Examining the effects of advertisement setting and actor race on African Americans' intentions to consume baseball.
Brown, Brandon ; Bennett, Gregg ; Ballouli, Khalid 等
Examining the effects of advertisement setting and actor race on African Americans' intentions to consume baseball.
Introduction
With a current buying power that is estimated to be nearly $1
trillion in annual spending (Nielson, 2013), African American consumers
in the United States are of significant importance to marketers
(Oyewole, 2007; Simpson, Snuggs, Christiansen, & Simples, 2000).
When coupled with the fact that African Americans account for 13% of the
U.S. population (U.S. Census Bureau, 2011) and yet still make up a
smaller percentage of fans among some of the major North American sports
leagues (SportsBusiness Daily, 2010), this consumer market becomes of
major significance for sports marketers responsible for increasing
attendance and viewership for Major League Baseball (MLB), Major League
Soccer (MLS), NASCAR, National Basketball Association (NBA), National
Football League (NFL), and National Hockey League (NHL). Perhaps the
most alarming development among African Americans' sport
consumption in recent years is the waning interest in MLB (Brown &
Bennett, 2015). Recent studies indicate that African Americans comprise
less than 10% percent of baseball fans in the US (Clinkscales, 2013),
and make up an even lesser percentage of consumers who view baseball on
television (Armour, 2002; Livingstone, 2011). These numbers might
suggest some dated, poorly communicated, or otherwise deficient
marketing strategies on the part of sport marketers attempting to reach
this demographic. Still, recent studies on the topic focus on the
motives and constraints of African Americans to attend sporting events,
and utilize inductive techniques and grounded theory (e.g., Stadler
Blank, Sweeney, & Fuller, 2014). In an attempt to advance these
efforts, the current study incorporates a deductive experimental design
to understand the effects of baseball advertising on African Americans,
whereby the match-up hypothesis and the theory of reasoned action
provide a framework for examining variables that might influence the
effectiveness of advertising.
According to Ogden and Hilt (2003), African Americans are consuming
increasingly less baseball largely because of a lack of collective
identity, a process that "involves an absorption of cultural traits
by individuals in the formation of self-identity," such that
minority group members might gravitate to one sport over another due to
aspects that facilitate cultural identity and group interest in the
sport (p. 213). For example, these authors found four factors of
collective identity that indicate a preference for the sport of
basketball among African Americans in recent years: an encouragement
from authority figures to play basketball, the depiction of the sport as
a form of self-expression and empowerment, the profusion of African
American players that are shown in media and marketing, and perceptions
of basketball's influence on social mobility. Given the decline in
African American attendance and participation in baseball, these
findings suggest that baseball lacks such factors through which African
Americans can identify. Thus, sport marketers might need to explore
cultural and social factors in their communications that may predispose
African American sport consumers to identify with the sport of baseball.
Cortese (2007) claims advertising is "a mechanism through
which members of a society assimilate their cultural heritage and
cultural ideologies" (p. 2). In image-oriented ads of today,
ideologies (i.e., images, concepts, principles) may frame the manner in
which people represent, interpret, and understand their cultural
identity and views of social life (Hall, 2013). Therefore, advertisers
increasingly recognize the significance of representing marginalized
groups in their communication efforts, and the overall importance of
multiculturalism in marketing, to gaining the interest and loyalty of
these consumers (Cortese, 2007). Theoretical principles of the match-up
hypothesis support this rationale--a greater perceived fit between an
endorser and the brand leads to more effective communication with target
consumer audiences (Misra & Beatty, 1990), as does a greater
perceived racial fit between advertisement actors (i.e., individuals who
serve as main characters or play some other supporting part in the
advertisement; Corbett, 2002) and the target audience (e.g., Appiah,
2001; Kerin, 1979; Kim & Cheong, 2011; Schlinger & Plummer,
1972). Therefore, in order to entice consumption of products, marketers
seek to alter consumers' attitudes by designing specific settings
and actors in advertisements that might allow consumers to envision
themselves using the same promoted product or service (Boyd & Shank,
2004; Kim & Cheong, 2011). In addition, given extant studies show
African Americans as having negative attitudes towards baseball (Brown
& Bennett, 2015; Ogden & Hilt, 2003), marketers can utilize the
match-up hypothesis as a way to investigate whether or not culturally
matching and racially similar baseball advertisements impact African
Americans' perceived fit with an advertisement message, which might
potentially alter these consumers' attitudes toward the sport of
baseball.
While attitudes are recognized as being important drivers of sport
consumption (Funk & James, 2004; Funk & Pastore, 2000),
attitudes alone may not necessarily account for significant variation in
consumers' behavioral intentions (Fishbein & Ajzen, 1975;
Glassman & Albarracin, 2006). However, according to theory of
reasoned action (Ajzen, 2002; Ajzen & Fishbein, 1980), researchers
can predict intentions by examining attitudes and subjective norms.
Research on the theory of reasoned action shows attitudes are correlated
with subjective norms, and that positive evaluations of both concepts
may subsequently affect behavior (Madden, Ellen, & Ajzen, 1992;
Sheppard, Hartwick, & Warshaw, 1988). Therefore, principles of both
the match-up hypothesis and the theory of reasoned action were employed
in this study as a means of developing a set of testable hypotheses that
address the elements of our research questions: To what extent do race
and setting in baseball advertisements influence perceived identity fit
among African Americans, and how might perceived identify fit affect
their attitudes toward baseball, subjective norms, and intentions to
consume baseball?
The main purpose of this study was to examine African
Americans' attitudes, subjective norms, and behavioral intentions
towards the sport of baseball by exploring the effects of setting and
race in print baseball advertisements. We propose baseball
advertisements portraying a sense of identity fit between prominent
advertisement characteristics (i.e., setting and race) and African
American viewers will influence the attitudes viewers have toward
baseball. Further, we propose that perceived fit with the advertisement
may also influence subjective norms, such that baseball advertisements
designed to identify with African American consumers may positively
influence their social opinions about the sport. In doing so, this
research fits within a larger body of extant literature on the reasons
why African Americans are consuming increasingly less baseball.
Theoretical Framework
Match-up Hypothesis
The central tenet of the match-up hypothesis is that consumers are
more likely to exhibit positive evaluations of an advertisement and
attitudes toward the product when the advertisement depicts a greater
perceived fit between the product and endorser (Fink, Cunningham, &
Kensicki, 2004; McDaniel, 1999). The theoretical principles of the
match-up hypothesis are founded on the source attractiveness model
(Kelman, 1961), which states that an advertisement's effectiveness
is dependent on how familiar, likable, and similar viewers find the
source to be in an advertisement (Ohanian, 1990). According to Erdogan
(1999) and Ohanian (1990), attractiveness is not always qualified in
terms of physical beauty--attractiveness is often referred to in terms
of resemblance between the source and consumers. To this end, consumers
typically view an advertisement more positively when the actors and
scenes resemble the consumers' physical appearances and cultural
norms, respectively (Boyd & Shank, 2004; Kim & Cheong, 2011).
Much of the research on match-up hypothesis involves perceived fit
or "match" between the advertisement endorser and the
advertised product (e.g., Kahle & Homer, 1985; Till & Busler,
2000). However, there is a growing body of literature focused on the
perceived fit between the advertisement endorser and consumer
(Cunningham, Fink, & Kenix, 2008; Kamins, & Gupta, 1994; Kim
& Cheong, 2011; Liu, Huang, & Minghua, 2007). By these studies,
match-up hypothesis has been employed to show how consumers'
attitudes regarding an advertisement, product, or brand can change
depending on whether they perceive a similarity between them and the
actors and scenes featured in the advertisement. For example, Kim and
Cheong (2011) found Asian Americans responded more favorably to golf
advertisements when Asian actors were used over Anglo actors. Similarly,
Boyd and Shank (2004) found that advertisements featuring female actors
were perceived by female viewers to be more truthful than those that
featured male actors. Further, research also shows consumers will
exhibit less desirable evaluations and attitudes if they observe no
sense of identity with an advertisement (Kamins, 1990; Khale &
Homer, 1985).
In addition to relating with the physical characteristics of
advertisement actors, research indicates consumers also respond to the
settings, scenes, or contexts fabricated and portrayed in an
advertisement. According to Pradeep (2013), "the brain seeks
context in all that it processes, so the selection of the setting for a
scene in an advertisement is important for providing context" (p.
2). To this end, the settings or "story themes" depicted in an
advertisement convey messages that are positioned to appeal (sometimes
in an illusorily fashion) to the culture and norms of the target
audience (Alden, Steenkamp, & Batra, 1999, p. 78). Moreover, Dyer
(2008) contends the setting of an advertisement has as much to do with
appealing to consumers as actors or language. This supports research
conducted by Alden et al. (1999), who claim "culture members"
establish whether the scenes and settings in advertisements exemplify
those ordinarily found in their own consumer culture prior to forming
attitudes and evaluations regarding the advertisement (p. 78).
Armstrong (2002b) suggests African American sport consumers
demonstrate a particular interest in sports they not only identify with
racially (i.e., sports that prominently feature players who are African
American), but also culturally (i.e., sports that represent their
cultural identities). According to Armstrong (2002b), cultural
identities can function as motivational factors--when they are present
in a sport, then African Americans will gain a sense of personal
association with the sport. For example, her findings suggest African
Americans are motivated to consume sports that feature
"excitement" and "skill" as fundamental qualities.
Other researchers have supported these findings, including Brown and
Bennett (2015), who performed interviews with African American sport
consumers and found baseball to be lacking in excitement and skill,
whereas participants perceived these qualities to be core features of
both basketball and football. Based on the aforementioned findings, we
posit that African Americans might perceive a greater fit with a
baseball advertisement if the setting is exciting and illustrates skill
on the part of actors. Furthermore, we hypothesize that actor race will
moderate the "setting-fit" relationship such that actors
having noticeably similar racial features will strengthen the
participants' perceptions of fit. As such, we propose the following
hypotheses:
H1: African American viewers will perceive a baseball advertisement
as being a greater fit with their identity if the advertisement setting
portrays a sense of excitement and skill.
H2: Actor race will moderate the relationship between setting and
perceived fit, such that African American actors will intensify African
American viewers' perceptions of fit.
Theory of Reasoned Action
Based on the principles of social cognitive theory, the theory of
reasoned action suggests attitudes and subjective norms have a
significant influence on an individual's intention to partake in
certain behaviors (Ajzen & Fishbein, 1980; Madden et al., 1992). The
theory has been applied rather extensively in sport management
literature as a means of understanding sport consumption (e.g.,
Cunningham & Kwon, 2003; Funk & James, 2004; Shoham, Rose, &
Kahle, 1998; Walker & Kent, 2013). For instance, Cunningham and Kwon
(2003) showed attitudes and subjective norms toward hockey were
significantly related to intentions to consume the sport. In the current
study, the theory of reasoned action is used as a means to better
understand the lack of baseball consumption among African Americans.
The theory of reasoned action posits one's beliefs about
certain behaviors are influenced by her or his attitudes and subjective
norms (Ajzen & Fishbein, 1980). Attitude refers to the way in which
one views a certain behavior as either favorable or unfavorable (Babiak,
Mills, Tainsky, & Juravich, 2012). An individual's attitudes
towards a behavior are a function of the desirability and attractiveness
of outcomes that are likely to stem from performing the behavior. As
such, consumers commonly behave in ways that consistently lead to
desirable and attractive outcomes (Fishbein & Ajzen, 2010). Bell and
Stephenson (2014) found different attitudinal motivations to compete in
5K races based on runners' perceived levels of ability. Runners who
had a high-level ability revealed attitudes toward racing that supported
their desire and attraction for competition and altruism, while runners
with low-level ability reported attitudes for racing that were focused
more on health and social affiliation outcomes or participation.
According to the match-up hypothesis, if individuals perceive a fit
between an actor and themselves through an advertisement, they will
likely be inclined to change attitudes towards the main product in the
advertisement (Boyd & Shank, 2003; Kim & Cheong, 2011). With
regard to the theory of reasoned action, African Americans might choose
not to consume baseball because of negative attitudes they have towards
baseball. Yet, if African Americans view advertisements better
positioned to match their identity, their attitudes and subsequent
intentions to consume the sport might be positively affected. Therefore,
the following hypothesis is put forth:
H3: African American viewers who perceive a greater fit with a
baseball advertisement will have more positive attitudes towards the
sport of baseball.
Subjective norms refer to an individual's intention to take
part in certain behaviors based on social pressures, or "social
expectations a person has from significant others" (Cunningham
& Kwon, 2003, p. 129). To this end, individuals traditionally
participate in behaviors they perceive as being supported by their
friends and family. Support might include direct support (e.g., direct
verbal support from significant others towards the individual about
partaking in the behavior) or indirect support (e.g., significant others
openly revealing positive attitudes towards the behavior; Hagger,
Chatzisarantis, & Briddle, 2001). For example, Kaplanidou and Gibson
(2012) showed how parents of youth female soccer players were influenced
by a social network of other soccer parents to attend youth soccer
travel events based on what authors called "peer influence"
(p. 8). Moreover, Babiak et al. (2012) found professional athletes were
more likely to donate to charity or engage in philanthropic behaviors
when significant others (i.e., coaches and teammates) were perceived as
valuing altruism.
Research on the match-up hypothesis illustrates how an intensified
sense of identity with an advertisement (i.e., increased perceived fit)
might not only influence consumers' attitudes, but also subjective
norms. For example, if African American consumers perceive an
advertisement's actors and setting as closely resembling and
relating to their culture, they might be more inclined to respond
favorably (both affectively and behaviorally; Alden et al., 1999) to
various marketing messages communicated via the advertisement. As such,
we put forth the following hypothesis:
H4: African American viewers who perceive a greater fit with a
baseball advertisement will have more positive subjective norms towards
the sport of baseball.
Research has suggested that the components of the theory of
reasoned action are positively associated with intentions. For example,
Cunningham and Kwon (2003) found a positive relationship between
attitudes and intentions to attend a hockey game, and subjective norms
and intentions to attend a hockey game. Furthermore, other studies have
also demonstrated a relationship between these variables and intentions
to consume sport (Cheng, Chen, Chen, & Lu, 2012; Lu et al., 2001).
Therefore, in the current study, it is expected that those who carry
positive attitudes towards baseball and those who have significant
others who positively evaluate baseball consumption will have intentions
to consume baseball. Thus, the following hypotheses are presented:
H5: African American viewers who carry more positive attitudes
towards baseball will have stronger behavioral intentions to consume
baseball.
H6: African American viewers who have more positive subjective
norms towards baseball will have stronger behavioral intentions to
consume baseball.
Method
Participants
Participants who agreed to take part in this study were asked to
read and answer various questions related to the dependent variables,
and concluded their participation by providing their demographic
information in the final section of the questionnaire. The demographic
makeup of the study sample (N = 283) was 70.7% males and 29.3% females,
with a racial breakdown that was entirely made up of self-identified
African Americans. Study participants who were 18-25 years of age
comprised 36.4% of the sample, while 26-34 year-olds made up 49.8%,
35-54 year-olds made up 9.9%, 55-64 year-olds made up 3.2%, and less
than one percent of the participants were over 65 years old.
Procedure
The extant literature on African American sport consumption
emphasizes two important findings: African American consumers are more
likely to consume a sport that depicts culturally significant
motivational characteristics (i.e., excitement and skill; Armstrong,
2002b; Brown & Bennett, 2015), as well as a sense of racial
similarity (Armstrong, 2002a, 2002b). In comparison to other racial
groups, the factors--excitement and skill--are particularly relevant to
African Americans. Studies suggest that if these factors are not
present, African Americans will be less likely to partake in sport
consumption (Armstrong, 2002b; Brown & Bennett, 2015). As such,
these factors are utilized within the current study to examine their
relationship with similarity and consequently, consumption. To measure
the effects of these aspects of sport as they relate to sport
advertising, we utilized them in a series of print advertisements to
investigate the extent to which they were able to prompt a perceived
sense of fit and, subsequently, affect the following dependent
variables: participants' attitudes towards baseball, subjective
norms, and behavioral intentions. An experimental study design was
employed to assess whether the presence of excitement and skill in an
advertisement setting, and the use of advertisement actors with
noticeably similar racial features, affected participants'
responses to survey items measuring the aforementioned dependent
variables. In order to ensure participants were consciously
acknowledging both the excitement and skill aspects (or lack thereof)
within the advertisements, and the racial makeup of the actors within
the advertisements, a manipulation check was conducted. The manipulation
check required the participants to properly identify whether an
advertisement contains excitement and skill. In addition, the
manipulation check required the participants to properly identify the
race of the actors within the advertisements.
The investigators utilized a relatively new online platform,
Amazon's Mechanical Turk (MTurk), to recruit participants to take
part in the experiment. Amazon's MTurk is a "market in which
anyone can post tasks to be completed and specify prices paid for
completing them," and the purpose of the system "was to have
human users complete simple tasks that would otherwise be extremely
difficult (if not impossible) for computers to perform" (Kittur,
Chi, & Suh, 2008, p. 453). Tasks typically require little time and
effort on the part of the participants, and participants make up a
population of over 500,000 individuals from 190 countries (MTurk, 2015).
Scholars are able to identify specific individuals based on attributes
ranging from race, gender, education, and social class, to name a few.
Berinsky, Huber, and Lenz (2012) found that MTurk is a viable source for
the social sciences. Specifically, Berinsky et al. (2012) examined this
online platform for internal and external validity, and found MTurk is a
valid online platform for data collection. In addition, Buhrmester,
Kwang, and Gosling (2011) assessed the MTurk platform for reliability
and data quality, and found MTurk "met or exceeded the psychometric
standards associated with published research" (p. 5). Moreover,
Paolacci, Chandler, and Ipeirotis (2010) found MTurk to be particularly
reliable for experimental data in the social sciences.
MTurk utilizes an incentive-based platform to recruit study
participants. Participants visit the MTurk website as a means to
participate in research and survey questionnaires for rewards in the
form of cash incentives that are paid directly from Amazon (researchers
pay a fee to Amazon to utilize MTurk platforms). In order to qualify for
this study, study participants were required to be self-identified
African Americans. Qualified participants were then randomly assigned to
one of four experimental conditions, which were each accompanied by
identical study directions and questionnaire items.
Study directions asked participants to provide information
regarding their demographic makeup (e.g., age, race, gender) and
previous baseball consumption history (i.e., "How often do you
watch baseball games on television?"). Participants were then asked
to view a print baseball advertisement embedded in the online survey and
answer questions in conjunction with viewing the advertisement.
Participants viewed one of four baseball advertisements created by one
specially trained graphic designer for the purposes of examining the
aforementioned research hypotheses. Specifically, participants were
randomly assigned to see one of the four following print baseball
advertisements: (a) an Anglo American baseball player making a skilled
play and depicted in an exciting setting ([Ad.sub.a]), (b) an African
American baseball player making a skilled play and shown in an exciting
setting ([Ad.sub.b], (c) an Anglo American baseball player shown in a
non-active scene (i.e., unexciting, no demonstration of skill)
([Ad.sub.c]), and (d) an African American baseball player portrayed in a
non-active scene (i.e., unexciting, no demonstration of skill)
([Ad.sub.d]). Additionally, the advertisements were purposely designed
to emphasize the product of baseball, as opposed to advertising a
specific team, brand, or event. This measure was taken to remove any
confounding issues that may otherwise have surfaced had we included
added copy (Golden, 1979). Visuals of the advertisements used for each
of the conditions are provided in the Appendix. In total, 72
participants viewed [Ad.sub.a,] 78 separate participants viewed
[Ad.sub.b], 91 separate participants viewed [Ad.sub.c], and 82 separate
participants viewed [Ad.sub.d.]
Measures
Perceived fit (i.e., match-up) between the advertisement and viewer
was measured using a three-item, seven-point Likert-type scale developed
by Dahlen (2005), who adapted items from Lange, Selander, and
Aberg's (2003) congruency scale ([alpha] = .81). A three-item scale
developed by Cunningham and Kwon (2003) was used to assess
participants' attitudes toward the sport of baseball ([alpha] =
.90). Responses were reported on a semantic differential, seven-point
Likert-type scale (i.e., extremely unpleasant to extremely pleasant,
extremely dull to extremely entertaining, and extremely worthless to
extremely valuable). Subjective norms were measured using Ajzen's
(2002) three-item, seven-point Likert-type scale that ranged from
strongly disagree to strongly agree ([alpha] = .91) (i.e., "Most
people who are close to me believe I would benefit from consuming
baseball"). A four-item, seven-point Likert-type scale ranging from
strongly disagree to strongly agree was adapted from Cunningham and Kwon
(2003) to assess intentions to consume baseball (i.e., "After
observing this advertisement, I would enjoy consuming baseball in the
near future."). A detailed summary of scale items is provided in
Table 1.
Based on recommendations put forth by Nunnally and Bernstein
(1994), the scales used in this research were above the required
threshold of .80 and, therefore, considered reliable. The scales were
further validated by measuring composite reliability and average
variance extracted in accordance with Hair, Black, Babin, and Anderson
(2010), who argue scores greater than .50 are necessary for construct
validity. The conditions were satisfied, as each of the factors showed
composite reliability and average variance extracted values greater than
.80 and .60, respectively.
Data Analysis
Structural equation modeling was used to test the aforementioned
series of hypotheses (see Figure 1 for an illustrative summary of study
predictions). The fit indices that were utilized to measure the goodness
of fit of the hypothesized model included the root mean square error of
approximation (RMSEA), the comparative fit index (CFI), and the
Tucker-Lewis index (TLI). In adhering to recommendations of Hu and
Bentler (1999) and Kelloway (1998), RMSEA values of less than .05, CFI
values greater than .95, and TLI values greater than .90 were considered
to be suggestive of acceptable model fit. Following the suggestions of
Marsh, Wen, and Hau (2004), moderation analysis was conducted whereby
race was used to create the setting x race product.
Results
Manipulation Check
As a way of assessing the experiment's manipulation, study
participants were asked to establish whether the advertisement setting
depicted a sense of excitement and skill (e.g., "Is this
advertisement exciting," and "Does the actor in this
advertisement seemed skilled?"). Moreover, study participants were
asked to assess if the actors in the advertisement were African American
or Anglo American (e.g., "What race are the actors in this
advertisement?"). A total of 72 study participants viewed
[Ad.sub.a] (excitement and skill/Anglo American), of which 73% (n = 53)
reported that the advertisement portrayed excitement and skill, and
believed that the advertisement actors were Anglo American. A total of
78 study participants viewed [Ad.sub.b] (excitement and skill/African
American), of which 95% (n = 74) responded that the advertisement was
exciting and featured a skilled African American actor. A total of 91
study participants observed [Ad.sub.c] (no excitement or skill/Anglo
American), of which 89% (n = 81) responded that the advertisement did
not depict a sense of excitement and skill, and featured actors who were
Anglo American. A total of 82 study participants viewed [Ad.sub.d] (no
excitement or skill/African American), of which 92% (n = 75) stated that
the advertisement did not depict a sense of excitement and skill, and
that actors were African American. Nearly all responses to the
manipulation check questions supported the conditions the researchers
intended for experimentation. Only those participants who fulfilled the
manipulation check (i.e., study participants who perceived the setting
and race as intended by authors for their respective advertisement
condition) were included in the subsequent stage of data analysis.
Descriptive Statistics
Means, standard deviations, and bivariate correlations for latent
variable were computed in order to understand the relationships between
these variables (see Table 2). Findings indicated participants who
perceived having a better fit with the advertisement (r = .55, p <
.01), expressed more positive attitudes toward baseball (r = .64, p <
.01), reported more favorable responses with regards to subjective norms
(r = .59, p < .01), and had stronger intentions to consume baseball
in the future (r = .64, p < .01).
Hypothesis Testing
Hypotheses were tested through observed path analysis, and findings
indicated that the model was an acceptable fit to the data: [chi
square]/df = 2.82; RMSEA (90% CI: .07, .09) = .08; CFI = .95; TLI = .94.
An illustrative summary of the tested model is presented in Figure 2.
Hypothesis 1 predicted that an advertisement setting depicting
characteristics of excitement and skill would be positively related to
perceived fit. This was not supported at the .05 alpha level, as the
results indicated setting did not significantly affect viewers'
perceptions of fit with the advertisement ([beta] = .061, p = .4).
However, our second hypothesis (Hypothesis 2), which predicted that
actor race would moderate the relationship between advertisement setting
and perceived fit, was supported. This effect was qualified by a
significant setting x race interaction ([beta] = -.178, p < .05).
Findings revealed that, among participants who observed baseball
advertisements with Anglo American actors, there were no significant
differences between the two advertisement settings with regard to
perceived fit (i.e., the exciting/skill condition had the same degree of
fit as the unexciting/no skill condition). Among participants who viewed
advertisements with African American actors, however, there were
significant differences between the two different setting
conditions--study participants in the exciting/skill condition who
viewed African American actors in the baseball advertisement conveyed
significantly higher levels of perceived fit with the advertisement than
did participants in the same condition who viewed Anglo American actors
in the advertisement. These interaction effects support Hypothesis 2.
Hypotheses 3 and 4 predicted a positive relationship between
perceived fit and attitudes towards baseball and subjective norms,
respectively. Both hypotheses were confirmed. Findings indicated that
perceived fit had a significant effect on participants' attitudes
towards baseball ([beta] = .704, p < .001), and on participants'
subjective norms ([beta] = .690, p < .001). With regard to the
principles on which the theory of reasoned action is based, Hypothesis 5
and Hypothesis 6 both predicted participants' favorable attitudes
towards baseball and positive subjective norms about baseball would
influence future behavioral intentions to consume baseball. Results
revealed that while Hypothesis 5 was not confirmed, Hypothesis 6 was
confirmed. Though findings indicate positive effects for attitudes
toward baseball on future intentions to consume, this effect was not
found to be significant ([beta] = .030, p = .78). For subjective norms,
however, this effect was found to be significant ([beta] = .887, p <
.001).
Discussion and Implications
Drawing from the literature concerning the match-up hypothesis,
this research examined whether specific similar characteristics of a
print baseball advertisement (i.e., actor race and advertisement
setting) would influence the African American participants involved in
this study to perceive various degrees of fit with the advertisement.
More specifically, a primary objective of this study was to determine if
African American participants would perceive a greater overall fit with
a baseball advertisement if the actors and settings shown in the
advertisement resembled their racial and cultural traits. Prior
researchers support the notion that settings and actors in
advertisements should not only match consumers' perceptions of an
advertised product or brand, but also fit with consumers'
self-identifying qualities and characteristics (see Cunningham et al.,
2008; Kamins, & Gupta, 1994; Kim & Cheong, 2011; Liu et al.,
2007). For example, Boyd and Shank (2004) found matching gender
qualities between advertisement actors and advertisement viewers led to
greater overall opinions of the advertisement. Similarly, Kim and Cheong
(2011) found that using advertisement actors' race to appeal to a
particular minority group (e.g., Asian American actors in a golf
advertisement to appeal to potential Asian American golf consumers) led
to greater perceptions of fit regarding many aspects of an advertisement
as opposed to when Anglo American actors were used. In this
investigation, we illustrate how advertisement settings portraying
excitement and skill (i.e., characteristics of sport African American
consumers most commonly seek; Armstrong 2002a, 2002b) significantly
influenced African American viewers' perceived fit with the
advertisement, but only when African American actors were portrayed in
the advertisement.
Support for our second hypothesis indicates that actor or endorser
race is a particularly significant moderating factor for affecting
consumers' perceptions of fit with an advertisement. This is
supportive of literature suggesting individuals who have a higher sense
of racial similarity will be more likely to consume products that
reinforce and exhibit their own racial traits (Brown & Bennett,
2015; Cunningham & Kwon, 2004; Xu, Shim, Lotz, & Almeida, 2004).
Further, support for the moderating effects of actor race on perceived
fit upholds scholars' assertions that individuals are likely to
display positive assessments towards individuals who are perceived as
being similar on a surface-level self-identity (Dovidio, Gaertner, &
Kawakami, 2003). Hence, a major implication of this study is that of the
perspective of race and diversity in sport marketing, as racial elements
seem to have the ability to significantly influence perceptions of
advertisements among minority group consumers.
Our results also offer some meaningful insights for comparable
studies in the field of reasoned action research in that they support
previous findings related to the impact of attitudes and subjective
norms on one's behaviors and intentions (Ajzen & Fishbein,
1980). Given the significant findings related to Hypothesis 3 and
Hypothesis 4 in this research (i.e., perceived fit was positively
related to participants' attitudes towards baseball and subjective
norms about the sport), our results also demonstrate that intentions to
consume baseball were strengthened when participants' attitudes
towards baseball and subjective norms about baseball were both positive.
These results are similar to prior research showing attitudes and
subjective norms having direct effects upon intentions (Ajzen &
Daigle, 2001; Ajzen & Driver, 1992; Lu, Lin, & Cheng, 2011).
Though subjective norms were meaningful in their prediction of
intentions to consume baseball (Hypothesis 6), our results indicate that
attitudes towards baseball were not (Hypothesis 5). This finding is
noteworthy given that previous research has demonstrated the influence
of attitudes on sport consumption (e.g., Cunningham & Kwon, 2003;
Cheng et al., 2012), and relatively fewer studies have explored the
effect of attitudes on sport consumption where African Americans are
concerned (e.g., Brown & Bennett, 2015; Stadler Blank et al., 2014).
Though future research on the topic is warranted, this result may
showcase the surface-level nature of the attitude variable, one which
may not aptly encompass the multi-layered dynamics influencing African
American sport consumption. In fact, Armstrong (2008) infers that
African Americans are different from other consumers in that their
consumption habits may be preceded by deeper-level, psychosocial
elements relating to culture. To this end, Armstrong (2008) contends
sports ought to be "imbued with artifacts and expressions that have
sociocultural salience to consumers of color," and should provide
"a sense of cultural congruity, or a culturally rewarding
experience that may differ from the experience perceived by Caucasian
consumers in the same setting" (p. 228). This perspective
articulates the multilayered dynamics associated with African American
sport consumption and offers insights for future research on factors
affecting attitudes.
Perhaps one of the more meaningful implications for the study is
that subjective norms, more so than attitudes, were significant
predictors of future intentions to consume baseball for our sample of
African Americans (Hypothesis 6). In reviewing previous research, it
seems that subjective norms are particularly important to African
American sport consumers, most notably due to the importance of
psychosocial involvement of individuals in varied parts of culture and
society (e.g., Cohen, Brown, & Welty Peachey, 2012). Psychosocial
involvement refers to the ability of individuals to be influenced by
family, friends, or significant others (Armstrong, 2002a). Armstrong
states that minorities will participate (or not participate) in certain
behaviors if other members of their culture also participate (or not
participate), and will do so at a greater speed of adoption (or
rejection) than members of majority groups. The result is a kind of
ripple effect, as individuals who are part of the minority group will
adhere to the sentiments of others like them and further convey their
negative or positive sentiments about a certain behavior. Our results
support Armstrong's notions. That is, when viewing other African
Americans portrayed in a baseball advertisement, study participants may
have felt more psychosocially involved with the sport.
Because the study of African American sport consumers is still in
its relative infancy, extant research on this minority group with
regards to sport consumption is still advancing and research designs are
still largely qualitative and grounded from a theoretical standpoint
(Brown & Bennett, 2015; Stadler Blank et al., 2014). There are many
possible reasons for the dearth of sport marketing literature on this
important minority group. For example, the lack of studies on African
American sport consumers might perhaps be due to the fact sport
management research has only begun to fully inquire and study these
consumers in recent decades (see Singer, 2005). In addition, because
recruiting large sample sizes of African American research participants
is a relatively difficult and oftentimes unachievable task (see Coker,
Huang, & Kashubeck-West, 2011), perhaps research on African American
sport consumers in sport management literature has been qualitative
because of limited access to these consumers and subsequently small
sample sizes. In this study, the authors were able to utilize new data
collection techniques via Amazon's MTurk, which allowed for a large
enough sample size of African Americans to conduct a thorough SEM
analysis. As such, this research advances current findings related to
African Americans and sport consumption by presenting a series of
testable hypotheses that shed light on novel ways in which sport
marketers might employ the influence of race and settings in
advertisements to attract more African American consumers to the sport
of baseball.
Limitations and Future Research
While this study reveals important information for both
academicians and practitioners, it is not without limitations. First,
data collection methods involved a relatively new online platform.
Although Amazon's MTurk has been found to be a viable resource for
collecting data within the social sciences (Berinsky et al., 2012), the
platform has not been widely utilized within the sport management field.
MTurk was reliable in its capacity to provide a sizable amount of
participants; however, the presence of a predominately male sample
(70.7%) hinders the results where female African Americans are
concerned. Additionally, individuals who participated in this survey
were largely made up of 18-34 year olds. This is probably due to the
technological nature of MTurk--a younger demographic is more likely to
participate on such platforms. Provided just over 30% of African
Americans in the US are between the ages of 18 and 34 (Nielson, 2011),
the large sample of African Americans in this study might not
necessarily reflect the opinions of all African American consumers in
the US. However, given the hypotheses in the current study, significant
findings among a younger demographic of African American consumers might
be viewed as being even more meaningful since this age group is gaining
more spending power and having influences on friends and early family
members (e.g., spouse and children).
The findings presented here may also be limited because of the
fictitious nature of the advertisements used in the study. The addition
of recognizable baseball brand names (e.g., MLB, Rawlings) or noticeable
celebrity endorsers (as opposed to common advertisement actors) might
influence the effects of both setting and race on African American sport
consumers' perceptions. Previous researchers argue that specific
brands and endorsers portray a sense of expertise related to the product
that might influence perceived fit and behavioral intentions (Fink et
al., 2004), and actual consumption decisions (McDaniel, 1999; Ruihley,
Runyan, & Lear, 2010). Therefore, we recommend future studies
increase the real-world bearing of this research by conducting original
experiments with advertisements featuring MLB players that included
African Americans, Anglo Americans, Hispanics, and Asian Americans to
see if endorser expertise may advance the findings in this study. Also,
motion advertisements such as television commercials may be better
suited to examine different advertisement settings that, according to
our findings, are influential of viewers' behavioral intentions
(Alden et al., 1999; Pradeep, 2013). In addition, as we did not examine
potential racial hue effects, future scholars interested in deeper level
race effects might be able to glean additional insights beyond those
examined in this study. Although an examination of actors'
different racial hues would have been interesting, our hypotheses were
focused mainly on differences between Anglo American and African
American actors in baseball advertisements.
This study is seemingly one of the first to utilize the theoretical
principles of match-up hypothesis in conjunction with the theory of
reasoned action. Our results illustrate how a setting x race product
influenced study participants' perceptions of fit with the
advertisement. When fit was greater, attitudes towards baseball and
subjective norms about baseball were both positively affected. To this
end, other researchers may want to employ both theories in future
endeavors as a means to advance the current knowledge in these fields.
Within the field of sport management, both match-up hypothesis and
theory of reasoned action have been utilized in studies pertaining to
sport consumption (see Cunningham & Kwon, 2003; Fink et al., 2004;
Kim & Cheong, 2011; Shoham, Rose, & Kahle, 1998; Walker &
Kent, 2013); however, fewer studies have specifically employed both
theories to examine motivations and constraints to sport consumption.
In sum, research on African American sport consumption in sport
marketing literature has largely gone without the attention of
academicians; therefore, very little is known when it comes to
advertising to this minority group more effectively to generate positive
consumption behaviors (Brown & Bennett, 2015; Stadler Blank et al.,
2014). This void in the literature is rather notable, as Armstrong
(2008) describes African American consumers as "the new mainstream
that is transforming how America will work, learn, spend, and play in
the coming decades...they will undoubtedly impact sport marketing theory
and practice" (p. 229). Given studies and recent trends in the
sport industry show consumption of baseball by African Americans to be
decreasing every year, it is important sport marketing researchers and
practitioners assess why marketing and promotion strategies have been
unsuccessful in reaching this important target audience. This study
provides insightful implications in this regard.
Brandon Brown, PhD, is an assistant professor in the Department of
Sport Management at the University of Tampa. His research interests
include sport marketing, branding, and minority consumption habits.
Gregg Bennett, EdD, is a professor in the Department of Health and
Kinesiology and director of the Center for Sport Management Research and
Education at Texas A&M University. His research interests include
branding and effective event marketing.
Khalid Ballouli, PhD, is an assistant professor in the Department
of Sport and Entertainment Management at the University of South
Carolina. His research interests include sport consumer behavior, music
in contemporary sport, and branding.
References
Ajzen, I. (2002). Perceived behavioral control, self-efficacy,
locus of control, and the theory of planned behavior. Journal of Applied
Social Psychology, 32, 665-683.
Ajzen, I., & Daigle, J. (2001). Predicting hunting intentions
and behavior: An application of the theory of planned behavior. Leisure
Science, 23, 165-178.
Ajzen, I., & Driver, B. L. (1992). Application of the theory of
planned behavior to leisure choice. Journal of Leisure Research, 24,
207-224.
Ajzen, I., & Fishbein, M. (1980). Understanding attitudes and
predicting social behavior. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.
Alden, D. L., Steenkamp, J. B. E., & Batra, R. (1999). Brand
positioning through advertising in Asia, North America, and Europe: The
role of global consumer culture. The Journal of Marketing, 63, 75-87.
Appiah, O. (2001). Black, White, Hispanic, and Asian American
adolescents' responses to culturally embedded ads. Howard Journal
of Communication, 12, 29-48.
Armour, T. (2002, August 5). Where are the black baseball fans?
Chicago Tribune. Retrieved from http://articles.chicagotribune.com/2002-
0805/features/0208050026_1_african-american-major-league-baseball-baseball-fans
Armstrong, K. L. (2002a). An examination of the social psychology
of Blacks' consumption of sport. Journal of Sport Management, 16,
267-288.
Armstrong, K. L. (2002b). Race and sport consumption motivations: A
preliminary investigation of a Black consumers' sport motivation
scale. Journal of Sport Behavior, 25, 309-330.
Armstrong, K. L. (2008). Consumers of color and the
"culture" of sport attendance: Exploratory insights. Sport
Marketing Quarterly, 17, 218-231.
Babiak, K., Mills, B., Tainsky, S., & Juravich, M. (2012). An
investigation into professional athlete philanthropy: Why clarity is
part of the game. Journal of Sport Management, 26, 159-176.
Bell, N., & Stephenson, A. L. (2014). Variation in motivations
by running ability: Using the theory of reasoned action to predict
attitudes about running 5K races. Journal of Policy Research in Tourism,
Leisure and Events, 6, 231-247.
Berinsky, A. J., Huber, G. A., & Lenz, G. S. (2012). Evaluating
online labor markets for experimental research: Amazon.com's
Mechanical Turk. Political Analysis, 20, 351-568.
Boyd, T., & Shank, M. (2004). Athletes as product endorsers:
The effect of gender and product relatedness. Sport Marketing Quarterly,
13, 82-93.
Brown, B., & Bennett, G. (2015). "Baseball is whack!"
Exploring the lack of African American baseball consumption. Journal of
Sport & Social Issues, 39, 287-307.
Buhrmester, M., Kwang, T., & Gosling, S. D. (2011).
Amazon's Mechanical Turk: A new source of inexpensive, yet
high-quality, data. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 6, 3-5.
Cheng, C., Chen, L., Chen, M., & Lu, W. (2012). Fan
participation behavior in baseball: An application of the theory of
planned behavior. International Journal of Sports Marketing and
Sponsorship, 14, 22-33.
Clinkscales, J. (2013, April 19). How African-Americans changed the
channel on baseball. The Sportsfan Journal. Retrieved from
http://www.thesportsfanjournal.com/sports/baseball/how-african-americans- changed-the-channel-on-baseball/
Cohen, A., Brown, B., & Welty Peachey, J. (2012). The
intersection of pop culture and non-traditional sports: An examination
of the niche market of quidditch. International Journal of Sport
Management and Marketing, 12, 180-197.
Coker, A. D., Huang, H. H., & Kashubeck West, S. (2009).
Research with African Americans: Lessons learned about recruiting
African American women. Journal of Multicultural Counseling and
Development, 37, 153-165.
Corbett, J. B. (2002). A faint green sell: Advertising and the
natural world. In M. Meister & P. M Japp (Eds.), Enviropop: Studies
in environmental rhetoric and popular culture (pp. 141-160). Westport,
CT: Praeger Publishing.
Cortese, A. J. (2007). Provocateur: Images of women and minorities
in advertising. Lanham, MD: Rowman & Littlefield Publishers.
Cunningham, G. B., & Kwon, H. (2003). The theory of planned
behavior and intentions to attend a sporting event. Sport Management
Review, 6, 127-145.
Cunningham, G. B., Fink, J. S., & Kenix, L. J. (2008). Choosing
an endorser for a women's sporting event: The interaction of
attractiveness and expertise. Sex Roles, 58, 371-378.
Dahlen, M. (2005). The medium as a contextual cue: Effects of
creative media choice. Journal of Advertising, 34, 89-98.
Dovidio, J. F., Gaertner, S. L., & Kawakami, K. (2003).
Intergroup contact: The past, present, and the future. Group Processes
& Intergroup Relations, 6, 5-21.
Dyer, G. (2008). Advertising as communication. Oxford, UK:
Routledge.
Erdogan, B. Z. (1999). Celebrity endorsement: A literature review.
Journal of Marketing Management, 15, 291-314.
Fink, J., Cunningham, G. B., & Kensicki, L. J. (2004).
Utilizing athletes as endorsers to sell women's sport:
Attractiveness versus expertise. Journal of Sport Management, 18,
350-367.
Fishbein, M., & Ajzen, I. (1975). Belief, attitudes, and
behavior: An introduction to theory and research. Reading, MA:
Addison-Wesley.
Fishbein, M., & Ajzen, I. (2010). Predicting and changing
behavior: The reasoned action approach. New York, NY: Psychology Press.
Funk, D. C., & James, J. D. (2004). The fan attitude network
(FAN) model: Exploring attitude formation and change among sport
consumers. Sport Management Review, 7, 1-26.
Funk, D. C., & Pastore, D. L. (2000). Equating attitudes to
allegiance: The usefulness of selected attitudinal information in
segmenting loyalty to professional sports teams. Sport Marketing
Quarterly, 9, 175-184.
Glassman, L., & Albarracin, D. (2006). Forming attitudes that
predict future behavior: A meta-analysis of the attitude-behavior
relation. Psychological Bulletin, 132, 778-822.
Golden, L. L. (1979). Consumer reactions to explicit brand
comparisons in advertisements. Journal of Marketing Research, 16,
517-532.
Hagger, M. S., Chatzisarantis, N., & Biddle, S. J. H. (2001).
The influence of self-efficacy and past behavior on the physical
activity intentions of young people. Journal of Sports Sciences, 19,
711-725.
Hair Jr., J. F., Black, W. C., Babin, B., & Anderson, R.
(2010). Multivariate data analysis (7th ed). Upper Saddle River, NJ:
Pearson Education.
Hall, C. (2013). White, male and middle class: Explorations in
feminism and history. Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley & Sons.
Hu, L. T., & Bentler, P. M. (1999). Cutoff criteria for fit
indexes in covariance structure analysis: Conventional criteria versus
new alternatives. Structural Equation Modeling: A Multidisciplinary
Journal, 6, 1-55.
Kahle, L. R., & Homer, P. M. (1985). Physical attractiveness of
the celebrity endorser: A social adaptation perspective. Journal of
Consumer Research, 11, 954-961.
Kamins, M. A. (1990). An investigation into the match-up hypothesis
in celebrity advertising: When beauty may only be skin deep. Journal of
Advertising, 19, 4-13.
Kamins, M. A., & Gupta, K. (1994). Congruence between
spokesperson and product type: A matchup hypothesis perspective.
Psychology and Marketing, 11, 569-586.
Kaplanidou, K., & Gibson, H. (2012). Differences between first
time and repeat spectator tourists of a youth soccer event: Intentions
and image approaches. Current Issues in Tourism, 15, 477-487.
Kelloway, E. K. (1998). Using Lisrel for structural equation
modeling. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications.
Kelman, H. C. (1961). Processes of opinion change. Public Opinion
Quarterly, 25, 57-78.
Kerin, R. A. (1979). Black model appearance and product
evaluations. Journal of Communication, 29, 123-128.
Kim, K., & Cheong, Y. (2011). The effects of athlete-endorsed
advertising: The moderating role of the athlete-audience ethnicity
match. Journal of Sport Management, 25, 143-155.
Kittur, A., Chi, E. H., & Suh, B. (2008). Crowdsourcing user
studies with Mechanical Turk. In Proceedings of the SIGCHI conference on
human factors in computing systems (pp. 453-456). New York, NY: ACM.
Lange, F., Selander, S., & Aberg, C. (2003). When weaker brands
prevail. Journal of Product & Brand Management, 12, 6-21.
Liu, M. T., Huang, Y., & Minghua, J. (2007). Relations among
attractiveness of endorsers, match-up, and purchase intention in sport
marketing in China. Journal of Consumer Marketing, 24, 358-365.
Livingstone, S. (2011, July 17). Curtis Granderson: Where are all
the black fans? USA Today. Retrieved from
http://content.usatoday.com/communities/dailypitch/post/2011/07/yanees-curtis- granderson-black-fans-mlb-lacking/1
Lu, W., Lin, S., & Cheng, C. (2011). Sports spectator behavior:
A test of the theory of planned behavior. Perceptual and Motor Skills,
113, 1017-1026.
Madden, T. J., Ellen, P. S., & Ajzen, I. (1992). A comparison
of the theory of planned behavior and the theory of reasoned action.
Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 18, 3-9.
Marsh, H. W., Wen, Z., & Hau, K. T. (2004). Structural equation
models of latent interactions: Evaluation of alternative estimation
strategies and indicator construction. Psychological Methods, 9,
275-300.
McDaniel, S. R. (1999). An investigation of match-up effects in
sport sponsorship advertising: The implications of consumer advertising
schemas. Psychology and Marketing, 16, 163-184.
Misra, S., & Beatty, S. E. (1990). Celebrity spokesperson and
brand congruence: An assessment of recall and affect. Journal of
Business Research, 21, 159-173.
MTurk. (2015, October 23). Distribute your work to thousands of
works instantly. Retrieved from https://requester.mturk.com/tour
Nielson. (2011). Nielson report: The state of the African-American
consumer. Retrieved from http://www.nielsen.com/us/en/insights/
reports/2011/state-of-the-african-american-consumer.html
Nielson. (2013). African-American consumers are more relevant than
ever. Retrieved from http://www.nielsen.com/us/en/insights/news/
2013/african-american-consumers-are-more-relevant-than-ever.html
Nunnally, J., & Bernstein, I. H. (1994). Psychometric theory
(3rd ed.). New York, NY: McGraw-Hill.
Ogden, D. C., & Hilt, M. L. (2003). Collective identity and
basketball: An explanation for the decreasing number of
African-Americans on America's Baseball diamonds. Journal of
Leisure Research, 35, 213-227.
Ohanian, R. (1990). Construction and validation of a scale to
measure celebrity endorsers' perceived expertise, trustworthiness,
and attractiveness. Journal of Advertising, 19, 39-52.
Oyewole, P. (2007). Fast food marketing and the African American
consumers: The impact of socio-economic and demographic characteristics.
Journal of International Consumer Marketing, 19, 75-108.
Paolacci, G., Chandler, J., & Ipeirotis, P. G. (2010). Running
experiments on Amazon Mechanical Turk. Judgment and Decision Making, 5,
411-419.
Pradeep, A. (2013). U.S. Patent Application 14/023,323. Washington,
DC: U.S. Patent and Trademark Office.
Ruihley, B. J., Runyan, R. C., & Lear, K. E. (2010). The use of
sport celebrities in advertising: A replication and extension. Sport
Marketing Quarterly, 19, 132-142.
Schlinger, M. J., & Plummer, J. T. (1972). Advertising in black
and white. Journal of Marketing Research, 9, 149-153.
Sheppard, B. H., Hartwick, J., & Warshaw, P. R. (1988). The
theory of reasoned action: A meta-analysis of past research with
recommendations for modifications and future research. Journal of
Consumer Research, 15, 325-343.
Shoham, A., Rose, G. M., & Kahle, L. R. (1998). Marketing of
risky sports: From intention to action. Journal of the Academy of
Marketing Science, 26, 307-321.
Simpson, E. M., Snuggs, T., Christiansen, T., & Simples, K. E.
(2000). Race, homophily, and purchase intentions and the Black consumer.
Psychology and Marketing, 17, 877-899.
Singer, J. N. (2005). Addressing epistemological racism in sport
management research. Journal of Sport Management, 19, 464-479.
SportsBusiness Daily. (2010, June 9). Fan demographics among major
North American sports leagues. Retrieved from
http://www.sportsbusinessdaily.com/Daily/Issues/2010/06/Issue-185/The-Back-Of- The-Book/Fan-
Demographics-Among-Major-North-American-Sports-Leagues.aspx
Stadler Blank, A., Sweeney, K., & Fuller, R. D. (2014). Room
for growth in professional sport: An examination of the factors
affecting African-American attendance. Sport Marketing Quarterly, 23,
225-240.
Till, B. D., & Busler, M. (2000). The match-up hypothesis:
Physical attractiveness, expertise, and the role of fit on brand
attitude, purchase intent and brand beliefs. Journal of Advertising,
29(3), 1-13.
U.S. Census Bureau. (2011). The Black population: 2010. Retrieved
from http://www.census.gov/prod/cen2010/briefs/c2010br-06.pdf
Walker, M., & Kent, A. (2013). The roles of credibility and
social consciousness in the corporate philanthropy-consumer behavior
relationship. Journal of Business Ethics, 116, 341-353.
Address author corespondence to:
Brandon Brown
University of Tampa
401 W Kennedy Blvd--Box 18F
Tampa, FL 33606
Email: bbrown@ut.edu
Appendix
Advertisement A. Action/Anglo American
Advertisement B. Action/African American
Advertisement C. No action/Anglo American
Advertisement D. No action/African American
Table 1
Standardized Regression Weights for Scale Items of Latent Variables
Scale Items Estimates
Perceived Fit (b) a. The advertisement fits well .85
with who I am as a person.
b. The advertisement fits well .88
with the image I have of myself.
c. The advertisement fits well .93
with the person I want to be.
(Based on Dahlen, 2005)
Attitudes Towards a. How pleasant of an experience .86
Baseball (a) is consuming baseball?
b. How would you describe baseball .88
in terms of entertainment?
c. What is the value of baseball .84
to you personally?
(Based on Cunningham and Kwon,
2003)
Subjective Norms (b) a. People important to me .89
(friends/family) would approve of
me consuming baseball.
b. Consuming baseball with people .87
close to me (friends/family) is
something I plan to do.
c. People close to me .89
(friends/family) believe I would
benefit from consumer baseball.
(Based on Ajzen, 2002)
Intentions to a. I intend to consume baseball .94
Consume Baseball (b) sometime in the near future.
b. I would enjoy consuming .95
baseball sometime in the near
future.
c. I have the resources .93
(transportation, TV, Internet) to
consumer baseball in the future.
d. I will try to consume baseball .95
sometime in the near future.
(Based on Cunningham and Kwon,
2003)
(a) Items were measured using a 7-point semantic differential scale.
(b) Items were measured using a 7-point Likert-type (1 = strongly
disagree, 7 = strongly agree).
Table 2
Means, Standard Deviations, and Bivariate Correlations for the Latent
Variables
M SD 1 2 3 4
1. Perceived fit 5.02 1.30 --
2. Attitudes towards 5.32 1.15 .64** --
baseball
3. Subjective norms towards 5.17 1.16 .59** .73** --
baseball
4. Intentions to consume 5.13 1.32 .64** .69** .78** --
baseball
** Correlation is significant at the 0.01 level (two-tailed)
COPYRIGHT 2016 Fitness Information Technology Inc.
No portion of this article can be reproduced without the express written permission from the copyright holder.
Copyright 2016 Gale, Cengage Learning. All rights reserved.