Increasing Visitor Engagement During Interpretive Walking Tours.
Douglas, David ; Ellis, Gary ; Lacanienta, Andrew 等
Increasing Visitor Engagement During Interpretive Walking Tours.
Increasing Engagement During Heritage Interpretation Talks
Engagement is pivotal to the overall quality of visitor experiences
at heritage sites (e.g., Beck & Cable, 1998; Moscardo, 1996, 1999;
Sutcliffe & Kim, 2014; Tilden, 1977). Engagement, a motivational
state of focused involvement in learning, has been associated with
numerous benefits. Among these are increased attention, active
participation, enjoyment, enthusiasm, and lack of anxiety (Reeve, 2012).
Researchers have found that greater levels of engagement promote
curiosity, exploration, discovery, and ultimately greater levels of
learning and satisfaction (Deci & Ryan, 2002; Ellis, Freeman, Jamal,
& Jiang, 2017; Moscardo, 1999). Interestingly though, engagement is
one of the least researched aspects of active participation (Fredricks,
Bohnert, & Burdette, 2014).
Interpreters use a variety of strategies to promote engagement. One
of these strategies is using props and cues inherent to the site. Props
and cues advance a pervasive theme (Lacanienta, Ellis, Taggart, Wilder,
& Carroll, 2018; Pine & Gilmore, 2011) and elevate
visitors' engagement in the story. Compared to a tour with a guide
in contemporary clothing and modern idiom, a tour with a guide in period
costume and period idiom provides added novelty, coherence, and richness
to the experience (Pine & Gilmore, 2011). Coherent themes allow for
a more focused processing of information (Kaplan & Kaplan, 1989;
Kaplan, 1995; Kaplan & Talbot, 1983).
Interpreters must also attend to how they tell the story. One
strategy is by using provocative questions (Ham, 1992; King, 2002;
Tilden, 1977). Little research attention has been paid to what kinds of
questions stimulate visitor engagement in an interpretive story.
Tilden's (1977) legendary grounded inquiry into interpretation
yielded the principle that any interpretation failing to connect the
story to something within the visitor will be "sterile" (p.
9). Questions encouraging visitors to make an interpretive scene
relevant to their own lives may be more engaging. Such questions seek to
connect a current personal interest with an occurrence that might have
also been a pressing issue in the event being interpreted. Thus, the
visitor gains an empathetic and engaging sense of what one aspect of
life might have been like.
Although the above reasoning provides warrant for the argument that
a connection exists between living-history interpretation type, question
type, and visitor engagement with interpretive tours, such evidence
should not be taken as certainty. Distractions exist that may weaken the
effects of these techniques. Environmental distractions, such as weather
or crying children, may distract from the tour and decrease engagement.
Unenthusiastic interpreters lacking animation and voice inflection can
also inhibit the quality of interpretive experiences. Further, the
structured use of questions in interpretive settings may not increase
visitor engagement. Although such questioning techniques have
demonstrated to be impactful in classroom settings (e.g., King, 2002)
they may not be effective in heritage tourism settings. It is important
to know how living-history interpretation type, question type, and
familiarity are related to levels of engagement in heritage tours. This
study examined the effect of living-history interpretation type (i.e.,
first-person, third-person, none), question type (i.e., relevant,
dissonant, customary), and familiarity with the context of the site
being interpreted on visitor engagement during a walking tour at a
heritage site.
Literature Review
Engagement
The idea of engagement was constructed from the foundation of the
idea of mindfulness (Langer, 2000). The resulting measure was consistent
with contemporary approaches to measuring engagement in that its
elements embraced cognitive, behavioral, and emotional engagement
(Fredricks, Bohnert, & Burdette, 2014; Reeve, 2012). The mindfulness
foundation is a departure from traditional information-processing
psychology. Cognitive psychologists have noted that, unless motivated
otherwise, humans are cognitive misers (Dunn & Risko, 2016; Kaplan,
1995; Langer, 2000). Rather than maximizing rewards through full and
effortful information processing, humans frequently use heuristics to
cut down on the effort needed to process information. In one approach to
describing this process of using heuristics, Langer proposed that people
often use scripts to perform familiar tasks. In doing so, they perform
tasks by running the script without much thought in a way that might be
described as mindless (Langer, 2014).
A second group of psychologists proposed cognitive misering is
performed through what they termed environmental cognitive sets (Duvall,
2013; Leff, Gordon, & Ferguson, 1974). Such sets are ways of
perceiving one's surroundings based on specific types of processed
information that form an integrated collective set. Applying such a set
to a place allows people to make sense of their surroundings with little
effort. Subsequent work has led to conceptualization of cognitive
misering as: (1) searching for cognitive simplicity rather than
utilizing complex cognitive strategies; (2) seeking a single explanation
for an event rather than to look for multiple causes and; (3) rejecting
the opportunity to consider more information when given the opportunity.
Taking the elements of scripts, cognitive sets, and minimal
information processing, Langer (2014) formed the
Mindfulness-Mindlessness (M-M) theory suggesting much of what people do
in everyday settings is processed mindlessly. She defined mindlessness
as:
Behavior that is over-determined by the past... when mindless, one
relies on categories and distinctions derived in the past. Mindlessness
is single-minded reliance on information without an active awareness of
alternative perspectives or alternative uses to which the information
could be put. When mindless, the individual relies on structures that
have been appropriated from another source (Langer, Hatem, Joss, &
Howell 1989, p. 140).
Conversely, mindfulness is described as:
A state of mind that results from drawing novel distinctions, examining
information from new perspectives, and being sensitive to context. When
we are mindful, we recognize that there is not a single optimal
perspective, but many possible perspectives on the same situation
(Langer, 1993, p. 44).
Mindfulness, or total focus on the present moment, is
characteristic of such heightened states of experience as flow
(Csikszentmihalyi, 1992), ecstasy (Maslow, 1962), deep play (Ackerman,
2011), and deep structured experience (Ellis et al., 2017).
Mindfulness has also been described as "a mode of functioning
through which the individual actively engages in reconstructing the
environment through creating new categories or distinctions, thus
directing attention to new contextual cues that may be consciously
controlled" (Alexander, Langer, Newman, Chandler, & Davies,
1989, p. 951). People who are mindful actively process information. They
think and question the world before them. The process of drawing novel
distinctions can lead to increased sensitivity to one's
environment, increased openness to new information, the creation of new
categories for constructing perception, and an increased awareness of
multiple perspectives in problem solving.
Mindfulness is particularly appropriate for examining engagement
(Langer, 2009). Mindfulness is used to explain human reactions to
interpretation and museum experiences by a variety of scholars (Ablett
& Dyer, 2009; Frauman, 2010; Kang & Getzel, 2012; Moscardo,
2009, 2014, 2017). Interpretation encourages more complex thought
related to elaboration, mindfulness, and engagement (Ballantyne, Packer,
& Falk, 2011; Moscardo, 2014; Walker & Moscardo, 2014).
Consequently, learning increases and changes to behavior and attitude
may take place.
Engagement is a motivational phenomenon. Motivation energizes and
shapes human behavior (e.g., Reeve, 2014). Humans are motivated to
engage in tasks or enterprises that give them opportunities to fulfill
needs or goals. In this vein, motivation is a complex construct that has
three components. The first component of motivation reflects actions and
time spent on task. The second is an emotional component wherein
engagement (i.e., interest, curiosity, and enjoyment) is manifest in the
activity. The third component is orientation, where beliefs about
opportunities resulting from activities affect our experience of the
activity. Engagement is thus a state of motivation containing elements
of interest, curiosity, and enjoyment (Reeve, 2012; 2014). Engagement
yields a deep and focused experience in an interpretive activity.
Living-History Interpretation
Living-history interpretation provides a staged experience and
immediate engagement into the past. Living-history interpretation is
"the re-creation of specific periods of the past or specific events
utilizing living interpreters usually clothed and equipped with the
correct tools and accouterments of a depicted era" (Luzader &
Spellman, 1996, p. 241). Two approaches to living-history interpretation
are widely used, first-person living history and third-person living
history.
First-person living history.
During first-person living history, interpreters assume the role of
an actual person and speak and act in character. They do not recognize
future time periods beyond that which they are representing and they
refuse to acknowledge visitors are from the present day. First-person
interpretation attempts to create a sense of time travel to a different
time, place, and set of circumstances.
First-person living history resembles a theatrical or staged
production. Audiences are invited to suspend disbelief to help create
the illusion. If the interpreter breaks out of character, coherence is
lost and the illusion ends. First-person interpretation has the
advantage of involving the visitor and communicating the essence of the
era quickly to those who have the imagination to immerse themselves in
the experience. Carrying out first-person interpretation requires
considerable skill, knowledge, and improvisation ability (Hunt, 2004).
The success of first-person interpretation depends to a great extent on
the ability of the interpreter to get into and stay in character.
First-person interpreters must conform to characters emotionally,
physically, and intellectually. In other words, an actor must become the
character (Pine & Gilmore, 2011).
Third-person living history.
During third-person living history the interpreter may use period
dress and idiom but moves freely in and out of time periods. An
interpreter might, for example, appear in period dress, but will
interact with the guests according to his or her role as the interpreter
of the 21st-century heritage site. The third-person approach enables a
rich experience for visitors who enjoy making comparisons between the
interpreted worlds and their own experiences. Third-person
interpretation may help to understand the contrast between present and
past conditions. Third-person living history tends to lead to more
complex conversations with visitors because visitors need to adjust
their understanding of the role-playing component and may experience
confusion with a guide switching in and out of time periods.
The degree of confusion present might also be related to the
visitors' degree of familiarity with the context of the
interpretive site. Visitors who have lower levels of knowledge may
experience greater engagement from the increased engagement and
theatrics of first-person interpretation, while those with greater
knowledge might enjoy third-person interpretation for its depth
facilitated by comparison between the interpretive modern experiences.
Question Types
One popular strategy in contemporary educational psychology
concerns the use of questioning types (King, 2002). Questions that
introduce uncertainty and require thoughtful processing may increase
engagement. Questions that are relevant or create dissonance may also
create curiosity and arousal and help visitors to make personal
connections. One educational questioning strategy employs an arousal
type teaching technique called conditional or ambiguous instruction.
Rather than teaching concepts with absolute certainty, instructors use
language that promotes more cognitive processing (Bruin, Meppelink,
& Bogels, 2015; Ritchart & Perkins 2000). Ambiguous situations
promote increased engagement because they demand cognition. The
individual becomes engaged by striving to make sense of the situation.
Such situations can be created through both relevance and dissonance
questions.
Relevance questions.
One of Tilden's (1977) principles of interpretation is,
"Interpretation that does not relate what is being displayed or
described to something with the personality or experience of the visitor
will be sterile" (p. 9). Relevance suggests the importance of
connecting the subject matter to the visitors' personal lives. Some
examples of relevance questions in a pioneer heritage setting follow:
* Have you ever had a time in your life where you wondered if you
would make it, like the pioneers we are discussing?
* Do you know anyone who (like the character being interpreted) is
a rough and tough character but incredibly loyal or dedicated to a
cause?
* For some pioneers the journey was too hard and they turned back.
Have you ever taken on something that was so hard that you wanted to
quit or turn back?
For many years, educators have employed a relevance questioning
technique called apperception. Apperception is the process through which
new experiences are understood through connection to previous
experiences. If interpreters have something difficult to explain, they
might first begin with something the visitor already knows. Then make a
comparison with new information. The process of making such comparisons
and drawing relevant inference from times past or unfamiliar situations
can be highly engaging.
Dissonance questions.
The theory of cognitive dissonance provides a basis for asking
dissonance questions to elicit engagement (Festinger, 1957). Cognitive
dissonance is a tension people feel when there is inconsistency between
thoughts and behaviors. People find inconsistencies uncomfortable and
are motivated to maintain consistency among their cognitions. When
people feel dissonance, it often occurs when two simultaneously
accessible thoughts or beliefs are inconsistent. Festinger argued that
to reduce this unpleasant condition, people become aroused while
struggling to adjust their thinking. Such arousal is highly engaging and
is a motivational force causing one to reduce the unpleasant arousal.
Some examples of dissonance questions in a pioneer heritage setting
follow:
* What crimes do you believe are bad enough that they are worthy of
death?
* What concerns would you have in trusting a man who has served
time in jail for attempted murder?
* If your religious leader you deeply respected instructed you to
practice plural marriage, how do you believe you would respond?
Not all dissonant situations are uncomfortable. Optimal arousal
theory (Ellis, 1973) suggests individuals may crave dissonance for its
excitement. As such, interpretive tours including dissonance questions
can be expected to yield high levels of engagement.
The Theory of Structured Experiences
Recent literature related to interpretation experiences provides
substantive evidence for the previously proposed relationships. The
Theory of Structured Experience (TSE; Ellis et al., 2017) outlines three
types of experiences: immersion, absorption, and engagement. Of
particular interest to this study is the notion of an engagement
experience defined as "a transitory condition of heightened
attention, emotion, and motivation characterized by (a) extraordinarily
high focus of attention on an unfolding narrative or story told in
words, actions, and/or music, (b) heightened emotions, and (c) agentic
inclinations" (Ellis et al., 2017, p. 7). Narrative has been
defined as a recounting of events organized in a sequence resulting in a
story (Kim, 2016). Engagement experiences also involve a story as is
ordinarily the case in interpretive talks.
Ellis et al. (2017) outlined propositions specific to engagement
experiences within the TSE: (1) as provocation increases, engagement
tends to increase, (2) as story coherence increases, engagement tends to
increase, (3) as personal relevance of the story increases, engagement
tends to increase, (4) as mindfulness on the story increases, engagement
tends to increase (Ellis et al., 2017; Reeve, 2012; Tilden, 1977). These
propositions align well with the reviewed literature and suggest that
the effect of living-history interpretation type (i.e., first- or
third-person) and question type (i.e., relevant or dissonant) on visitor
engagement may be solidly founded in the theory.
Visitors who have differing levels of familiarity with the subject
matter may also respond differently to question types and varying types
of living-history interpretation. Perhaps, for example, visitors with
greater levels of familiarity may prefer frank discussions with
interpreters about relatively complex facets of the narrative rather
than working through the theatrics of first-person living-history
interpretation. Third-person interpretation or no living-history
interpretation may thus be more effective for guests who have a high
level of familiarity. Those forms of interpretation may allow for a
certain depth of knowledge to be shared without too much acting.
Conversely, visitors with lower levels of familiarity may prefer the
novelty of the theatrical performance of an interpreter engaged in
first-person living-history interpretation. Therefore, we hypothesized
that an interaction effect exists among living-history interpretation
type, question type, and guests' familiarity with the context of
the site.
Method
Data were collected during interpreter-led walking tours at the Old
Deseret Village at This is The Place Heritage Park during the months of
April-August, 2006. This is the Place Heritage Park is a living-history
attraction that presents the story of everyday life in a typical Utah
settlement during 1847-1897. The landscape is composed of more than 40
re-created and original buildings from settlements throughout Utah.
Costumed interpreters demonstrate life as it would have been for
19th-century Utahans. During special event days, interpreters portraying
several of Utah's most prominent citizens of the period, including
Brigham Young, Mary Fielding Smith, and Orrin Porter Rockwell, can be
seen walking the streets of the Village.
Orrin Porter Rockwell was the character depicted in the
living-history tours. Rockwell was a famous and colorful Mormon lawman,
bodyguard for Mormon leaders, and scout in the Utah territory. He has
been described as a pioneer-day Samson, destroying angel, and a tough
man with a loyal heart, an iron will and an excellent aim (Dewey, 1986).
Rockwell was a gunfighter, religious enforcer, and United States Marshal
(Hollister, 1886).
Participants
The sample included 176 adult visitors. The average age of
participants was 39.7 years (SD=14.19), and 55% were female. The average
party size for the tours was 10.15 (SD=11.38). Data were collected only
from adult participants, age 18 or older.
Measurement
Engagement was conceptualized as consisting of elements of
behavior, emotion, and cognition (Fredricks, Bohnert, & Burdette,
2014), and was measured using an adaptation of Moscardo's
mindfulness scale (Frauman, 1999). Sample items included, "my
curiosity is aroused," and "my interest has been
captured." Consistent with the emotional component of Reeve's
(2012) conceptualization of emotional engagement the questionnaire
included items measuring enjoyment and lack of anxiety. Scores were
summed across responses to nine items, with response categories ranging
from 1 (very strongly disagree) to 9 (very strongly agree). Reliability
estimates for the nine-item scale ranged from .95 to .96 across
responses to six interpretive talks.
Familiarity was measured using a behavior-anchored rating scale
(e.g., Jones & George, 2006) and measured visitors' knowledge
of Later-Day Saint (LDS) history. Participants were asked to rate
themselves from 0 to 100 on their familiarity with LDS history. To
assist them in their rating, they were directed to count the numbers of
books about LDS history that they had read, the number of classes on LDS
history they had taken and number of LDS Church history sites they had
visited. Following those counts, visitors recorded a response on a
single continuum, ranging from none (scored "0") to "10
times or more" (Scored 100). Sample responses ranged from 0 (one
participant) to 100 (five participants). The mean self-reported
familiarity was 57.68 (SD=26.34).
Procedures
Through personal invitations and a poster conspicuously displayed
at the entrance to the park, visitors were invited to participate in a
45-minute walking tour of the Old Deseret Village. Tours took place May
through September of 2006. The tour was titled, "Porter
Rockwell's Guided Tour of Old Deseret." One interpreter, who
was not blind to the hypotheses and who is an authority on Latter-Day
Saint history was used for the entire data collection period.
After an initial greeting, the interpreter presented himself to
visitors in one of three roles. In the "first-person living
history" role, he introduced himself, in character and in period
dress, as Orrin Porter Rockwell. In the "third-person living
history" role, the interpreter dressed as Orrin Porter Rockwell,
but introduced himself and conducted the tour from his own perspective
as a contemporary authority on Latter-Day Saint history (e.g.,
"Porter Rockwell would have dressed in clothing much like what I am
wearing"). In the "no living history" condition, the
interpreter did not use period dress and conducted the tour from his
perspective as a contemporary authority on Latter-Day Saint history.
After introducing himself, the interpreter informed visitors that
(1) they could participate fully in the study by participating in the
tour and completing a questionnaire; (2) they could choose to
participate in the tour, but not complete the questionnaires; or (3)
they could choose to have no involvement in the study whatsoever.
Participants were then given a questionnaire booklet and a pencil and
were asked to complete a measure of visitors' degrees of prior
experience with Latter-Day Saint history. Subsequently, visitors were
asked to respond to each of a series of six duplicate copies of the
nine-item engagement scale. These sheets were completed following an
interpretive talk at each of six stops on the walking tour. At each
stop, visitors listened to a three- to five-minute interpretive talk. At
the conclusion of each talk, the interpreter posed one of three types of
questions: a relevance question; a dissonance question; or a customary
question (i.e., do you have any questions?). Following any brief
discussion that occurred, visitors completed the nine-item measure of
their engagement at that stop on the tour. At the end of the tour,
visitors completed measures of satisfaction, learning, and the
interpreter's behavior.
Design and Manipulation of Study Variables
A two-factor design was used, with living-history interpretation
type as the between-subjects factor and question type as the
within-subjects factor. The between-subjects variable was living
history. That variable had three levels: (1) first-person living
history, (2) third-person living history, and (3) no living history. The
question type variable was manipulated during the interpretive walking
tours. Immediately after completing a threeto five-minute interpretive
talk at each of the six stops on the walking tour, the interpreter
either posed a question designed to evoke a search for personal
relevance among the guests (relevance), a question that was designed to
create dissonance within the guests (dissonance), or he simply asked,
"Does anyone have a question?" (customary). At one stop, for
example, the interpreter recounted a story in which Native Americans
attempted to sell two children captured from a warring tribe to Mormon
pioneers. When the pioneers refused to purchase the slaves, the Native
Americans promptly killed one of the children. Relevance and dissonance
questions associated with that interpretive story were as follows:
* Dissonance Question: How do you think the pioneers should have
dealt with people offering to sell stolen kids as slaves?
* Relevance Question: Have you ever had something stolen or know
someone who has been robbed? How did you feel and what went through you
mind?
Table 1 provides a summary of the dissonance and relevance
questions that were posed following the talk at each of the six stops on
the tour.
Following a random start, the relevance and dissonance questions
were presented in an alternating pattern. "Pattern A" began
with a relevance question and then alternated with dissonance questions
for the remainder of the stops on the tour. "Pattern B" began
with a dissonance question, and then alternated with relevance questions
for the remainder of the tour. "Pattern C" was the condition
in which the interpreter simply ended each talk with the query,
"does anyone have a question?" The purpose of alternating
provoking dissonance type questions with more comfortable relevance
questions was to keep the overall tone of the walking tour pleasant and
not overly serious in nature.
Data Analysis
Data were analyzed through linear mixed modeling procedures
(Maxwell & Delaney, 2004; Raudenbush & Bryke, 2002; West, Welch,
& Galecki, 2007). The unit of analysis was the individual
experience, within visitors. This approach involves restricted maximum
likelihood estimation of fixed effects (question type, living-history
type, familiarity, and their interactions) and random effects
(individual differences among participants) in a multi-level model.
Mixed modeling procedures provide a number of important advantages over
traditional least squares analysis of variance procedures. Among the
more notable of these is the ability to account for violation of the
assumption of compound symmetry in the covariance matrix of observations
taken at successive points in time.
Results
Descriptive Statistics
Descriptive statistics for each of the variables are presented in
Table 2.
Descriptive statistics revealed that the dissonance questions
produced the highest engagement scores in the sample data (M = 70.67, SD
= 9.30), followed by relevance questions (M = 70.12, SD = 10.15), and no
questions (M = 67.06, SD = 12.31). For the living-history conditions,
the mean for third-person living history produced the highest engagement
score (M = 71.22, SD = 9.77), followed by first-person living history (M
= 69.34, SD =10.03), and no living history (M = 66.84, SD = 12.24). The
correlation between engagement and familiarity was .20 (p < .01).
Within-group correlations were .17 (p <. 01), .12 (p = .04), and .28
(p < .01), in the relevance, dissonance, and control question types,
respectively.
The group means suggest that both the living-history interpretation
type and question type factors served to elevate visitor engagement in
the sample data. For the question type variable, the means of both the
relevance question condition (M=70.20) and the dissonance question
condition (M=71.93) were higher than the customary question condition
(M=67.79).
Four items were used as a manipulation check to measure guide
behavior. These were taken from each participant at the conclusion of
each tour. The means were consistently high, ranging from 8.02 (SD =
1.47) to 8.67 (SD = .62) on a 9-point scale. These results suggest a
high degree of consistency in the delivery of the tours and a high level
of commitment toward the interpretation.
Hypothesis Tests
Results of the mixed model analysis are presented in Table 3. A
significant three-factor interaction was found F(4, 157.76) = 4.85, p =
.001. Two plots were constructed to facilitate interpretation of that
interaction (Figure 1). Although the familiarity variable was treated as
a continuous covariate in calculation of the F ratios, that variable was
dichotomized to facilitate plotting and interpretation of the
significant interaction. Thus, Figure 1 includes separate plots for
visitors scoring below the median familiarity score (visitors with low
familiarity) and visitors scoring above the median familiarity score
(visitors with high familiarity).
Low-familiarity participants.
The plot of means of visitors with low familiarity (Figure 1) shows
highest engagement scores for visitors who experienced third-person
living history following both dissonance and relevance questions.
Low-familiarity visitors in the no living history condition reported
high levels of engagement only when dissonance questions were posed.
When relevance or customary questions were posed to low-familiarity
visitors in the no living history condition, engagement levels were
lowest among all treatment combinations. Question type did not seem to
differentially affect engagement of low-familiarity visitors who
experienced first-person living history. For that group, all scores were
clustered near the middle of the distribution, at 67.10 (the grand mean
is 69.10).
High-familiarity participants.
The pattern of means for visitors with high levels of familiarity
is quite different. When no living history is used with high-familiarity
visitors, dissonance questions seem to decrease engagement. In the
presence of both first-person and third-person living history, however,
dissonance questions seem to evoke high levels of engagement. The
opposite pattern appears to exist for relevance questions. When guests
with high levels of familiarity are exposed to no living history,
relevance questions evoke engagement. Engagement declines when relevance
questions are used along with first-person living history, and relevance
questions produce low engagement when used with high-familiarity
visitors in third-person living history. Customary questions seem to
evoke greater engagement in the no living history condition and the
third-person living history condition. A decline in engagement is
evident when customary questions are used with first-person living
history.
Discussion
The purpose of this study was to examine the effect of
living-history interpretation type (i.e., first-person, third-person,
none), question type (i.e., relevant, dissonant, customary), and
familiarity on visitor engagement during a walking tour at a heritage
site. Question type interacted with both interpretation type and level
of familiarity. Visitors with low familiarity were most engaged when
relevance or dissonance questions were asked during third-person
living-history interpretation. In contrast, high-familiarity visitors
reported highest levels of engagement in the following treatment
conditions: No living-history interpretation with relevance questions,
first-person living-history interpretation and both relevance and
dissonance questions, and third-person living-history interpretation and
dissonance questions. A decision tree based on the interaction effect is
presented in Figure 2. This heuristic model suggests interpretation
strategies that might be appropriate given visitors of high and low of
familiarity. It is important to note, however, that this table is based
on a single study at a unique heritage site. The recommendations should
be regarded as preliminary and tenuous.
This three-factor interaction is consistent with Field and
Wagar's (1976) argument that visitors are diverse and an assortment
of approaches to engagement will be required. It is also consistent with
the long-practiced technique of attempting to know one's audience
before programming begins. Should the pattern of results shown in this
study be verified with further research, it suggests best practices for
enhancing interpretation programs. For example, if at Old Deseret
Village, an interpreter knew that a participant, well-versed in
Latter-Day Saint history wanted a guided tour, he or she might pull out
all of the stops and give a first-person tour with a balance of
dissonance and relevance questions to create high levels of engagement.
However, if the interpreter, expert in the character to be portrayed,
was unavailable, a third-person tour with only dissonance questions may
also yield similarly high levels of engagement. Conversely, if the tour
group was composed of visitors interested in, but uninformed of,
Latter-Day Saint history, then a third-person tour with a balance of
dissonance and relevance questions might be most engaging. Perhaps
offering visitors with lower levels of knowledge too many engaging
activities creates information overload and thus lower levels of
engagement.
Results confirm what many interpreters have long known, namely,
heritage tour interpretation and effective question asking may increase
visitor engagement. This is salient because visitor engagement is a key
element of quality interpretive experiences (Moscardo, 1999). The
increased levels of attention, active participation, satisfaction,
learning, and enthusiasm (Deci & Ryan, 2002; Reeve, 2012) as a
result of increased engagement may allow for a richer and more
meaningful interpretation experience. Consequently, a richer
interpretation experience facilitates deeper pondering, reflection, and
mindfulness (Ballantyne, Packer, & Falk, 2011; Moscardo, 2014;
Walker & Moscardo, 2014) leading to changes in perception, behavior,
attitude, and learning.
Limitations and Future Research
Several limitations may have influenced the interpretation and
generalization of results to other populations. These limitations
include sample size, sampling techniques, and the ceiling effect
described in the previous section. The study was limited to adults age
18 or older and was not designed to afford inferences about experiences
of younger children. Although first-person living history did not
produce engagement scores that were significantly different from
third-person living history in the adult sample, a much different result
might occur in a study of experiences of children and youth. Tilden
(1977) emphasized the interpretation for children must be fundamentally
different, and such successful experience offerings seem to focus on
first-person living history for children and third-person for adults.
Finally, it is also notable that a single interpreter executed this
experiment. The important question of engagement variability across
different interpreters has not been addressed. Tilden (1977) emphasized
that interpretation is a skill that may be learned, and interpreters
with different levels of skill may produce very different levels of
guest engagement. Variation in engagement as a function of differences
in sites and contexts (e.g., museums, zoos, battlefields, rendezvous)
sites has not been studied. Additionally, accumulation of effects from
prior interpretive acts is indeed a threat to internal validity. We
might argue, though, that conducting the field experiment using the
organic procedures followed day-to-day at the heritage site would tend
to build external validity.
Further research might be directed toward three areas. First,
results of this study need to be replicated. This might be accomplished
by using different interpreters, settings, and topics. Second, research
might address the question of how tour type and question type are
related to Tilden's (1977) revelation and relate principles. The
interaction effects with levels of familiarity suggest that much needs
to be learned about how to more effectively connect to visitors of
heritage tourism sites. Lastly, interpretation research may benefit from
employing measures and examining propositions from the theory of
structured experiences (Ellis et al., 2017).
References
Ablett, P. G., & Dyer, P. K. (2009). Heritage and hermeneutics:
Towards a broader interpretation of interpretation. Current Issues in
Tourism, 12(3), 209-233.
Ackerman, D. (2011). Deep play. Vintage.
Alexander, C.N., Langer, G.J., Newman, R.I., Chandler, H.M., &
Davies, J.L. (1989). Transcendental meditation, mindfulness, and
longevity: An experimental study with the elderly. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 57, 950-964.
Ballantyne, R., Packer, J. and Falk, J. ( 2011), Visitors'
learning for environmental sustainability: testing short- and long-term
impacts of wildlife tourism experiences using structural equation
modeling, Tourism Management, Vol. 32 No. 6, pp. 1243-1252 .
Beck, L., & Cable, T. (1998). Interpretation for the 21st
century: Fifteen guiding principles for interpreting nature and culture.
Champaign, IL: Sagamore.
Brown, K. W., & Ryan, R. M. (2003). The benefits of being
present: mindfulness and its role in psychological well-being. Journal
of personality and social psychology, 84(4), 822.
Brown, K. W., Ryan, R. M., & Creswell, J. D. (2007).
Mindfulness: Theoretical foundations and evidence for its salutary
effects. Psychological inquiry, 18(4), 211-237.
Bruin, E. I., Meppelink, R., & Bogels, S. M. (2015).
Mindfulness in higher education.
Csikzentmihalyi, M. (1992), "The flow experience and its
significance for human psychology", in Csikzentmihalyi, M. and
Csikzentmihalyi, I.S. (Eds), Optimal experience: Psychological studies
of flow in consciousness, Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp.
15-35.
Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (2002) ed. Handbook of self
determination research. Rochester, NY: The University of Rochester
Press.
Dewey, R. L. (1986). Porter Rockwell: A biography. Wantagh, NY:
Paramount Books.
Dunn, T. L., & Risko, E. F. (2016). Understanding the cognitive
miser: Cue utilization in effort avoidance.
Duvall, J. (2013). Using engagement-based strategies to alter
perceptions of the walking environment. Environment and Behavior, 45(3),
303-322.
Ellis, G. D., Freeman, P. A., Jamal, T., & Jiang, J. (2017). A
theory of structured experience. Annals of Leisure Research, 1-22.
Ellis, M. J. (1973). Why people play. Englewood Cliffs, NJ:
Prentice-Hall, Inc.
Festinger, L. (1957). A theory of cognitive dissonance. Stanford,
CA: Stanford University Press.
Field, D. R. & Wagar, J. A. (1976). People and Interpretation.
In G. W. Sharp (ed). Interpreting the environment. New York: John Wiley
& Sons, Inc.
Frauman, E. (1999). The influence of Mindfulness of Information
services and sustainable management practices at coastal South Carolina
State Parks. Doctoral Dissertation, Clemson University. (UMI No.
9929721)
Frauman, E. (2010). Incorporating the concept of mindfulness in
informal outdoor education settings. The Journal of Experiential
Education, 33(3), 225-238
Fredricks, J. A., Bohnert, A. M., & Burdette, K. (2014). Moving
beyond attendance: Lessons learned from assessing engagement in
afterschool contexts. New Directions for Student Leadership, 2014(144),
45-58.
Ham, S. (1992). Environmental interpretation: A practical guide for
people with big ideas and small budgets. Golden, CO: North America
Press.
Hollister, O. J. (1886). Life of Schuyler Colfax. New York: Funk
& Wagnalls.
Hunt, S. J. (2004). Acting the part: 'living history' as
a serious pursuit. Leisure Studies, Vol. 23, No.4, 387-403, 2004.
Jones, G.R. & George, J.M. (2006). Contemporary management.
(4th ed.). Boston: McGraw-Hill Irwin.
Kang, M., & Gretzel, U. (2012). Perceptions of museum podcast
tours: Effects of consumer innovativeness, internet familiarity and
podcasting affinity on performance expectancies. Tourism Management
Perspectives, 4, 155-163.
Kaplan, R., & Kaplan, S. (1989). The experience of nature: A
psychological perspective. CUP Archive.
Kaplan, S. (1995). The restorative benefits of nature: Toward an
integrative framework. Journal of environmental psychology, 15(3),
169-182.
Kaplan, S., & Kaplan, R. (1978). Humanscape. Landscape Journal.
Kaplan, S., & Talbot, J. F. (1983). Psychological benefits of a
wilderness experience. Behavior and the natural environment, 6, 163-203.
Kim, J. H. (2016). Understanding narrative inquiry: The crafting
and analysis of stories as research. Sage publications.
King, A., (2002). Guiding knowledge construction in the classroom:
Effects of teaching children how to question and how to explain.
American Educational Research Journal, 31(2), 338-368.
Lacanienta, A., Ellis, G., Taggart, A., Wilder, J., & Carroll,
M. (2018). Does theming camp experiences lead to greater quality,
satisfaction, and promotion? Journal of Youth Development, 13(1-2),
216-239.
Langer, E. J. (1993). A mindful education. Educational
Psychologist, 28(1), 43-50.
Langer, E. (2000). The construct of mindfulness. Journal of Social
Issues, 56(1), 1-9.
Langer, E. J. (2009). Counterclockwise: Mindful health and the
power of possibility. New York, NY: Ballantine Books.
Langer, E. J. (2014). Mindfulness. Da Capo Lifelong Books.
Langer, E. J., & Moldoveanu, M. (2000). The construct of
mindfulness. Journal of social issues, 56(1), 1-9.
Langer, E., Hatem, M., Joss, J., & Howell, M. (1989).
Conditional teaching and mindful learning: The role of uncertainty in
education. Creativity Research Journal, 2, 139-150.
Leff, H., Gordon, L., & Ferguson, J. (1974). Cognitive set and
environmental awareness. Environment and Behavior 6, 395-447.
Luzader, J., & Spellman, J. (1996). "Living history: Hobby
or profession?" Proceedings of the 1996 National Interpreters
Workshop: 241-243.
Maslow, A. H. (1962). Lessons from the peak-experiences. Journal of
humanistic psychology, 2(1), 9-18.
Maxwell, S. E., & Delaney, H. D. (2004). Designing experiments
and analyzing data: A model comparison perspective (Vol. 1). Psychology
Press.
Moscardo, G. (1996). "Mindful visitors heritage and
tourism." Annals of Tourism Research, 23(2), 376-397.
Moscardo, G. (1999). Making visitors mindful: Principles for
creating sustainable visitor experiences through effective
communication. Champaign, IL: Sagamore.
Moscardo, G. (2001). Cultural and heritage tourism: The great
debates. In B. Faulkner, G. Moscardo, & E. Laws (Eds.), Tourism in
the 21st century (pp. 3-17). New York: Continuum.
Moscardo, G. (2009). Understanding tourist experience through
mindfulness theory. In M. Kozak & A. Decrop (Eds.), Handbook of
tourist behavior (pp. 99-115). New York, NY: Routledge.
Moscardo, G. (2014). Interpretation and tourism: holy grail or
emperor's robes? International Journal of Culture, Tourism and
Hospitality Research, 8(4), 462-476.
Moscardo, G. (2017). Exploring mindfulness and stories in tourist
experiences. International Journal of Culture, Tourism and Hospitality
Research, 11(2).
Pine, B. J., & Gilmore, J. H. (2011). The experience economy.
Boston: Harvard Business School Press.
Raudenbush, S. W., & Bryk, A. S. (2002). Hierarchical linear
models: Applications and data analysis methods (Vol. 1). Sage.
Reeve, J. (2012). A self-determination theory perspective on
student engagement. In Handbook of research on student engagement (pp.
149-172). Springer US.
Reeve, J. (2014). Understanding motivation and emotion. John Wiley
& Sons.
Ritchhart, R., & Perkins, D. (2000). Life in the mindful
classroom: Nurturing the disposition of mindfulness. Journal of Social
Issues, 56(1) 27-47.
Sutcliffe, K., & Kim, S. (2014). Understanding children's
engagement with interpretation at a cultural heritage museum. Journal of
Heritage Tourism, 9(4), 332-348.
Tilden, F. (1977). Interpreting our heritage. Chapel Hill, NC: The
University of North Carolina Press.
Walker, K., & Moscardo, G. (2014). Encouraging sustainability
beyond the tourist experience: ecotourism, interpretation and values.
Journal of Sustainable Tourism, 22(8), 1175-1196.
West, B. T., Welch, K. B., & Galecki, A. T. (2007). Linear
mixed model. Chapman Hall/CRC.
David Douglas
Church Educational System
The Church of Jesus Christ of Latter-day Saints
Gary Ellis
Texas A&M University
Andrew Lacanienta
California Polytechnic State University
1 Grand Avenue
San Luis Obispo, CA 93407-9000
alacanie@calpoly.edu
Dissonance Question Relevance Question:
Interpretive A verbal query that A verbal query that
Talk per Walking encourages consideration encourages connections
Tour Stop of incongruent beliefs between new stimuli and
of values personal history or
experiences.
Stop 1: Porter & When do you think people Have you ever seen anyone
The Mormons are ever justified in intentionally hurt another
violence and should or really picked on and
fight back? persecuted?
Stop 2: Porter's What concerns would you Have you ever felt all
Long hair have, in trusting a man alone, really alone? Or
who has served time in experienced a time where you
jail for attempted were so discouraged that you
murder? wondered if you'd make it?
Stop 3: Prophet How do think people Have you ever had something
Avenged & the should deal with stolen or know someone who
Move West Indians offering to has been robbed? How'd you
sell stolen Indian kids feel?
as slaves?
Stop 4: Porter What crimes do you Have you ever really wanted
the Lawman reckon are bad enough to take the law into your
that they are worthy of hands or felt responsible to
death? administer immediate justice
in a situation?
Stop 5: The Real When rough and tumble My world has changed
Porter people like me die and somethin' fierce. What do
meet their maker, if you you reckon have been the
had a chance to say greatest changes in your
something to the good lifetime that you've seen?
Lord about our judgment,
what would you say?
Stop 6: Plural If your religious leader Do you know anyone who is a
Marriage: instructed you to rough and tough character
practice plural but incredibly loyal or
marriage, how do you dedicated to a cause?
reckon you should
respond?
*Note: Questions above are in first-person living history form.
Grammatical and vernacular corrections were made for the third-person
living history condition and the no living history condition.
Table 1: Dissonance and Relevance Questions by Interpretive Talk
Confidence Interval
Mean SE Lower Upper
Group Means
Question Type
Relevance Question 70.196 1.401 67.429 72.963
Dissonance Question 71.925 1.490 68.982 74.869
Customary Question 67.786 1.092 65.629 69.944
Living History Type
First-person 69.987 1.394 67.232 72.741
Third-person 72.088 1.455 69.233 74.942
No Living History 67.834 1.159 65.545 70.123
Cell Means
Relevance Question
First-person 69.485 3.242 63.081 75.888
Third-person 72.528 1.650 69.270 75.786
No Living History 68.576 2.106 64.417 72.735
Dissonant Question
First-person 72.820 1.985 68.899 76.742
Third-person 74.117 3.439 67.326 80.909
No Living History 68.838 2.055 64.780 72.897
Customary Question
First-person 67.655 1.746 64.205 71.104
Third-person 69.617 2.063 65.543 73.692
No Living History 66.087 1.853 62.427 69.747
Table 2. Group and Condition Means
Source df F p [R.sup.2]PRE(*)
Question Type 2, 157.758 2.768 .066 .014
Living History Type 2, 157.758 2.406 .093 .020
Familiarity 1, 157.758 13.851 <.001 .024
QT by LH 4, 157.758 4.251 .003 .018
QT by Familiarity 2, 157.758 1.187 .308 .004
LH by Familiarity 2, 157.758 .783 .459 .086
QT by LH by Familiarity 4, 157.758 4.847 .001 .064
(*) [R.sup.2]PRE values reflect proportional reduction in variance
attained by contrasting a model containing only each source of variance
with the null model.
Table 3. Mixed Model Hypothesis Tests
COPYRIGHT 2018 National Association for Interpretation
No portion of this article can be reproduced without the express written permission from the copyright holder.
Copyright 2018 Gale, Cengage Learning. All rights reserved.