Crosby and Taylor, 1983).
Several scholars have investigated conative loyalty or intentions
within the context of sport, many of who measured the construct
differently. Bodet and Bernache-Assollant (2011), Matsuoka et al.
(2003), and Shapiro et al. (2013) all used a single-item measure of
attendance intention. Wu, Tsai, and Hung (2012) used a three-item scale
(intentions to attend games, watch games on TV, and purchase
merchandise). Yoshida, Gordon, Nakazawa, and Biscaia (2014) had slightly
different items (intentions to attend another sporting event, buy
additional products, and spend more than 50% of the sport budget on the
team), while Yoshida, Heere, and Gordon (2015) used two of the same
items as in Yoshida et al.'s (2014) research, but used a
probability-of-making-the-same-choice item for the merchandise item.
Kwon et al. (2005) used three items (likely to attend future games,
likely to buy team merchandise, likely to buy the team clothing). Trail
and colleagues (Trail, Fink, & Anderson, 2003; Trail et al., 2005)
used the same three items as Kwon et al. (2005) plus a fourth item
(likelihood of supporting the team); whereas, Harrolle et al. (2010)
utilized five items--the same four as Trail, Fink, and Anderson (2003)
and Trail et al. (2005) plus an item that measured likelihood of
watching the game highlights with other people.
Interestingly, in the multi-item scales of conative loyalty, some
items may not load highly on the scale, indicating perhaps they
represent different aspects of conative loyalty. For example, in
Harrolle et al.'s (2010) research, both the attending future games
item and watching highlights item had less than 35% common variance,
indicating that they may be distinct. In addition, in Yoshida et
al.'s (2015) research, one item (the probability of attending
another sporting event of my team) only had 29% shared variance. There
were comparable results in Trail et al.'s (2005) research, as well.
The results of all these studies indicate perhaps the different aspects
of conative loyalty should not be measured as one construct, but rather
should be treated as in Gray and Wert-Gray's (2012) study in which
the authors ran four separate regressions with attendance intentions,
media based intentions, purchase of team merchandise intentions, and
word-of-mouth communication intentions as the dependent variables.
For the purposes of this study, we chose three items from prior
research (the likelihood of attending, likelihood of buying licensed
merchandise, and likelihood of continuing to support the team; Trail et
al., 2005) and modified them to be specific to Formula 1 racing and the
U.S. Grand Prix. However we also added a word-of-mouth (WOM) item from
Gray and WertGray (2012) to measure whether attendees would spread the
word about their experiences during the U.S. Grand Prix. WOM is quite
critical to increasing awareness of and interest in a sport event
(Shreffler & Ross, 2013) and allows marketers to reach a greater
number of people, thus increasing return on investment (Sernovitz,
2009). Based on the research, we posited the following hypothesis:
Hypothesis 8: The correlation between the four items measuring
conative loyalty will be insufficient to warrant joining the items into
a single factor (i.e., each item will share less than 50% of the
variance with any other of the four items).
Relationship Between Points of Attachment and Conative Loyalty
Identity theory does not directly address conative loyalty.
However, Stryker and Burke (2000) suggested role identities are
expressed in behaviors that represent the identities. Further, Oliver
(1999) and Crosby and Taylor (1983), among others, hypothesized that
conative loyalty leads to behavioral loyalty. Hence, it would seem role
identities might also influence conations (intentions) before
influencing actual behaviors.
This relationship is supported extensively within sport consumer
research. For example, researchers have shown team identification to be
related to conative loyalty across a multitude of studies (see Bodet
& Bernache-Assolant, 2011; Gray & Wert-Gray, 2012; Matsuoka et
al., 2003; Shapiro et al., 2013; Trail et al., 2005; Wu et al., 2012;
Yoshida et al., 2015). However, there is a relative dearth of research
that examines relationships between points of attachment and conative
loyalty with the exception being Kwon et al. (2005). These authors found
that when all six points of attachment (team, university, sport, player,
level, and coach) were utilized in a multiple regression model,
attachment to the team explained the most variance in conative loyalty
(represented by a likelihood of attending games and buying merchandise
or team clothing), with attachment to the coach and attachment to the
university adding little. The remaining points of attachment were not
significant predictors.
In our model, we hypothesize that attachment to the team,
attachment to COTA, and attachment to Formula 1 will have direct effects
on various aspects of conative loyalty, while attachment to driver,
attachment to place (Texas), and attachment to racing will have indirect
effects on aspects of conative loyalty. However, the impacts of each
will vary by the aspect of conative loyalty as was shown by Gray and
Wert-Gray (2012). These researchers showed team identification explained
10% of the variance in attendance intentions, 38% in WOM communication
intentions, 24% in merchandise consumption intentions, and 11% in media
consumption intentions. Based on the above research, we created
additional hypotheses:
Hypothesis 9: Attachment to the driver (indirectly) and attachment
to the team (directly) will be differentially related to intention to
buy team merchandise, intention to support the team, intention to attend
the next U.S. Grand Prix, and intentions to tell others about their
experiences, with the former two more highly related than the latter
two.
Hypothesis 10: Attachment to place (indirectly) and attachment to
COTA (directly) will be differentially related to intention to purchase
team merchandise, intention to support the team, intention to attend the
next U.S. Grand Prix, and intentions to tell others about their
experiences, with the latter two more highly related than the former
two.
Hypothesis 11: Attachment to racing (indirectly) and attachment to
Formula 1 (directly) will be differentially related to intention to buy
team merchandise, intention to support the team, intention to attend the
next U.S. Grand Prix, and intentions to tell others about their
experiences, with the latter two more highly related than the former
two.
In sum, we seek to extend the aforementioned research with a few
newly developed factors specific to Formula 1 and to posit different
motives will differentially impact points of attachment, which in turn
will be hierarchically ordered. We further posit points of attachment
will subsequently be related to aspects of conative loyalty
differentially, all of which is depicted in Figure 1. Although this
figure depicts only one model, we are testing four models that are the
same except that each conative loyalty item is interchanged as the
dependent variable. In Model 1, the conative loyalty item is the
intention to attend the next year's U.S. Grand Prix. In Model 2,
the conative loyalty item is the intention to purchase team merchandise
in the future. In Model 3, the conative loyalty item is the intention to
continue to support the participant's favorite Formula 1 team in
the future. In Model 4, the conative loyalty item is the intention to
tell others about the participant's experiences at the race. We
chose to test these four models separately rather than to include all
four conative loyalty items in one model to determine the differential
effects on each, a design that is similar to Gray and Wert-Gray (2012).
If we had included all of them in the same model, they would modify
other relationships in the model, making the overall effects less clear.
Method
Procedure
The sample (N=247) comprised attendees of the 2014 Formula 1 U.S.
Grand Prix held in Austin, Texas. Data were collected over the course of
three days, during which Formula 1 racing teams participated in several
practice sessions, qualifying trials, and actual competition racing. A
nearly equal number of questionnaires were completed during each of the
three days of the event. A random sampling design was utilized whereby
researchers intercepted attendees at various site locations across the
approximately eight miles of concourse and spectator traffic space at
COTA. Respondents were intercepted as they entered and exited the track
facility, as well as during race breaks and qualifying rounds. The
respondents were asked to complete a short questionnaire on which they
were to respond to different scale items measuring motives and points of
attachment. The purpose of the study was provided in the questionnaire
with the instructions for completion of the survey. Researchers obtained
institutional approval for research containing human subjects prior to
survey collection. Participation was voluntary, and confidentiality was
assured by efforts to hide participant information using a numeric
coding system as recommended by Hair, Black, Babin, Anderson, and Tatham
(2006).
Participants
The sample was made up of 54.0% males, and the average age of
participants was 41 years. The racial makeup comprised 64.6% Caucasians,
10.7% Hispanics, 3.3% Asians, 2.4% African Americans, and 1.3% other
races. Ten percent of the respondents were international attendees with
a primary residence outside of the US. Respondents had an average range
for household income that was $100,000-$149,999 (28.2%).
Measures
The scales used in this study were the Motivation Scale for Sport
Consumption (MSSC) and the Points of Attachment Index (PAI). Trail and
James (2001) initially developed the MSSC to examine nine motives, but
more recent researchers have examined relationships among these nine
motives and found two categories of fan motives and spectator motives
(Woo et al., 2009). Consistent with the results of Woo et al. (2009), we
measured five "spectator motives" (social interaction,
nostalgia, drama, skill, and knowledge). Nostalgia was included in this
dimension and social interaction was moved here due to input from the
focus group. We also included the overarching motive of escape and the
motive of perceived success of the team (adapted from Funk & James,
2006) as opposed to vicarious achievement, which is generally included
in the MSSC. Furthermore, based on a focus group among longtime Formula
1 fans, we created some sport-specific motives we categorized as
"product and place motives" (track aesthetics, sound of new
cars, look of new cars, engine of new cars, and technological aspects).
All of these had three items, except for drama (four), engine of new
cars (one), and technological aspects (two), which led to a total of 31
items. All items were measured using 7-point Likert-type scales that
ranged from strongly disagree to strongly agree. Previous sport
marketing researchers have utilized the MSSC and shown good construct
reliability, discriminant validity, criterion validity, and internal
consistency (e.g., Fink et al., 2002; Robinson & Trail, 2005;
Robinson et al., 2004; Trail & James, 2001).
Based on Trail, Robinson et al. (2003), a total of six subscales
were used to measure points of attachment for (a) driver, (b) team, (c)
place, (d) venue, (e) auto racing, and (f) Formula 1. Each subscale had
three items; thus, the PAI had a total of 18 items with a 7point
Likert-type scale response format (strongly disagree to strongly agree).
Previous scholars have shown the PAI to have a strong reliability and
convergent validity (e.g., AVE .48-68; Robinson & Trail, 2005).
Further, three scale items from Trail et al.'s (2005) conative
loyalty scale were included and a fourth item measuring WOM from Gray
and Wert-Gray (2012) was also included. As was described in the previous
section, each of the conative loyalty items was employed separately as
an outcome dependent variable in each of the four models. Each of the
items was prefaced with the following statement: "We are interested
in what motivates you to attend the Formula 1 U.S. Grand Prix. Please
rate the extent to which you disagree or agree with the following
items."
Results
We used SEM to test the measurement models and the structural
models. The CFA on the motives revealed an adequate fit (RMSEA = .069,
CI = .061-.077, [chi square]/df= 787.4/440 = 1.79), as did that on
points of attachment (RMSEA = .077, CI = .064-.090, [chi square]/df =
254.6/120 = 2.12). Cronbach's alpha values for the motives scales
ranged from .719 to .950, and the AVE values ranged from .627 to .880.
Alpha values were good for points of attachment, and they ranged from
.721 to .888. The AVE values ranged from .409 to .750 (Table 1). All of
the subscales were distinct from each other as AVE values exceeded the
squared correlations (Table 2). The structural models fit adequately
well (Model 1 [attendance intentions]: RMSEA = .083, CI = .078-.088,
[chi square]/df = 2519/1302 = 1.93; Model 2 [merchandise intentions]:
RMSEA = .083, CI = .078-.087, [chi square]/df = 2517/1302 = 1.93; Model
3 [support intentions]: RMSEA = .083, CI = .078-.088, [chi square]/df =
2526/1302 = 1.94; Model 4 [WOM intentions]: RMSEA = .084, CI = =
.079-.089, [chi square]/df = 2560/1302 = 1.97). As per Cohen (1988), all
paths in all of models were significant and meaningful, ranging from
[beta] = .279 to [beta] = .859 (see Table 3), except for a few paths to
some of the conative loyalty variables.
Discussion
Based on identity theory and extant literature on motivations,
points of attachment, and spectator consumption behavior, we sought to
investigate the differential effects of motives and points of attachment
on conative loyalty by testing four versions of a model of conative
loyalty of Formula 1 U.S. Grand Prix attendees. In addition, this study
has significant implications for sport consumer research as it
demonstrates how motives related to spectatorship, success of the team,
and product/place significantly impact points of attachment, which
mediate the motive-conative loyalty relationship. Lastly, we have
developed and shown there are specific motives relevant to Formula 1
racing.
Overview of Significant Findings
Not only did we find that the four proposed models fit the data
moderately well, we determined motives and points of attachment combined
to explain about 37% of the variance in intentions to attend the U.S.
Grand Prix the next year, 41% of the variance in intentions to buy team
licensed merchandise in the future, 52% of the variance in supporting a
favorite Formula 1 team in the future, and a little more than 23% of WOM
intentions. Interestingly, different points of attachment contributed
different amounts of the variance. Attachment to COTA explained the
majority of variance in attendance intentions (32%), whereas attachment
to the team explained a minor amount (less than 1%) and attachment to
Formula 1 explained less than 4%. Conversely, attachment to the team
explained most of the variance in supporting the team in the future
(44%), whereas attachment to COTA and Formula 1 explained very little
(less than 4%). Explaining the variance in intentions to buy team
merchandise was more evenly distributed with attachment to Formula 1
explaining 15% and attachment to the team explaining 14%, but attachment
to COTA explained only 4%. Although not much variance in WOM intentions
was explained, most of it was done by attachment to COTA (19%).
Support for Hypotheses
Our model shows support for the theoretical framework that we used
as well. Identity theory (Stryker & Burke, 2000) suggests motives
impact role identities, which in turn results in behavior or, as in our
research, behavioral intentions. We were able to show these
relationships. Specifically, theory and applied research on motives
suggest there are multiple motives (Sloan, 1989) and that they can be
distinguished by category (Trail, Robinson et al. 2003; Woo et al.,
2009). We found a second order spectator motive construct was apparent
and well represented by social interaction, nostalgia, drama,
appreciation of physical skills, and acquisition of knowledge (see
Figure 2). We also found that a second order product/place motive
construct existed and was moderately represented by our newly created
motives specific to Formula 1 racing: aesthetics of the track, sound of
the cars, look of the new cars, new engine, and technology (see Figure
2). In support of Hypothesis 1, these two higher order constructs were
also correlated with the success of team motive and the overarching
motive of escape. Although these motives categories were correlated to
some degree, they were still distinct from each other--the largest
amount of shared variance was between success of the team and spectator
motives at less than 44% (see Table 3).
We were also able to support Hypotheses 2-4. Attachment to place
(Texas) was related to attachment to COTA. However, shared variance was
not as much as anticipated (less than 22%). This may have been due to
the discontent with the amount of money (taxes) that was being spent
supporting COTA and Formula 1 (see Sylt, 2014). Furthermore, the shared
variance between attachment to driver and attachment to team was fairly
high (approximately 53%), supporting Hypothesis 3 (as well as the focus
group's adamant assertions). This amount of shared variance between
attachment to player and attachment to team is much higher than what
scholars have typically found in prior research (Robinson & Trail,
2005; Trail, Robinson et al., 2003; Woo et al., 2009), which indicates
that Formula 1 is unique in this regard. We also found support for
Hypothesis 4 and Woo et al.'s (2009) research in that attachment to
sport would be related to attachment to Formula 1 (45%).
Our findings showed that motives differentially impact how people
relate to the team and other aspects (Wann, 1995, 1997; Wann et al.,
2001, 2008). In support of Hypothesis 5 and Woo et al. (2009), we showed
that the motive of success of the team directly impacted attachment to
the driver (60%), and indirectly impacted attachment to the team (32%)
through attachment to the driver. Further, we found that spectator
motives impacted attachment to racing (74%) and indirectly impacted
attachment to Formula 1 (33%), supporting Hypothesis 6 and previous work
by Woo et al. (2009). Although Hypothesis 7 was supported, the amount of
variance explained in attachment to place by the product/place motives
was lower than we anticipated (less than 20%) and the indirect effect on
attachment to COTA was lower still (4%). This may have been due to the
combination of product and place in the motives, but we are unsure.
Our findings showed support for Hypothesis 8, as well. The
correlations among the four conative loyalty items were not high, with
the largest amount of shared variance less than 35%. This is consistent
with the findings of Gray and Wert-Gray (2012) and perhaps explains some
of the low item loadings for some conative loyalty items in previous
research (Harrolle et al., 2010, Trail et al., 2005; Yoshida et al.,
2015).
We also found support for Hypothesis 9, in that we found that
attachment to the team and attachment to the driver (indirectly)
differentially impacted the four conative loyalty items. The largest
impact was on supporting the team in the future, followed by intention
to buy team licensed merchandise, supporting the research of Kwon et al.
(2005). However, the two points of attachment were not significantly
related to attending the U.S. Grand Prix the following year or
intentions to tell others about their U.S. Grand Prix experiences.
Although we thought the latter relationships would be lower than the
former, we did not expect them to be non-significant. We assumed there
would be at least some relationship, as previous researchers have shown
(Gray & Wert-Gray, 2012), but this was not the case.
Attachment to place (indirectly) and attachment to COTA (directly)
showed the most consistent effects across the four conative loyalty
items. We posited these points of attachment would be more highly
correlated to intention to attend the next U.S. Grand Prix and in
telling others about U.S. Grand Prix experiences than the other two
items (Hypothesis 10). This was supported, as attachment to COTA
explained 32% of intentions to attend and 18% of WOM intentions,
respectively, whereas this point of attachment explained only around 4%
of intentions to buy team merchandise and support the team. These latter
relationships were similar to Kwon et al.'s (2005) findings.
Finally, Hypotheses 11 was only partially supported. We proposed
the attachment to Formula 1 (directly) and the attachment to racing
(indirectly) would differentially impact the four conative loyalty
items. Attachment to Formula 1 explained about 15% of the variance in
purchasing team licensed merchandise in the future, but did not explain
a significant amount of variance in any of the other conative loyalty
items (as per Cohen, 1988). The indirect effects of attachment to racing
were not significant. These latter results were consistent with Kwon et
al.'s (2005) results, in which attachment to sport or to level of
sport was not significantly related to conative loyalty as a whole.
In sum, most of our hypotheses were supported through the findings.
Primarily, motives variably impacted points of attachment, which in turn
related to different conative loyalty items. Further, all of our models
fit adequately well, and we explain relatively fair amounts of variance
across most of our proposed relationships.
Implications of the Study
There are several practical implications for sport marketers and
sport managers, as well as Formula 1 racing in particular. First, it is
requisite to understand that what motivates people to attend a Formula 1
race in particular is varied and different in some aspects than other
sports. In addition, the points of attachment are varied, as well. As
such, one implication of the research is not surprising--segmenting this
market is necessary, and marketing to these segments will vary fairly
substantially relative to both motives and points of attachment. Second,
with regard to the track facilities that host Formula 1 races, our
results indicated that attachment to COTA is what drives people to want
to attend the Formula 1 U.S. Grand Prix in the future and to discuss
their past experiences at the event with others. To a lesser and small
extent, attachment to Formula 1 and attachment to Texas also contribute
to intentions to attend. However, our results showed that attachment to
the driver and attachment to the team does not. Therefore, marketing
should focus on the host track facility and to some extent Formula 1
itself, but not on the teams or drivers, at least for attendance
reasons. Mean scores for the six different points of attachment support
this argument: attachment to the team was the lowest mean of all the
different points of attachment, whereas attachment to COTA was the
highest mean score. This might indicate one of two things. Perhaps this
was a representative sample of attendees, and they are not huge fans of
specific Formula 1 teams, or of the drivers (mean score only slightly
higher than team). Or, maybe this particular sample was not
representative of all attendees. We doubt the latter is the case as the
respondents in this research were randomly surveyed at various entrance,
concourse, and exit locations of COTA across multiple days, so the
sample should be representative.
Third, success of the team and driver is what drives fans to
support their favorite team in the future and buy team licensed
merchandise. As noted previously, a reduction in the number of drivers
and teams at certain Formula 1 races has anecdotally had a deleterious
impact on fandom (Weaver, 2014). This is not surprising based on results
of this research. Further, attachment to COTA and to Formula 1 does not
drive future loyalty to the team to any substantial extent. That is, the
sport and the venue do not drive future team-related revenues, but the
team and drivers do.
Fourth, the product and place motives we created and investigated
specific to Formula 1 (e.g., new engines, new look of the car, sound of
the engines, etc.), do not seem to be related that much to attachment to
the place, to the racetrack (COTA), or to future support of the sport.
This offers complementary support for the anecdotal information found in
the popular press and from our focus group that fans do not enjoy the
latest alterations to the engines or cars. The correlation matrix (see
Table 2) does show that aesthetics of the track and new car technology
is an exception to the rest of the product motives, as these two motives
show a moderate amount of shared variance with some of the points of
attachment and some of the intentions.
Fifth, support for the model and the variables therein should
indicate to sport marketers and sport managers the need to identify the
motives and points of attachment that are relevant to the specific
environment. For example, we found it necessary to modify the variables
and model from previous studies (e.g., Trail et al., 2005; Trail, Fink,
& Anderson, 2003; Trail, Robinson et al., 2003; Woo et al., 2009)
due to the unique aspects of our research context (i.e., Formula 1
racing as the sport and COTA as the venue). We employed previous
research as the foundation to build our model and used many of the
variables included in previous studies. Not surprisingly, variables that
were previously examined by sport marketing researchers explained a
majority of the variance in this model. As a result, the newly created
factors and aspects of the model did explain some variance, but it was
small relative to previously investigated factors.
Limitations and Future Directions
Since we formulated this model and some of these variables
specifically for Formula 1 racing and this venue, this particular model
is not generalizable to other sports or even to other types of racing
without modifications. We are also limited by the fact that the
respondents were all at the venue and thus were sufficiently motivated
and attached to want and be able to attend. In addition, there are other
statistical models that are plausible; however, the theory and extant
research justified we employ the current model so we limited ourselves
to testing only the four versions of it. Further, we believe this sample
is representative of the population of attendees at the Formula 1 U.S.
Grand Prix due to the random selection of respondents; however, if it is
not then this is certainly a limitation.
Future researchers should examine whether there are unique motives
and/or points of attachment specific to the investigated environment. In
addition, it is necessary to investigate whether there are multiple
aspects of conative loyalty or that this research was unique in that
regard. Finally, researchers need to continue to assess the varied
relationships among motives and points of attachment. It is now
relatively apparent that motives do in fact impact points of attachment,
which seemingly mediate (or perhaps moderate, as per Kim, Trail, and
Magnusen [2013]) the relationship to conative loyalty. Nonetheless, both
motives and points of attachment should be incorporated in a more
comprehensive model of conative loyalty; a combination of what Harrolle
et al. (2010) did and what we have done here.
Khalid Ballouli, PhD, is an assistant professor in the Department
of Sport and Entertainment Management at the University of South
Carolina. His research interests include sport consumer behavior, music
in contemporary sport, and branding.
Galen T. Trail, PhD, is a professor of sport management in the
Department of Sport Administration and Leadership at Seattle University.
His research interests include sport consumer behavior and marketing
sustainability through sport.
Todd C. Koesters, JD, MSA, is an assistant professor in the
Department of Sport and Entertainment Management at the University of
South Carolina. His research interests include sport consumer behavior,
sales and marketing, and sponsorship and ROI/ROO.
Matthew J. Bernthal, PhD, is an associate professor in the
Department of Sport and Entertainment Management at the University of
South Carolina. His research interests include consumer behavior in
sport and entertainment, and marketing ethics.
References
Alberstadt, B. (2014, June 17). Top 10 most beautiful Formula 1
destinations. Retrieved from
http://www.therichest.com/sports/racing-sports/top-10most-
beautiful-formula-1-destinations/
Bodet, G., & Bernache-Assolant, I. (2011). Consumer loyalty in
sport spectatorship services: The relationships with consumer
satisfaction and team identification. Psychology & Marketing, 28,
781-802.
Branscombe, N. R., & Wann, D. L. (1991). The positive and
self-concept consequences of sports team identification. Journal of
Sport & Social Issues, 15, 115-127.
Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral
sciences (2nd ed.). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates,
Publishers.
Crosby, L. A., & Taylor, J. R. (1983). Psychological commitment
and its effects on post-decision evaluation and preference stability
among voters. Journal of Consumer Research, 9, 413-431.
Dick, A. S., & Basu, K. (1994). Customer loyalty: Toward an
integrated conceptual framework. Journal of the Academy of Marketing
Science, 22, 99-113.
DiZinno, T. (2013, November 17). More than 113,000 Sunday, 250,000
for weekend, in year two at COTA. Retrieved from
http://motorsportstalk.nbcsports.com/2013/11/17/more-than-113000-sunday-250000- for-weekend-in-year-2-at-cota/
Eagly, A. H., & Chaiken, S. (1993). The psychology of
attitudes. Fort Worth, TX: Harcourt Brace Jovanovich College Publishers.
Fink, J. S., Trail, G. T., & Anderson, D. F. (2002). An
examination of team identification: Which motives are most salient to
its existence? International Sports Journal, 6, 195-207.
Fisher, R. J., & Wakefield, K. (1998). Factors leading to group
identification: A field study of winners and losers. Psychology and
Marketing, 15, 23-40.
Funk, D. C., & James, J. D. (2006). Consumer loyalty: The
meaning of attachment in the development of sport team allegiance.
Journal of Sport Management, 20, 189-217.
Funk, D. C., Mahony, D. F., Nakazawa, M., & Hirakawa, S.
(2001). Development of the sport interest inventory (SII): Implication
for measuring unique consumer motives at team sporting events.
International Journal of Sports Marketing and Sponsorship, 3, 291-316.
Gantz, W., & Wenner, L. A. (1995). Fanship and the television
sports viewing experience. Sociology of Sport Journal, 12, 56-74.
Golson, J. (2014, March 11). The new F1 cars sound like living,
breathing machines. Retrieved from
http://www.wired.com/2014/03/2014-f1-sound/
Gray, G. T., & Wert-Gray, S. (2012). Customer retention in
sports organization marketing: Examining the impact of team
identification and satisfaction with team performance. International
Journal of Consumer Studies, 36, 275-281.
Gutman, B. (2012, November 16). How brands are activating theirs
sponsorships at Formula 1. Retrieved from
http://www.forbes.com/sites/marketshare/2012/11/16/how-brands-are-activating- their-sponsorships-at-Formula1/
Gwinner, K., & Swanson, S. R. (2003). A model of fan
identification: Antecedents and sponsorship outcomes. Journal of
Services Marketing, 17, 275-294.
Hair, J., Black, W., Babin, B., Anderson, R., & Tatham, R.
(2006). Multivariate data analysis (6th ed.). Upper Saddle River, NJ:
Pearson Educational International.
Harrolle, M. G., Trail, G. T., Rodriguez, A., & Jordan, J. S.
(2010). Conative loyalty of Latino and Non-Latino professional baseball
fans. Journal of Sport Management, 24, 456-471.
Hawkins, D. I., Best, R. J., & Coney, K. A. (2004). Consumer
behavior: Building marketing strategy. Boston, MA: McGraw-Hill Irwin.
Jensen, J. A., Cobbs, J., & Groza, M. D. (2014). The niche
portfolio strategy to global expansion: The influence of market
resources on demand for Formula One racing. Journal of Global Marketing,
27, 247-261.
Johnson, D. (2014, March 27). Sebastian Vettel says sound of
Formula One's new engines is s***. Retrieved from
http://www.telegraph.co.uk/sport/
motorsport/formulaone/sebastian-vettel/10726491/Sebastian-Vettelsays-sound-of- Formula-Ones-new-engines-is-s.html
Kim, Y. K., Trail, G. T., & Magnusen, M. J. (2013). Transition
from motivation to behaviour: examining the moderating role of
identification (ID) on the relationship between motives and attendance.
International Journal of Sports Marketing & Sponsorship, 14,
190-211.
Kwon, H. H., Trail, G. T., & Anderson, D. F. (2005). Are
multiple points of attachment necessary to predict cognitive, affective,
and conative, or behavioral loyalty? Sport Management Review, 8,
255-270.
Matsuoka, H., Chelladurai, P., & Harada, M. (2003). Direct and
interaction effects of team identification and satisfaction on intention
to attend games. Sport Marketing Quarterly, 12, 244-253.
Milne, G. R., & McDonald, M. A. (1999). Sport marketing:
Managing the exchange process. Sudbury, MA: Jones and Bartlett.
Oliver, R. L. (1999). Whence consumer loyalty? Journal of
Marketing, 63, 33-44.
Robinson, M., & Trail, G. T. (2005). Relationships among
spectator gender, motives, points of attachment, and sport preference.
Journal of Sport Management, 19, 58-80.
Robinson, M. J., Trail, G. T., & Kwon, H. (2004). Motives and
points of attachment of professional golf spectators. Sport Management
Review, 7, 167-192.
Schiffman, L. G., & Kanuk, L. L. (2004). Consumer behavior (8th
ed.). Upper Saddle River, NJ: Pearson Prentice Hall.
Sernovitz, A. (2009). Word of mouth marketing: How smart complaints
get people talking. New York, NY: Kaplan.
Shapiro, S., Ridinger, L., & Trail, G. T. (2013). An analysis
of multiple spectator consumption behaviors, identification, and future
behavioral intentions within the context of a new college football
program. Journal of Sport Management, 27, 130-145.
Shreffler, M. B., & Ross, S. D. (2013). The word-of-mouth
phenomenon: Its presence and impact in a sport setting. International
Journal of Sport Communication, 6, 1-18.
Sloan, L. R. (1989). The motives of sports fans. In J. H. Goldstein
(Ed.), Sports, games, and play: Social & psychological viewpoints
(2nd ed.; pp. 175-240). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Stryker, S., & Burke, P. J. (2000). The past, present, and
future of an identity theory. Social Psychology Quarterly, 63, 284-297.
Sylt, C. (2014, August 23). Formula One's billion dollar man.
Retrieved from http://www.forbes.com/sites/csylt/2014/08/23/formula-ones-billion-dollar-man/
Sylt, C. (2015, February 1). Formula 1 loses 25 million viewers
driven by switch to pay TV. Retrieved from
http://www.forbes.com/sites/csylt/
2015/02/01/f1-loses-25-million-viewers-driven-by-switch-to-pay-tv/
Theis, M. (2014, November 4). Attendance down again at Formula 1.
Retrieved from http://www.bizjournals.com/austin/news/2014/11/
04/attendance-down-again-at-Formula 1.html
Trail, G. T., Anderson, D. F., & Fink, J. S. (2005). Consumer
satisfaction and identity theory: A model of sport spectator conative
loyalty. Sport Marketing Quarterly, 14, 98-112.
Trail, G. T., Fink, J. S., & Anderson, D. F. (2003). Sport
spectator consumption behavior. Sport Marketing Quarterly, 12, 8-17.
Trail, G. T., & James, J. D. (2001). The motivation scale for
sport consumption: Assessment of the scale's psychometric
properties. Journal of Sport Behavior, 24, 108-127.
Trail, G. T., Kwon, H., & Anderson, D. F. (2009). Does
advertising mitigate the negative effects of losing on satisfaction and
conative aspects of sport attendance? A case study in intercollegiate
athletics. International Journal of Sport Communication, 2, 109-124.
Trail, G. T., Robinson, M., Dick, R., & Gillentine, A. (2003).
Motives and points of attachment: Fans versus spectators in
intercollegiate athletics. Sport Marketing Quarterly, 12, 217-227.
Wann, D. L. (1995). Preliminary validation of the sport fan
motivation scale. Journal of Sport and Social Issues, 19, 377-396.
Wann, D. L. (1997). Sport psychology. Upper Saddle River, NJ:
Prentice Hall.
Wann, D. L., & Branscombe, N. R. (1993). Sports fans: Measuring
degree of identification with their team. International Journal of Sport
Psychology, 24, 1-17.
Wann, D. L., Ensor, C. L., & Bilyeu, J. K. (2001). Intrinsic
and extrinsic motives for originally following a sport team and team
identification. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 93, 451-454.
Wann, D. L., Grieve, F. G., Zapalac, R. K., & Pease, D. G.
(2008). Motivational profiles of sport fans of different sports. Sport
Marketing Quarterly, 17, 6-19.
Wann, D. L., Schrader, M. P., & Wilson, A. M. (1999). Sport fan
motivation: Questionnaire validation, comparisons by sport, and
relationship to athletic motivation. Journal of Sport Behavior, 22,
114-139.
Weaver, P. (2013, May 24). Seven F1 teams face crisis over looming
bill for extra [pounds sterling]1.32bn. Retrieved from
http://www.theguardian.com/sport/2013/may/24/f1-teams-crisis-bill-costs
Weaver, P. (2014, November 2). Teams going bankrupt in F1 is
nothing new but solution is needed. Retrieved from
http://www.theguardian.com/
sport/2014/nov/02/formula-one-bankrupt-marussia-caterham
Woo, B., Trail, G. T., Kwon, H. H., & Anderson, D. F. (2009).
Testing models of motives and points of attachment. Sport Marketing
Quarterly, 18, 38-53.
Wu, S. H., Tsai, C. Y. D., & Hung, C. C. (2012). Toward team or
player? How trust, vicarious achievement motive, and identification
affect fan loyalty. Journal of Sport Management, 26, 177-191.
Yoshida, M., Gordon, B., Nakazawa, M., & Biscaia, R. (2014).
Conceptualization and measurement of fan engagement: Empirical evidence
from a professional sport context. Journal of Sport Management, 28,
399-417.
Yoshida, M., Heere, B., & Gordon, B. (2014). Predicting
behavioral loyalty through community: Why other fans are more important
than our own intentions, our satisfaction, and the team itself. Journal
of Sport Management, 29, 318-333.
Address author correspondence to:
Khalid Ballouli
University of South Carolina
Carolina Coliseum, Room 2026-N
Columbia, SC 29208
Email: ballouli@sc.edu
Table 1
Factor Loadings, Confidence Intervals, Alpha Coefficients, and AVE
Values
[beta] CI Alpha AVE
Acquisition of Knowledge .845 .689
It provides opportunities to .729 .655-.803
increase my knowledge about the
sport
It provides opportunities to .832 .780-.885
increase my understanding of the
strategy
It provides opportunities to .918 .880-.956
learn about the technical aspects
of the sport
Physical Skill of Drivers .864 .710
The skills of the drivers .802 .742-.862
The abilities of the drivers .831 .777-.885
The talents of the drivers .893 .850-.936
Escape .873 .775
It provides an escape from my .912 .873-.951
day-to-day routine
It provides a distraction from my .887 .844-.929
everyday activities
It provides a diversion from .841 .790-.891
"life's little problems" for me
Nostalgia .789 .632
The Grand Prix brings back .753 .678-.829
nostalgic feelings for me
The Grand Prix reminds me of the .778 .706-.849
history of F1 racing
The Grand Prix brings back fond .850 .790-.909
memories for me.
Aesthetic Quality of the Circuit .856 .724
The circuit at COTA is beautiful .791 .727-.855
The circuit at COTA is stunning .883 .834-.932
The circuit at COTA is .876 .830-.929
magnificent
Drama .851 .652
F1 is often very dramatic .764 .696-.831
F1 is often very exciting .837 .785-.889
F1 is often thrilling .829 .776-.883
The rivalries between the drivers .797 .736-.857
are intense
Technology .719 .627
Recent technologies (KERS/DRS) .763 .673-.852
have made the racing more
exciting
The improved technology in F1 has .820 .734-.906
made it more interesting
New Engine (single item)
The new V6 engines have made F1
more exciting
Social Interaction .846 .665
I love having an opportunity to .726 .649-.803
socialize with other F1 fans.
I enjoy having an opportunity to .961 .913-1.00
interact with other F1 fans.
It's great being able to talk to .737 .662-.812
other F1 fans.
Success of the Team .872 .677
My favorite racing team being .817 .757-.877
successful is important to me
My favorite team winning more .819 .760-.878
races than it loses is important
to me
My favorite team winning is most .833 .776-.889
important
Cars .795 .674
The look of the cars (e.g., new .711 .630-.791
noses) this year is exciting
The cars are great looking this .874 .821-.928
year
The cars look really good this .868 .814-.922
year
Sound .950 .880
I really like the sound of the .936 .913-.959
new engines
This year I really enjoy the .938 .915-.961
sound of the cars
The new engines sound great .940 .917-.962
Attachment to the Team .774 .576
I consider myself to be a "real" .761 .685-.838
fan of a particular racing team
I would feel a loss if I had to .670 .578-763
wasn't a fan of a particular
racing team
Being a fan of a particular .836 .771-.901
racing team is very important to
me
Attachment to the Driver .887 .750
I am more a fan of the driver .845 .796-.895
than of his racing team
I am a fan of a specific driver .887 .846-.929
more than I am a fan of his
racing team
I consider myself a fan of a .866 .821-.911
certain driver rather than of his
racing team
Attachment to the Sport .888 .735
First and foremost I consider .699 .621-.777
myself an auto racing fan
Auto racing is my favorite sport .938 .902-973
Of all sports I prefer auto .915 .877-.954
racing
Attachment to Formula 1 .724 .409
I am a fan of F1 regardless of .552 .438-.666
what teams are racing
I am not just a fan of one .598 .491-.705
specific F1 team, but F1 in
general
I consider myself a fan of F1 and .751 .668-.835
not just one specific team
Attachment to Place .834 .664
I feel connected to the state of .924 .870-.979
Texas
I am a Texan .769 .697-.842
I support Texas as a state .740 .662-.817
Attachment to COTA .721 .547
COTA is the best circuit in the .630 .526-.734
U.S.
I am a big fan of COTA .797 .716-.879
Being a fan of COTA is very .780 .697-.864
important to me
Conative Loyalty (separate items)
I am likely to come to the
Formula 1 U.S. Grand Prix next
year
I am likely to buy team licensed
merchandise in the future
I am likely to continue to
support my favorite F1 team in
the future
I will tell people about my
experiences here at this race
Table 2
Correlations (below the diagonal), Squared Correlations (above the
diagonal), and AVE Values (on the diagonal)
1 2 3 4 5 6
Aesthetics .689 .144 .334 .167 .098 .066
Cars .379 .674 .046 .227 .026 .108
Drama .578 .215 .652 .244 .140 .005
Technology .409 .476 .494 .627 .123 .027
Escape .313 .161 .374 .351 .775 .030
Attachment .257 .328 .073 .164 .172 .664
to place
Attachment .634 .317 .496 .401 .359 .414
to COTA
Attachment .249 .107 .371 .302 .311 .017
to Driver
Attachment .332 .090 .462 .310 .286 -.066
to Sport
Attachment .317 .109 .446 .376 .331 .050
to Team
Attachment .428 .097 .639 .355 .367 .026
to FI
Acquisition .434 .260 .548 .483 .466 .115
Knowledge
Nostalgia .347 .181 .466 .345 .298 .133
Physical .566 .246 .666 .438 .450 .126
Skill
Social .449 .186 .556 .485 .406 .198
Interaction
Sound .058 .500 -.031 .319 .060 .305
Success .268 .193 .420 .293 .293 .038
of Team
New Engine .110 .363 .035 .322 .086 .198
Attendance .363 .120 .439 .343 .242 .248
Intention
Merchandise .362 .063 .570 .299 .235 .004
Intention
Support .313 .181 .534 .411 .343 .067
Intention
WOM .472 .193 .470 .338 .286 .081
Intention
7 8 9 10 11 12
Aesthetics .402 .062 .110 .100 .183 .188
Cars .100 .011 .008 .012 .009 .068
Drama .246 .138 .213 .199 .408 .300
Technology .161 .001 .096 .141 .126 .233
Escape .129 .097 .082 .110 .135 .217
Attachment .171 .000 .004 .003 .001 .013
to place
Attachment .547 .129 .138 .202 .205 .229
to COTA
Attachment .359 .750 .179 .292 .159 .276
to Driver
Attachment .371 .423 .735 .248 .348 .222
to Sport
Attachment .449 .540 .498 .576 .162 .334
to Team
Attachment .453 .399 .590 .403 .409 .287
to FI
Acquisition .479 .525 .471 .578 .536 .689
Knowledge
Nostalgia .439 .366 .440 .449 .479 .488
Physical .537 .437 .478 .488 .633 .589
Skill
Social .484 .337 .316 .459 .457 .469
Interaction
Sound .029 .056 -.122 -.051 -.109 .121
Success .446 .559 .401 .757 .353 .462
of Team
New Engine .137 .159 .070 .091 .051 .212
Attendance .508 .212 .263 .237 .434 .298
Intention
Merchandise .426 .451 .541 .552 .593 .511
Intention
Support .490 .495 .472 .671 .509 .484
Intention
WOM .429 .265 .268 .240 .478 .362
Intention
13 14 15 16 17 18
Aesthetics .120 .320 .202 .041 .072 .012
Cars .033 .061 .035 .250 .037 .132
Drama .217 .444 .309 .001 .176 .001
Technology .119 .192 .235 .102 .086 .104
Escape .089 .203 .165 .004 .086 .007
Attachment .018 .016 .039 .093 .001 .039
to place
Attachment .193 .288 .234 .001 .199 .019
to COTA
Attachment .134 .191 .114 .003 .312 .025
to Driver
Attachment .194 .228 .100 .015 .161 .005
to Sport
Attachment .202 .238 .211 .003 .573 .008
to Team
Attachment .229 .401 .209 .012 .125 .003
to FI
Acquisition .238 .347 .220 .015 .213 .045
Knowledge
Nostalgia .632 .190 .131 .010 .139 .039
Physical .436 .710 .283 .000 .184 .010
Skill
Social .362 .532 .665 .000 .135 .003
Interaction
Sound .102 -.009 -.003 .880 .001 .388
Success .373 .429 .368 -.038 .677 .001
of Team
New Engine .197 .099 .059 .623 .033 NA
Attendance .344 .371 .465 -.039 .217 -.001
Intention
Merchandise .430 .487 .443 -.161 .516 .001
Intention
Support .330 .530 .473 -.053 .639 .054
Intention
WOM .268 .432 .398 -.056 .229 -.028
Intention
19 20 21 22 M SD
Aesthetics .132 .131 .098 .223 5.8 1.22
Cars .014 .004 .033 .037 5.1 1.32
Drama .193 .325 .285 .221 5.6 1.16
Technology .118 .089 .169 .114 5.0 1.36
Escape .059 .055 .118 .082 5.3 1.56
Attachment .062 .000 .004 .007 4.6 1.94
to place
Attachment .258 .181 .240 .184 5.2 1.27
to COTA
Attachment .045 .203 .245 .070 4.7 1.57
to Driver
Attachment .069 .293 .223 .072 5.1 1.72
to Sport
Attachment .056 .305 .450 .058 4.5 1.49
to Team
Attachment .188 .352 .259 .228 5.6 1.11
to FI
Acquisition .089 .261 .234 .131 5.2 1.32
Knowledge
Nostalgia .118 .185 .109 .072 4.5 1.48
Physical .138 .237 .281 .187 5.6 1.22
Skill
Social .216 .196 .224 .158 5.3 1.26
Interaction
Sound .002 .026 .003 .003 3.8 2.02
Success .047 .266 .408 .052 4.7 1.48
of Team
New Engine .000 .000 .003 .001 3.9 1.81
Attendance NA .147 .167 .213 5.7 1.61
Intention
Merchandise .383 NA .343 .157 5.2 1.58
Intention
Support .409 .586 NA .178 5.4 1.50
Intention
WOM .461 .396 .422 NA 6.3 1.18
Intention
Table 3
Path Coefficients and Fit Indices Across Different Models
Model Model Model Model
1 2 3 4
Path [beta] [beta] [beta] [beta]
Escape [left and right arrow] .320 .323 .323 .323
Success
Escape [left and right arrow] .339 .345 .345 .345
Product/Place Motives
Escape [left and right arrow] .540 .543 .543 .543
Spectator Motives
Success [left and right arrow] .279 .283 .284 .283
Product/Place Motives
Success [left and right arrow] .659 .663 .663 .663
Spectator Motives
Product/Place Motives [left and .561 .564 .564 .564
right arrow] Spectator Motives
Success [right arrow] Attachment .767 .774 .776 .770
to Driver
Attachment to Driver [right .717 .724 .731 .718
arrow] Attachment to Team
Product/Place Motives [right .441 .407 .408 .408
arrow] Attachment to Place
Attachment to Place [right arrow] .469 .459 .462 .442
Attachment to COTA
Spectator Motives [right arrow] .856 .858 .858 .859
Attachment to Racing
Attachment to Racing [right .663 .671 .667 .666
arrow] Attachment to F1
Attachment to Team [right arrow] -.040
Intention to attend F1 U.S Grand
Prix next year
Attachment to Team [right arrow] .374
Intention to buy team merchandise
in the future
Attachment to Team [right arrow] .667
Intention to continue to support
my favorite F1 team in the future
Attachment to Team [right arrow] .077
WOM Intention
Attachment to COTA [right arrow] .566
Intention to attend F1 U.S Grand
Prix next year
Attachment to COTA [right arrow] .202
Intention to buy team merchandise
in the future
Attachment to COTA [right arrow] .209
Intention to continue to support
my favorite F1 team in the future
Attachment to COTA [right arrow] .433
WOM Intention
Attachment to F1 [right arrow] .194
Intention to attend F1 U.S Grand
Prix next year
Attachment to F1 [right arrow] .387
Intention to buy team merchandise
in the future
Attachment to F1 [right arrow] .064
Intention to continue to support
my favorite F1 team in the future
Attachment to F1 [right arrow] .159
WOM Intention
[R.sup.2]
Attachment to Driver 58.9% 59.9% 60.1% 59.4%
Attachment to Team 51.4% 52.4% 53.4% 51.5%
Attachment to Place 19.4% 16.5% 16.7% 16.6%
Attachment to COTA 22.0% 21.1% 21.4% 19.5%
Attachment to Racing 44.0% 45.1% 44.5% 44.4%
Attachment to F1 73.3% 73.6% 73.7% 73.8%
Intention to attend F1 U.S 37.0%
Grand Prix next year
Intention to buy team 40.7%
merchandise in the future
Intention to continue to 52.1%
support my favorite F1
team in the future
WOM Intention 23.3%
Model Fit RMSEA .083 .083 .083 .084
[chi square]/df 1.93 1.93 1.94 1.97
COPYRIGHT 2016 Fitness Information Technology Inc.
No portion of this article can be reproduced without the express written permission from the copyright holder.
Copyright 2016 Gale, Cengage Learning. All rights reserved.