Algorithms are useful: Understanding them is even better!
Hurst, Chris ; Hurrell, Derek
Algorithms are useful: Understanding them is even better!
This is the first of two articles on the use of a written
multiplication algorithm and the mathematics that underpins it. In this
first article, we present a brief overview of research by mathematics
educators and will then provide a small selection of some of the many
student work samples we have collected during our research into
multiplicative thinking. We contend that many primary-aged children are
taught algorithms for multiplication and division without an appropriate
understanding of the mathematical structure and concepts that underpin
those algorithms. This is not about demeaning the use of standard
algorithms. They have stood the test of time and can be elegant ways of
getting a solution. However, imagine the power we give to students if we
underpin the strength of algorithms with understanding! In the second
article, we elaborate on what we believe are the key mathematical
underpinnings of algorithms.
Introduction
Algorithms are very useful methods for calculation when numbers are
too large to mentally calculate quickly or accurately. For
multiplication, this is generally when there is a need to multiply
numbers of two digits or more by another number of a similar magnitude.
For example, when attempting to multiply a single-digit number by a
double-digit number, students should be considering other strategies,
such as applying the distributive property, and exercising their
understanding of place value (e.g., 17 x 6 is 10 x 6 which is 60 and 7 x
6 which is 42 so 17 x 6 is 60 + 42 = 102), which allows them to complete
these calculations mentally. However, where algorithms are deemed as
necessary it would be preferable if the user of the algorithm had an
understanding of not only what they were doing, but also, why they are
doing it.
An algorithm can be defined as a step-by-step procedure used to
solve a problem or complete a task (Anderson et al., 2007). The key here
is the word 'procedure' and how this word can sometimes be
interpreted. A generally accepted definition of procedure may be a
series of actions carried out in a certain order, which seems innocuous
enough. However, if the procedure is used without understanding, that is
a different matter. Skemp's seminal article differentiated between
relational and instrumental learning, with the latter largely equating
to "rules without reasons" (1976, p. 20). One of the arguments
for the efficacy of algorithms is that they save time and lessen the
cognitive load on students, therefore allowing students more
'resources' for problem solving to occur (Merrienboer &
Sweller, 2005).
This may be particularly so for students who are cognitively less
efficient in mathematics. However, it is important that students do not
become too reliant on procedures and algorithms but rather that they
have the opportunity to be involved in productive struggle (Jonnson,
Norvquist, Liljekvist & Lithner, 2014) to enhance the development of
conceptual understanding (Hiebert & Grouws, 2007). It is the
exploration of the mathematics behind the procedure that is important,
not the uninformed use of the procedure. Whilst we encourage the use of
algorithms to aid students in their mathematical development, the use of
them without understanding may indeed be impeding that development.
We can choose to teach an algorithm as a purely mechanical way of
reaching a solution, but if we do so, much of the potential power of the
algorithm is lost. Brosseau (1997) stated that algorithms are designed
to be efficient, and to avoid meaning. What he meant by this, was that
you can focus on the mechanics without needing to understand what you
are doing. For instance, when you "carry the one" you are
dealing with it on its face value as being one, not the fact that you
are actually renaming 10 ones as one 10, or 10 tens as one hundred etc.
Students need to understand and be able to articulate this, which be
facilitated by exposure to concrete materials to model the regrouping
process. This is supported by Ellis and Yeh (2008), who assert that
"traditional algorithms used for multiplication may be efficient
but they are not transparent.. .[that is] they do not allow students to
see why they work" (p. 368). Algorithms need to be developed
through a thorough understanding of the distributive property, gradually
increasing the size of the numbers and developing the grid or area
representation for multiplication. Davis (2008), supported this in
saying that, "An algorithm for multi-digit whole-number
multiplication can be reformatted in a grid, which can connect the
standard algorithm to area" (p. 88). Being able to see all the
partial products gives the students the opportunity to understand how
the multiplication algorithm works. Using a grid representation for
multiplication takes some time and effort both on the part of the
teachers and the students but this is time well invested. This
investment could save a good deal of time later through minimising the
need for remediation. This is supported through the work of Englert and
Sinicrope (1994) who wrote "although the time spent in developing
the multiplication algorithm using this visual approach (grids) is
greater than the time needed to use a more traditional approach, less
time is needed for review and reteaching. Students are able to attach
meaning to the multiplication algorithm" (p. 447).
As noted before, a second article has been written about the
written multiplication algorithm. In it, we describe the mathematics
that underpins the written algorithm and we outline a teaching sequence
and learning progression for developing students' understanding of
how and why the written algorithm works.
Evidence
Following is some evidence collected from our research.
Semi-structured interviews were conducted with these students. We posed
two research questions:
* Are students able to perform the vertical written algorithm for
multiplication?
* Are they able to articulate an understanding of the algorithm and
why it works?
Sample
The data presented here come from an ongoing study into
children's multiplicative thinking. As part of the study, students
from Years 5 and 6 were engaged in semi-structured interviews to
ascertain their level of understanding of a range of multiplicative
concepts, including their strategies for multiplication, one of which
was their use of a written method or algorithm. We found that there was
great variation across the sample of 81 students, with some
demonstrating a strong understanding and efficient use of the written
algorithm while others struggled to correctly use and/ or articulate
about a strategy, including the written algorithm. The samples presented
have been purposely selected to show that some students are attempting
to use a written algorithm for multiplication without understanding it
and/or when the use of a mental strategy might have been more effective.
Pseudonyms have been used for the students.
Katie (Year 6)
Katie was asked to calculate the answer for 17 x 6.
She set it out as a vertical algorithm (Figure 1) and explained her
working in the following way: "Six goes into seven once, write down
one and carry one and add it to the 'one' in the 17. Six times
two is 12". It was immediately evident that Katie's attempt to
use the algorithm was confused.
We wanted to see if different numbers would lead to a different
result so we asked Katie to calculate the answer for 13 x 4 (Figure 2).
This time she described her working in this way:
"Four times three is 12, write down the two and carry the one.
Add it to the one in the 13 to get two. Four times two is eight so the
answer is 82".
Katie was asked to check her working for 13 x 4 and this time she
arrived at 92! She was then given 20 x 4 and the following conversation
ensued:
Katie: I don't need to set that out, I can do it in my
head--it's 80.
Int: How did you work it out?
Katie: You do 2 x 4 = 8 and add a zero.
Int: Where did the zero come from?
Katie repeated it and said, "You add the zero back on"
but could not say where it came from or what it showed.
Is Katie using algorithms effectively?
In Katie's articulation of how she solved the 13 x 4 problem
there is some mathematics that we can celebrate. For example, she seems
to know some of the multiplication facts. This is important, but in
itself does not necessarily indicate an understanding of multiplication,
particularly if she has merely memorised the number facts. Indeed, there
seems to be a lot of mathematical thinking which she does not employ.
For instance, the two multiplication examples both involved one case of
'trading up' yet Katie's explanations of each were quite
different. She did 'trade up' in the second example but
combined the tens incorrectly.
Also, she made no mention that the one she 'carried' was
actually worth ten. Saying that, "Add it to the one in the thirteen
to get two" does not reflect a strong understanding of place value.
Her language in fact hides what is happening. While it may seem like
semantics, it actually does not make sense, as adding one to 13
'gets' 14. She really needs to articulate that what she is
doing is adding one lot of 10, which can then lead her to "four
lots of 10 (4 x 10), add one lot of ten, equals five lots of ten, or
50". When we talk about the process we actually use partial
products so it would seem sensible to underpin this 'talk'
with work which allows Katie to utilise a model in which the partial
products are able to be overtly illustrated. Such a model is the grid
(multiplicative array) model for 13 x 4 which can be used to develop the
vertical algorithm (Figure 4).
A question to ask here is: does Katie really need to write a
standard algorithm at all? As she has some mastery over multiplication
facts, she may actually be better served by using mental computation, by
employing the distributive property and perhaps recording a few notes to
aid her memory. That is, "four lots of three is 12, four lots of 10
is 40, 40 and 12 is 52". Of course this is all predicated on Katie
understanding that the distributive property can be legitimately used
here.
Is Katie able to effectively use algorithms? The evidence suggests
not. A standard algorithm is useful but Katie is using it when mental
strategies should suffice, and she is using it poorly because she does
not understand the parts that make the whole. Katie needs to be assisted
to understand how and why the vertical algorithm works, so her classroom
experiences should be built around some use of concrete materials such
as bundling sticks and MABs, trading games, the distributive property of
multiplication, and the use of the array to develop the grid method.
James (Year 6)
James was also asked to work out 17 x 6. Initially he did 6 x 7 =
42, wrote it down and then 1 x 6 = 6 and wrote it in front to get 642
(Figure 5). When asked if he thought the answer was right, James
corrected his work. He was then given 34 x 4 and immediately said that
the 34 meant 30 + 4. He correctly used the vertical algorithm (Figure
6).
James was then given a two-digit by two-digit multiplication 29 x
37. He explained it in this way (Figure 7): "Seven times nine is
63, put down the three and carry the six above the two. Three twos make
six, and add that to the six I carried--it makes twelve".
Is James using algorithms effectively?
In one instance, James was able to use an algorithm to multiply a
two-digit number by a one-digit number (34 x 4), but in another
instance, his original attempt was incorrect. James showed he is capable
of remembering and using some multiplication facts (6 x 7 = 42), and
that he was able to partition 34 into 30 and four and to complete the
second algorithm correctly. However these did not give him the facility
to work with a two-digit by two-digit multiplication problem.
In the two-digit by two-digit multiplication James was not prompted
to estimate to check the range of his solution before he started the
problem, as we wanted to see if he did so of his own accord. He did not
estimate before calculating, or to check the reasonableness of the
answer (ten lots of 29 is 10 x 29 which equals 290, which is already
more than the answer of 123, and 37 lots of 29 is going to be bigger
still). He then does not seem to recognise that in carrying out the
procedure he has missed some of the partial products. He has not dealt
with all of the distributed parts. He has dealt with 7 x 9 = 63 and
dealt with 3 x 2 = 6 but has worked with these second set of digits with
their face value without any consideration of their place value, that is
30 x 20 = 600. The parts he has totally neglected are 7 x 20 and 30 x 9.
Is James able to effectively use algorithms? The evidence suggests
not. Although the fact he recognises partitioning as a viable strategy
and manages to calculate the two-digit by one-digit multiplication
example, James is still not displaying sound understanding of the
structure of multiplication. As with Katie, James would benefit greatly
from the use of the array and distributive property to inform the grid
representation. This should help James understand the distributed parts
he needs to deal with.
Todd (Year 6)
Todd was asked to provide the answer for 7 x 15 and he responded
with, "I'd use place value, I'd go 7 times 10 and 7 times
5 and add the results of those together". The interviewer asked if
he would just do that in his head and Todd said that he would. The
interview with Todd was conducted at a later date than those with Katie
and James, and it was decided to have bundling sticks available to see
if they illuminated children's understanding. When asked if he
could use bundling sticks to show 7 x 15, Todd took seven bundles of ten
and seven groups of five. Todd also wrote down the working he had done
mentally and to show what he had done with the bundling sticks.
Todd was asked to provide the answer for 23 x 4.
He used a 'double-double' strategy and explained that in
his head, he would work out 40 times 2 and 6 times 2 (from the 46 x 2)
and add the two answers together. No written algorithm was used. The
interviewer asked, "What do you call it when you break up a number
like that?" Todd suggested it might be 'place value'. The
interviewer then asked Todd if he had heard of the term
'partitioning' and Todd said that he hadn't.
For 400 x 23, the following discussion occurred.
Todd: Basically it's the same as 23 x 4 except you're
adding more place value, so you know the answer to that is [wrote 92,
circled the two zeros on the 400] and then you add two zeros.
Interviewer: What tells you that you're 'adding more
place value'?
Todd: The zeros?
Interviewer: What do the two zeros do in the number?
Todd: If there was only one zero, it would be 40, then you could go
up to thousands, then forty thousand and so on.
Todd's work sample follows.
Is Todd using algorithms effectively?
Todd is better positioned to effectively use algorithms than Katie
or James as he has some key knowledge upon which algorithms are built.
Although Todd does not know the term 'partitioning', he does
know how to employ it. This situates him well to make the connection
between partitioning and the distributive property of multiplication,
upon which grid multiplication and the algorithm are founded. Some
explicit teaching around this idea, specifically using the terms
'partitioning' and the 'distributive property',
would be beneficial. Todd has some understanding of multiplication when
powers of ten are involved. Again, although he said that it is
'place value', he is partially correct and would likely
benefit from some explicit teaching around the idea of digits moving a
place to the left when multiplied by ten. Todd is unlikely to experience
difficulties related to the second line of a two-digit algorithm because
he seems to be aware that the presence of two zeros in 9200 indicates
that he is "adding more place value".
Conclusion
The samples presented indicate some of the typical issues we have
seen during our research work. The main concern shown here is a lack of
understanding and application of the distributive property when
attempting to use a written algorithm. There are two key questions that
need to be broached when considering these samples and any other samples
of student work. These are:
* What is it about each student's work, which suggests they
need to be supported with explicit teaching to enable them to
effectively use algorithms?
* What mathematical understandings does each child need to develop
which would better equip them to use algorithms?
In order to establish what the issues are, teachers need a deep
understanding of the structure of algorithms so that they may identify
in very specific terms how to help each student. The key ideas that
underpin algorithms are intricately connected and such connections need
to be made, explicitly taught to and explored deeply by students.
References
Anderson, J., Briner, A., Irons, C., Shield, M., Sparrow, L., &
Steinle, V. (2007). The Origo handbook of mathematics education. Origo
Education, Brisbane.
Brousseau, G. (1997). Theory of didactical situations in
mathematics. Dordrecht: Kluwer.
Davis, B. (2008). Is 1 a prime number? Developing teacher knowledge
through concept study. Mathematics Teaching in the Middle School, 14(2),
86-91.
Ellis, M. & Yeh, C. (2008). Creative arithmetic: Exploring
alternative methods. Teaching Children Mathematics, 14(6), 367-368.
Englert, G. R. & Sinicrope, R. (1994). Making connections with
two-digit multiplication. Arithmetic Teacher, 41, 446-48.
Hiebert, J., & Grouws, D. A. (2007). The effects of classroom
mathematics teaching on students' learning. In J. Frank, & K.
Lester (Eds.), Second handbook of research on mathematics teaching and
learning (Vol. 1) (pp. 371-404). Greenwich, CT: Information Age.
Jonsson, B., Norvqvist, M., Liljekvist, Y., & Lithner, J.
(2014). Learning mathematics through algorithmic and creative reasoning.
The Journal of Mathematical Behavior, 36, 20-32.
Merrienboer, J. G., & Sweller, J. (2005). Cognitive load theory
and complex learning: Recent developments and future directions.
Educational Psychology Review, 17(2), 147-177. http://dx.doi.
org/10.1007/s10648-005-3951-0
Skemp, R.R. (1976). Relational and instrumental understanding.
Mathematics Teaching, 77, 20-26.
Chris Hurst
Curtin University, WA
<c.hurst@curtin.edu.au>
Derek Hurrell
University of Notre Dame, WA
<Derek.Hurrell@nd.edu.au>
Caption: Figure 1.
Caption: Figure 2.
Caption: Figure 3.
Caption: Figure 4.
Caption: Figure 5.
Caption: Figure 6.
Caption: Figure 7.
COPYRIGHT 2018 The Australian Association of Mathematics Teachers, Inc.
No portion of this article can be reproduced without the express written permission from the copyright holder.
Copyright 2018 Gale, Cengage Learning. All rights reserved.