首页    期刊浏览 2024年10月06日 星期日
登录注册

文章基本信息

  • 标题:Impairments of Memory and Learning in Older Adults Exposed to Polychlorinated Biphenyls via Consumption of Great Lakes Fish
  • 作者:Susan L. Schantz
  • 期刊名称:Environmental Health Perspectives
  • 印刷版ISSN:0091-6765
  • 电子版ISSN:1552-9924
  • 出版年度:2001
  • 卷号:June 2001
  • 出版社:OCR Subscription Services Inc

Impairments of Memory and Learning in Older Adults Exposed to Polychlorinated Biphenyls via Consumption of Great Lakes Fish

Susan L. Schantz

An association between in utero polychlorinated biphenyl (PCB) exposure and impaired childhood intellectual functioning has been reported, but the potential impact of PCB exposure during adulthood on intellectual functioning has received little attention. We assessed the impact of PCBs and other fish-borne contaminants on intellectual functioning in older adults. The subjects were 49- to 86-year-old Michigan residents recruited from an existing cohort. Fish eaters ate [is greater than] 24 lb of sport-caught Lake Michigan fish per year and non-fish eaters ate [is less than] 6 lb of Lake Michigan fish per year. A battery of cognitive tests including tests of memory and learning, executive function, and visual--spatial function was administered to 180 subjects (101 fish eaters and 79 non-fish eaters). Blood samples were analyzed for PCBs and 10 other contaminants. We evaluated cognitive outcomes using multiple regression. PCBs and dichlorodiphenyl dichloroethene (DDE) were markedly elevated in fish eaters. After controlling for potential confounders PCB, but not DDE, exposure was associated with lower scores on several measures of memory and learning. These included the Weschler Memory Scale verbal delayed recall (p = 0.001), the semantic cluster ratio (p = 0.006), and list A, trial 1 (p = 0.037), from the California Verbal Learning Test. In contrast, executive and visual--spatial function were not impaired by exposure to either PCBs or DDE. In conclusion, PCB exposure during adulthood was associated with impairments in memory and learning, whereas executive and visual--spatial function were unaffected. These results are consistent with previous research showing an association between in utero PCB exposure and impairments of memory during infancy and childhood. Key words: DDE, dichlorodiphenyl dichloroethene, executive function, Great Lakes fish, learning, memory, older adults, polychlorinated biphenyls, PCBs, visual--spatial function. Environ Health Perspect 109:605-611 (2001). [Online 5 June 2001]

http://ehpnet1.niehs.nih.gov/docs/2001/ 109p605-611schantz/abstract.html

Great Lakes fish are contaminated with a host of pollutants including chlorinated organic compounds such as polychlorinated biphenyls (PCBs) and dichlorodiphenyl dichloroethene (DDE), pesticides such as mirex and dieldrin, and trace amounts of metals such as lead and mercury. Although commercial fishing on the Great Lakes is subject to federal regulation, sport fishing is not regulated. Advisories have been developed to inform the public about which fish are safe to eat, but the impact of these advisories on fish consumption is unknown.

The health impacts of regular consumption of sport-caught Great Lakes fish are not clear. However, previous studies have reported an association between in utero exposure to PCBs via maternal consumption of Great Lakes fish and impaired intellectual functioning in infancy (1) and childhood (2-4). In these children, the clearest and most consistent negative association between PCB exposure and intellectual function was seen on tests involving verbal competence and short-term memory processing. Recently another team of investigators has reported a similar association between PCB exposure from Great Lakes fish and short-term memory processing in infancy (5). Deficits in intellectual function have also been reported in children whose mothers were exposed to PCBs and related compounds from non-fish sources in the Netherlands (6) and in Taiwan (7). In contrast, no relationship between PCB exposure and childhood intellectual functioning was observed in a cohort of children in North Carolina (8).

The potential impact of PCBs and other fish-borne contaminants on the intellectual functioning of adults has received very little attention. Older adults may be at increased risk from exposure to fish-borne contaminants such as PCBs and DDE because body burdens of these lipophilic compounds increase with age (9). At the same time, many aspects of neurological function decline with advancing age (10). We conducted the present study to determine if exposure to PCBs and other fish-borne contaminants negatively impacts neurocognitive functioning in older adults.

Methods

Sample

The subjects were selected from an existing cohort of people who consumed large amounts of Great Lakes fish (n = 572) or little or no Great Lakes fish (n = 419). The cohort was originally established in 1980-1982, and its characteristics have been described previously (9,11). The heavy fish eaters regularly consumed greater than 24 lb of sport-caught Lake Michigan fish per year, with consumption ranging from 24 to 270 lb/year (median = 38.5 lb/year). Subjects were recruited through visits to marinas, bait shops, and fishing clubs, and by referrals from other participants. The comparison group of people who consumed little or no Great Lakes fish was recruited through random-digit dialing in the communities where the fish eaters were recruited. They reported eating less than 6 lb of sport-caught fish per year.

The current study was restricted to individuals [is greater than or equal to] 49 years of age in 1992 (n = 549). Subjects were randomly selected from 12 cells defined by age (49-59, 60-69, [is greater than or equal to] 70), sex, and fish-eating status, with the goal of recruiting a total of 150-200 people. The details of subject recruitment and the demographic characteristics of participants and refusers have been published previously (12,13). Of those who were contacted, 104 of 158 fish eaters and 84 of 188 non-fish eaters were enrolled in the study (n = 188). Fish eaters who were enrolled had demographic and lifestyle characteristics similar to those of fish eaters who did not enroll, with the exception that fish eaters who participated were more likely to be employed (44% vs. 23%). This difference did not appear to be related to health status, as neither self-ratings of health nor number of hospitalizations within the previous 5 years differed by participant status. Among the non-fish eaters, there were no significant differences between participants and those who did not enroll.

The 188 subjects who agreed to participate were contacted by phone to arrange an in-home visit. A single examiner who was unaware of their exposure history administered a battery of cognitive and motor tests. Table 1 shows the order in which the tests were administered. The findings of the motor function tests have been reported elsewhere (13). All procedures involving human subjects were in accordance with national and institutional guidelines and were approved by the institutional review boards at three cooperating institutions: The University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign, Michigan State University, and the Michigan Department of Community Health. All subjects provided written informed consent.

Table 1. Order of administration of neuropsychological test battery
and interview.(a)

Test                                          References   No.(b)

Wechsler Memory Scale (immediate recall)       (14-16)
  Logical Memory                                           179
  Visual Reproduction                                      178(c)
WAIS-R vocabulary subtest                        (17)      179
Grooved Pegboard Test(d)
  Dominant hand                                            164
  Non-dominant hand                                        161
Static Motor Steadiness Test(d)
  Dominant hand                                            165
  Non-dominant hand                                        163
Wechsler Memory Scale (delayed recall)
  Logical memory                                           179
  Visual reproduction                                      178(c)
Wisconsin Card Sorting Test                    (18,19)     178(c)
California Verbal Learning Test                (20,21)     177(e)
    (immediate and short delay recall)
Trail-Making Test                                (22)
  Trail A                                                  178(c)
  Trail B                                                  177(c,f)
WAIS-R Digit Symbol                              (17)      178(c)
Stroop color-word test                           (23)      177(c,g)
California Verbal Learning Test                            177(e)
    (20 min delayed recall and recognition)
Hooper Visual Organization Test                  (24)      178(c)
Short Category Test                              (25)      178(c)
Demographic/health interview                               179
State-Trait Anxiety Inventory                    (26)      179
Beck Depression Inventory                        (27)      179

WAIS-R, Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-Revised.

(a) A self-administered questionnaire covering fish consumption and
occupational history was mailed to participants in advance and
collected on the day of testing.

(b) Based on a potential maximum of 179, 8 participants refused to
provide blood and 1 other participant was eliminated from analysis
due to marked cognitive problems resulting from surgery to his carotid
artery.

(c) One subject had severe macular degeneration; all vision-dependent
tests were removed from analysis.

(d) Tests obtained from Lafayette Instruments, Lafayette, IN; for
results of the motor tests, refer to Schantz et al. (13).

(e) One subject refused the CVLT, and another was eliminated from
analysis because the standardized instructions were misunderstood.

(f) One subject refused to complete Trail B.

(g) One color blind subject was unable to complete the Stroop
color-word test.

Main Outcome Measures

Memory and learning. Wechsler Memory Scale. We administered two subtests of the Wechsler Memory Scale Form I (WMS) (14) using Russell's revised procedures (15). The Logical Memory subtest consists of two paragraph-length passages. Each is read aloud by the examiner, followed by an immediate recall trial. After 30 min of unrelated testing, a delayed recall trial is administered without warning. Responses were scored according to the criteria of Power et al. (16). Scores from the two stories were summed, for a total of 46 points possible.

The Visual Reproduction subtest assesses recall of visually presented material. The test stimuli consist of four abstract geometrical designs printed on three 6 in x 4 in cards (the third card contains two figures). The cards are presented for 10 sec; after each is removed, the subject is asked to draw the design(s) from memory. As with LM, recall is assessed again after a 30-min delay. The drawings were scored according to Wechsler's (14) criteria and summed, for a total of 14 possible points.

California Verbal Learning Test. The California Verbal Learning Test (CVLT) (20) is a repeated-trials learning test comprising two fictional shopping lists (A and B). Each 16-item list of common household items contains four items from each of four general semantic categories (e.g., fruits). The examiner reads the lists aloud to the subject for recall. List A is presented five times, with immediate recall trials following each. List B is presented once, followed by free and category-cued recall of List A (short delay). After 20 min (long delay), memory for List A is assessed once more by free recall, cued recall, and recognition.

The CVLT was computer-administered using Fridlund and Delis's program (21), which scores the test according to the criteria of Delis et al. (20). The program yields 27 scores, quantifying different aspects of learning such as recall, recognition, use of strategies, and interference. Nine scores were selected a priori for analysis: List A Trials 1-5 total, List A short delay free recall, List A long delay free recall, Learning Slope, Perseverations, Semantic cluster ratio, Discriminability, Proactive interference (List B adjusted for List A Trial 1), and Recognition versus long delay free recall.

Executive function. Wisconsin Card Sorting Test. The Wisconsin Card Sorting Test (18) assesses abstract reasoning, concept formation, and shift of set strategies. Four "key cards" with geometric designs are placed before the subject. These cards differ along three dimensions: the shape, color, and number of the designs on the card (e.g., one red triangle, two green stars, etc.). The test stimuli are 128 similar cards, each containing random combinations of shape, color, and number. The subject is instructed to match each of the cards in the decks to one of the key cards, but not by which dimension the cards should be matched (initially color). The subject must deduce the correct sorting principle from examiner feedback as to whether each placement is "right" or "wrong." After 10 consecutive cards are correctly placed, the sorting principle is changed without warning. The test continues until six categories are completed or the cards are exhausted.

The test was scored according to Heaton's criteria (18), using the Psychological Assessment Resources' computer program (19). Four of the 12 scores computed by the program were selected a priori for analysis: total categories completed (0-6), failure to maintain set, perseverative responses, and percent perseverative errors (18).

Trail-Making Test Parts A and B. The Trail-Making Test (22) assesses complex visual scanning and attention under time pressure. Part A requires the subject to draw a line to connect 25 numbered circles in consecutive order. Part B also consists of 25 labeled circles, but the task is to connect the circles by alternating numbers with letters (i.e., 1-A-2-B-3-C, etc.). The score for each is the time to completion.

Short Category Test, Booklet Format. The Short Category Test (25) assesses abstract reasoning ability, concept formation, and hypothesis testing. The Short Category Test consists of five booklets, each containing 20 cards depicting various geometric shapes, lines, colors, and figures. All of the cards within a booklet are organized according to a single principle or concept. The subject is not told what the principle is, but must deduce it from examiner feedback and use it to provide the correct response to the remaining cards. The score is the total number of incorrect responses out of 100.

Stroop color--word test. The Stroop test (23) measures attention and selective concentration. It consists of three cards, each containing 100 stimulus items. The items on the cards are, respectively, color names printed in black (word), "X's" printed in colored ink (color), and color names printed in incongruous-colored ink (color--word; e.g. "blue" printed in red ink). The task for the first card is to read the words; the task for the second and third cards is to name the color of the ink the items are printed in. The subject is instructed to read/name the items on each card as quickly as possible for 45 sec.

The Stroop test was administered and scored according to the protocol of Golden (23), except that new cards were printed using a larger, bolder font. These cards preserved the order of items and the hue of the colors used in the originals, but made it easier for elderly subjects to discriminate the test items.

Visual--spatial function. Digit Symbol Substitution. The Digit Symbol Subtest from the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-Revised (WAIS-R) (17) is a coding task assessing attention, visual scanning, visuomotor speed, and incidental memory. A key at the top of the page pairs nine nonsense symbols with the numbers 1-9. There are 93 divided boxes. The top sections of the boxes contain the numbers 1-9 in random order; the bottom sections are blank. The task is to draw the correct symbol below each number by referring to the key at the top of the page. The score is the number of squares the subject is able to complete in 90 sec.

Hooper Visual Organization Test. The Hooper Visual Organization Test (24) is a mental assembly task consisting of 30 simple line drawings of common objects, each of which has been cut into pieces and placed randomly on a card. The score is the number of correctly identified objects out of 30.

Confounding Variables

Through a careful review of the literature, we identified a large number of demographic, lifestyle, psychological, and health-related variables as potential confounders. These are listed in Table 2 and were described in detail in a previous publication (13). A comparison of the demographic, lifestyle, and psychological characteristics of the fish eaters and non-fish eaters has also been published previously (12). Fish eaters and non-fish eaters had very similar demographic characteristics, reported similar patterns of smoking and alcohol consumption, and had comparable scores on measures of intellectual functioning and affect. The only medical condition that differed between fish eaters and non-fish eaters was arthritis, which was reported more frequently in male fish eaters. Although PCB and DDE levels are correlated with fish-eating status, we hypothesized that any effects observed on the outcome measures would be due to contaminant exposure rather than fish consumption per se. Therefore, fish eaters and non-fish eaters were combined and analyses were performed using regression procedures.

Table 2. Summary of covariates by category.(a)

                                    Psychological
Demographic      Lifestyle          functioning         Health

Age              Activity level     Overall             Self-rating of
Sex                Leisure/sports       intellectual      overall
Education          Occupational         function          health
Income             Household          WAIS-R            General health
Employment         Stair climbing       Vocabulary        (e.g., body
  status         Tobacco use            subtest           mass index,
Marital status   Alcohol            Emotion/stress        hearing loss,
Social support       consumption      Beck Depression     visual
Household size   Caffeine               Inventory         impairment)
Dominant hand        consumption      State-Trait       Physical
                                        Anxiety           symptoms
                                        Inventory         (e.g.,
                                                          fatigue,
                                                          dizziness,
                                                          minor colds)
                                                        Medical
                                                          conditions
                                                          (e.g.,
                                                          arthritis,
                                                          thyroid
                                                          disease,
                                                          hypertension)
                                                        Prescription
                                                          and nonpre-
                                                          scription
                                                          drug use,
                                                          categorized
                                                          by class
                                                          (e.g.
                                                          antibiotics,
                                                          diuretics,
                                                          acetami-
                                                          nophen)
                                                        History of
                                                          psychiatric
                                                          illness,
                                                          head injury,
                                                          stroke

(a) For a complete list, see Schantz et al. (13).

Measures of Exposure

Several weeks after completion of the neuropsychological assessment battery, we collected a blood sample from 96% (n = 180) of the participants for residue analysis. One sample was not obtained because of a collapsed vein, one subject died before a sample could be collected, and six subjects refused the blood draw. The samples were analyzed for PCBs and 10 other contaminants including DDE, polybrominated biphenyls, hexachlorobenzene, oxychlordane, dieldrin, mirex, mercury, and lead (11,28). We analyzed PCBs, DDE, and other organochlorines in serum by gas chromatography. Lead and mercury were analyzed in whole blood by atomic absorption spectrophotometry. Serum or blood concentrations of the contaminants have been reported previously (12,28). Four chemicals, PCBs, DDE, lead, and mercury were higher in the heavy fish eaters than in those who ate little or no fish, but PCBs and DDE clearly emerged as the primary contaminants of concern and are, therefore, the focus of this report. The mean levels of each of the four contaminants in each of the 12 enrollment cells defined by age, sex, and fish-eating status are shown in Table 3. The levels of PCBs and DDE were markedly elevated in heavy fish eaters, both relative to age- and sex-matched non-fish eaters and relative to levels typically observed in the population at large (29). Non-fish eaters, while also exposed to PCBs and DDE, had much lower levels of both chemicals. Only 15 participants (8.4%) had serum PCB levels below the detection limit of 3.0 ng/g, and only 6 (3.4%) had DDE levels below the detection limit of 1.0 ng/g.

Table 3. Summary statistics for PCB, DDE, lead, and mercury by
sex, age, and fish-eating status.

                                        Fish eaters

             Age
Sex        (years)     Exposure     No.     Mean [+ or -] SD

Male        < 60       PCB(a)        16   15.88 [+ or -] 14.8
                       DDE(b)             12.26 [+ or -] 9.2
                       Lead(c)             4.40 [+ or -] 1.5
                       Mercury(d)          3.18 [+ or -] 4.8

            60-69      PCB           16   23.03 [+ or -] 19.1
                       DDE                18.12 [+ or -] 17.7
                       Lead                5.13 [+ or -] 1.8
                       Mercury             3.96 [+ or -] 2.7

         [is greater   PCB           14   24.69 [+ or -] 19.3
           than or     DDE                28.09 [+ or -] 37.4
          equal to]    Lead                4.38 [+ or -] 1.7
             70        Mercury             3.06 [+ or -] 2.9

Female      < 60       PCB(a)        21    8.67 [+ or -] 4.6
                       DDE(b)              7.09 [+ or -] 4.6
                       Lead(c)             2.45 [+ or -] 1.5
                       Mercury(d)                  ND

            60-69      PCB           17   14.04 [+ or -] 6.8
                       DDE                11.50 [+ or -] 6.3
                       Lead                3.29 [+ or -] 1.3
                       Mercury                     ND

         [is greater   PCB           17   13.52 [+ or -] 10.8
           than or     DDE                22.63 [+ or -] 16.1
          equal to]    Lead                3.38 [+ or -] 1.9
             70        Mercury                     ND

                                       Fish eaters

             Age
Sex        (years)     Exposure     Median     Min-Max

Male        < 60       PCB(a)        10.60     ND-65.20
                       DDE(b)         9.60   1.80-36.70
                       Lead(c)        5.00   2.00-7.00
                       Mercury(d)     2.40     ND-20.50

            60-69      PCB           17.25     ND-65.20
                       DDE            8.70     ND-52.70
                       Lead           5.00   3.00-9.00
                       Mercury        3.75     ND-9.80

         [is greater   PCB           19.95     ND-75.00
           than or     DDE           13.05   5.40-145.00
          equal to]    Lead           4.00   2.00-7.00
             70        Mercury        2.85     ND-7.40

Female      < 60       PCB(a)         7.60     ND-23.00
                       DDE(b)         6.20     ND-15.90
                       Lead(c)        2.00     ND-7.00
                       Mercury(d)     ND       ND-3.00

            60-69      PCB           12.40   4.00-26.30
                       DDE           10.00   2.90-26.40
                       Lead           3.00     ND-6.00
                       Mercury        2.00     ND-6.00

         [is greater   PCB           12.10     ND-49.00
           than or     DDE           16.20   7.00-62.60
          equal to]    Lead           3.00     ND-7.00
             70        Mercury        ND       ND-8.00

                                       Non-fish eaters

             Age
Sex        (years)     Exposure     No.   Mean [+ or -] SD

Male        < 60       PCB(a)        11   6.11 [+ or -] 4.2
                       DDE(b)             7.12 [+ or -] 5.9
                       Lead(c)            3.45 [+ or -] 1.6
                       Mercury(d)         2.05 [+ or -] 1.9

            60-69      PCB           12   6.82 [+ or -] 3.5
                       DDE                9.99 [+ or -] 8.0
                       Lead               5.08 [+ or -] 4.1
                       Mercury                   ND

         [is greater   PCB            7   5.66 [+ or -] 2.0
           than or     DDE                8.03 [+ or -] 6.4
          equal to]    Lead               3.67 [+ or -] 1.6
             70        Mercury                   ND

Female      < 60       PCB(a)        19   4.64 [+ or -] 2.2
                       DDE(b)             4.94 [+ or -] 4.3
                       Lead(c)            2.47 [+ or -] 2.2
                       Mercury(d)                ND

            60-69      PCB           16   8.71 [+ or -] 8.5
                       DDE                8.46 [+ or -] 7.9
                       Lead               2.75 [+ or -] 1.3
                       Mercury                   ND

         [is greater   PCB           13   5.05 [+ or -] 3.8
           than or     DDE                7.22 [+ or -] 6.1
          equal to]    Lead               3.54 [+ or -] 1.4
             70        Mercury                   ND

                                     Non-fish eaters
             Age
Sex        (years)     Exposure     Median    Min-Max

Male        < 60       PCB(a)        5.00      ND-15.30
                       DDE(b)        4.80      ND-17.40
                       Lead(c)       3.00      ND-6.00
                       Mercury(d)    2.10      ND-5.00

            60-69      PCB           5.95    3.40-15.50
                       DDE           7.95    1.80-31.50
                       Lead          4.00      ND-15.00
                       Mercury        ND       ND-4.00

         [is greater   PCB           4.90    4.00-10.00
           than or     DDE           6.90    3.00-21.40
          equal to]    Lead          3.50    2.00-6.00
             70        Mercury        ND       ND-3.50

Female      < 60       PCB(a)         ND       ND-9.20
                       DDE(b)        4.50      ND-19.60
                       Lead(c)       2.00      ND-9.00
                       Mercury(d)     ND       ND-4.10

            60-69      PCB           6.10      ND-25.90
                       DDE           6.10    1.60-32.50
                       Lead          2.00    2.00-6.00
                       Mercury        ND       ND-3.30

         [is greater   PCB           4.70      ND-11.00
           than or     DDE           5.80      ND-20.90
          equal to]    Lead          3.00    2.00-6.00
             70        Mercury        ND       ND-3.00

Abbreviations: Max, Maximum; Min, minimum; ND, below the limit of
detection.

(a) Detection limit=3.0 ng/g.

(b) Detection limit=1.0 ng/g.

(c) Detection limit=2.0 [micro]g/dL.

(d) Detection limit=2.0 ng/g.

On average, males had higher PCB levels than females (mean: males 15.2 vs. females 9.2; p = 0.01), and PCB levels were also associated with increasing age (chi-square test, p = 0.04). For DDE however, the mean levels did not differ by sex (p = 0.11), but they did differ by age (chi-square test, p [is less than] 0.001). Lead and mercury were slightly elevated in fish eaters relative to non-fish eaters, but the levels were near background in all but a few subjects. Over one-half (54%) of participants had mercury levels below the detection limit of 2.0 ng/g. Polybrominated biphenyls, a contaminant unique to the state of Michigan, were detected in a subset of the subjects, but the levels did not differ between fish eaters and non-fish eaters. None of the other chemicals were detected in any of the subjects.

Statistical Analyses

We used SAS software, version 7 (SAS Institute Inc., Cary, NC) to perform all statistical analyses. We used regression analyses to assess the relationship between performance on each outcome measure and exposure to PCBs and DDE, while simultaneously controlling for potential confounders. Multiple linear regression was used for normally distributed continuous outcomes. Outcomes with distributions exhibiting appreciable skewness were dichotomized and analyzed by logistic regression.

We developed regression models for each of four classes of control variables (Table 2), starting with all correlates of the outcome variable identified through univariate analyses as being significant at p [is less than] 0.20. The preliminary multivariable model for each class of control variables retained all variables significant at p [is less than] 0.10. The final regression model combined the four separate models and the exposure variables for PCBs and DDE. We entered the exposure variables on the logarithmic scale and replaced nondetectable values with one-half the limit of detection. We explored alternate methods for handling participants with PCB and/or DDE levels below the detection limit (including removing them from the analyses altogether, but the final models for the outcome measures under discussion were essentially unchanged. PCBs and DDE were retained throughout the analyses irrespective of their statistical significance. All other variables were retained in the final model if they remained significant at p [is less than] 0.10. Variants of this approach have been applied in other settings when a large number of potential confounding variables need to be considered (30,31). Variables discarded previously because of their lack of significance were added back into the final model if they significantly improved its predictive power. We based formal checks for outliers and influential observations on leverage statistics and standardized residuals (32). Collinearity was assessed by variance inflation factors and condition numbers (32); no adjustments were needed. Prespecified interactions between PCB, DDE, and specific control variables (age and sex) were also tested for significance. Predicted scores on the cognitive tests were obtained from the final regression model.

Because serum lead and mercury concentrations were slightly elevated in fish eaters, we also evaluated the effects of these two contaminants on the various cognitive outcomes. As with PCB and DDE, lead and mercury were entered on the log scale and nondetectable values were replaced with one-half the detection limit. However, unlike the primary exposure variables, lead and mercury were retained in the final model only if they significantly (p [is less than] 0.10) added to its predictive power.

Results

Neuropsychological evaluations and blood samples were available from 180 subjects. However, one male non-fish eater who reported recent carotid artery surgery and had severely impaired cognitive performance was excluded a priori from all statistical analyses, leaving a final sample size of 179 (101 fish eaters and 78 non-fish eaters). All participants were white, lived independently, and were in good general health. The median age was 64.3 years (range 49-86 years), 42.5% were male, and 48% had a high school education or less. Approximately 57% of the sample had an annual income of [is less than] $40,000.

In the final sample of 179 people, total serum PCBs ranged from below the detection limit to 75 ng/g, with a median of 7.9 ng/g. DDE values ranged from nondetectable to 145 ng/g, with a median of 8.1 ng/g.

Measures of Memory and Learning

After controlling for potential confounders, higher PCB exposure was associated with lower scores on the verbal delayed recall (logical memory) portion of the WMS (Table 4). PCB was significant in the initial bivariate analysis (p = 0.009) and remained significant in the final multivariate regression model (p = 0.001). The negative beta coefficient for PCB (on the log scale) indicates an inverse relationship between PCB exposure and verbal delayed recall. DDE was not significant in the initial bivariate analysis (p = 0.312), but became significant in the presence of PCB, age, and the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale vocabulary score (p = 0.016). The beta coefficient for DDE was positive, indicating that higher DDE exposure was associated with higher scores on verbal delayed recall. Lead and mercury exposure did not significantly impact verbal delayed recall and were not retained in the final model.

Table 4. Covariates, [Beta]-coefficients, and significance levels of
neuropsychological outcome measures.

                                 Covariates included
Outcome measure                  in final model                  No.

Memory and Learning
  Wechsler Memory Scale
    Logical memory
      Immediate                  Education, income,              166(b)
                                   vocabulary, blurred vision,
                                   peptic ulcer, NSAIDS
      Delayed                    Age, vocabulary, smoking        179
                                   status, blurred vision,
                                   antibiotics, NSAIDS
    Visual reproduction
        (referent > 10.0)
      Immediate(c)               Age, vocabulary, health         178
                                   self-rating, kidney
                                   disease, acetaminophen
      Delayed                    Age, vocabulary, nonleisure     174(d)
                                   activity level, arthritis,
                                   hardening of the arteries,
                                   bladder problems,
                                   acetaminophen, vitamin use
  California Verbal
        Learning Test
    List A, Trial 1              Age, sex, vocabulary, leisure   175(e)
                                   activity level, allergies,
                                   hardening of the arteries,
                                   kidney disease, "other"
                                   motor conditions (e.g.,
                                   carpal tunnel syndrome),
                                   calcium channel blockers,
                                   diabetes medications, sex
                                   hormone replacement
                                   therapy, antidepressants
    List A, Trials 1-5 Total     Age, sex, vocabulary, Beck      176(f)
                                   depression score, leisure
                                   activity level, allergies,
                                   diabetes, "other" motor
                                   conditions
    List A, short delay          Age, sex, income, vocabulary,   164(b)
        free recall                asthma, numbness/tingling
                                   in extremities
    List A, long delay           Age, sex, marital status,       177
        free recall                vocabulary, hypoglycemia,
                                   numbness/tingling in
                                   extremities, kidney disease
    Learning slope               Education, asthma, chronic      177
                                   bronchitis, hypertension,
                                   history of near-drowning
                                   incident, calcium channel
                                   blockers, antidepressants,
                                   diabetes medication
    List B, adjusted for         List A trial 1, age, sex,       177
        List A Trial 1             vocabulary, bladder
                                   trouble, anemia, diuretics,
                                   narcotics/psychotropic
                                   medication
    Perseverations(c)            Employment status,              177
        (referent > 0)             vocabulary, emphysema,
                                   "other" motor conditions,
                                   fatigue, diuretics, sex
                                   hormone replacement
                                   therapy, "other"
                                   medications (e.g.,
                                   digestive aids, laxatives)
    Semantic Cluster Ratio       Vocabulary, smoking status,     174(g)
                                   blurred vision, NSAIDS, sex
                                   hormone replacement
                                   therapy, potassium
                                   replacement
    Discriminability(c)          Age, sex, vocabulary, state     177
        (referent > 94%)           anxiety, acetaminophen,
                                   vitamin use
    Recognition hits vs. long    Sex, vocabulary, employment     177
        delay free recall(c)       status, smoking status,
        (referent < 36.3)          hardening of the arteries,
                                   numbness/tingling in
                                   extremities, diabetes
                                   medication, sex hormone
                                   replacement therapy
Executive function
  Stroop color-word test
    Interference score(h)        Income, state anxiety,          164(b)
                                   hypoglycemia
  Wisconsin Card Sorting Test
    Number of categories         Age, vocabulary, arthritis,     178
        completed(c)               "other" motor condition,
        (referent = 6)             diabetes medication
    Failure to maintain set(c)   Age, state anxiety, "other"     178
        (referent > 0)             motor condition, blurred
                                   vision, ACE inhibitors,
                                   steroidal hormones
    Perseverative responses(c)   Age, education, frequent        178
        (referent < 22)            headaches
    Percent perseverative        Age, vocabulary, history of     178
        errors(c)                  near-drowning incident,
        (referent < 16.05)         frequent headaches,
                                   antidepressants,
                                   beta blockers
  Trail-Making Test
    Trail A(i)                   Age, vocabulary, smoking        178
                                   status, emphysema, balance
                                   trouble, diabetes
                                   medication, sex hormone
                                   replacement therapy
    Trail B(i)                   Age, education, vocabulary,     177
                                   state anxiety, cancer
                                   (current), hardening of the
                                   arteries, beta blockers
  Short Category Test
    Total errors                 Age, vocabulary, kidney         178
                                   disease,
                                   anti-arrhythmatics,
                                   acetaminophen,
                                   antidepressants,
                                   vitamin use
Visual-spatial function
  Hooper Visual Organization
        Test(c)
  Total correct                  Age, sex, vocabulary,           178
        (referent > 25)            ACE inhibitors
  WAIS-R
  Digit Symbol raw score         Age, sex, vocabulary,           176(e)
                                   education, stiff muscles,
                                   headache, diuretics,
                                   acetaminophen

                                      LogPCB             LogDDE

                                 [Beta]             [Beta]
Outcome measure                   (a)     p-Value    (a)     p-value

Memory and Learning
  Wechsler Memory Scale
    Logical memory
      Immediate                   -0.33    0.44       0.42    0.24
      Delayed                     -1.33    0.001      0.85    0.02
    Visual reproduction
        (referent > 10.0)
      Immediate(c)                 0.07    0.82      -0.07    0.79
      Delayed                     -0.23    0.39       0.01    1.0
  California Verbal
        Learning Test
    List A, Trial 1               -0.42    0.04       0.06    0.71
    List A, Trials 1-5 Total      -1.33    0.16       0.39    0.63
    List A, short delay            0.05    0.86      -0.11    0.68
        free recall
    List A, long delay            -0.43    0.17       0.44    0.11
        free recall
    Learning slope                -0.01    0.95       0.04    0.44
    List B, adjusted for           0.26    0.15      -0.06    0.70
        List A Trial 1
    Perseverations(c)             -0.21    0.43      -0.17    0.47
        (referent > 0)
    Semantic Cluster Ratio        -0.24    0.006      0.05    0.51
    Discriminability(c)            0.44    0.12      -0.24    0.34
        (referent > 94%)
    Recognition hits vs. long      0.03    0.91       0.41    0.12
        delay free recall(c)
        (referent < 36.3)
Executive function
  Stroop color-word test
    Interference score(h)          0.13    0.88      -0.35    0.63
  Wisconsin Card Sorting Test
    Number of categories          -0.33    0.24      -0.21    0.40
        completed(c)
        (referent = 6)
    Failure to maintain set(c)    -0.15    0.57       0.27    0.24
        (referent > 0)
    Perseverative responses(c)    -0.28    0.28      -0.08    0.74
        (referent < 22)
    Percent perseverative         -0.42    0.13      -0.08    0.73
        errors(c)
        (referent < 16.05)
  Trail-Making Test
    Trail A(i)                     0.01    0.67      -0.01    0.73
    Trail B(i)                    -0.01    0.90       0.04    0.16
  Short Category Test
    Total errors                  -0.15    0.91      -0.57    0.63
Visual-spatial function
  Hooper Visual Organization
        Test(c)
  Total correct                   -0.24    0.37       0.32    0.18
        (referent > 25)
  WAIS-R
  Digit Symbol raw score           1.29    0.14      -1.44    0.06

Abbreviations: ACE, angiotensin-converting enzyme; NSAIDS,
nonsteroidal anti-inflammatory drugs

(a) Regression coefficient from multiple regression analysis.

(b) Thirteen participants with missing values for income were
eliminated from the final model.

(c) Based on logistic regression; variables dichotomized based on
median value for entire sample, except where noted.

(d) Four participants with missing values for nonleisure activity level
were eliminated.

(e) Two influential observations removed.

(f) One influential observation removed.

(g) Three influential observations removed.

(h) Color--word card raw score, adjusted for color card raw score.

(i) Time in seconds, log transformed.

To further illustrate the negative impact of PCB exposure on verbal delayed recall, we divided the sample into quartiles based on PCB exposure and calculated observed and predicted recall scores (Table 5). All but 5 of the 45 subjects in the highest PCB exposure quartile were fish eaters. A clear downward trend in verbal recall scores was seen with increasing PCB exposure.

Table 5. Memory and learning test scores by PCB exposure quartile.

             WMS logical memory, delayed recall

PCB Level              Observed        Predicted
(ng/mL)     No.   (mean [+ or -] SD)     mean

ND(a)-4.6    44   9.67 [+ or -] 3.4      9.72
4.7-7.8      45   9.09 [+ or -] 5.2      9.26
7.9-13.8     45   8.39 [+ or -] 4.2      8.28
13.9-75.0    45   7.66 [+ or -] 3.9      7.54

                CVLT Semantic Cluster Ratio

PCB Level              Observed        Predicted
(ng/mL)     No.   (mean [+ or -] SD)     mean

ND(a)-4.6    43   2.41 [+ or -] 0.9      2.31
4.7-7.8      44   2.20 [+ or -] 0.9      2.29
7.9-13.8     45   2.13 [+ or -] 0.8      2.12
13.9-75.0    43   1.88 [+ or -] 0.7      1.89

                   CVLT List A, Trial 1

PCB Level              Observed        Predicted
(ng/mL)     No.   (mean [+ or -] SD)     mean

ND(a)-4.6    44   7.64 [+ or -] 2.1      7.53
4.7-7.8      45   7.31 [+ or -] 2.3      7.26
7.9-13.8     45   6.91 [+ or -] 2.0      7.10
13.9-75.0    43   6.14 [+ or -] 1.5      6.11

(a) Below the detection limit of 3.0 ng/mL.

Higher PCB exposure was also related to lower scores on two outcomes from the CVLT, the semantic duster ratio (p = 0.006), and List A, Trial 1 (p = 0.037). DDE, lead, and mercury exposure were not related to these outcomes. The observed and predicted scores for the Semantic Cluster Ratio and List A, Trial 1, by PCB exposure quartile are shown in Table 5. For both tests, we observed a clear downward trend in scores with increasing PCB exposure.

Measures of Executive and Visual--Spatial Function

In contrast to the measures of memory and learning, PCB, DDE, lead, and mercury exposure were not associated with poorer scores on any of the tests of executive or visual--spatial function. The final models including covariates, beta coefficients, and p-values for each outcome variable are shown in Table 4.

Age and Sex Interactions

PCB and DDE body burdens increase with age (9), and animal studies suggest that certain PCB-related cognitive deficits may be sex-specific (33). Therefore, we also tested age by exposure and sex by exposure interaction terms for statistical significance. However, none of the PCB or DDE by age or sex interactions was significant.

Discussion and Conclusions

This study suggests, for the first time, that PCB exposure during adulthood may be associated with impairments in certain aspects of memory and learning. Older adults exposed to PCBs through consumption of sport-caught Great Lakes fish had lower scores on the verbal delayed recall portion of the WMS as well as on two measures of the CVLT. In contrast, visual memory and other aspects of cognition including executive and visual--spatial function appeared to be unaffected by PCB exposure. Other contaminants in the fish, including DDE, mercury, and lead, did not appear to have any negative impact on cognitive function. Earlier we reported that fine motor function also seemed to be unimpaired by PCB or DDE exposure (13).

Wechsler Memory Scale Logical Memory

Perhaps the most striking effect of PCB exposure was the relationship between higher PCB exposure and delayed recall on the logical memory portion of the WMS. This test assesses the individual's memory for two paragraph-length passages (stories) 30 min after they are read aloud by the examiner. Participants with serum PCB concentrations in the upper quartile ([is greater than or equal to] 13.9 ng/g) scored, on average, about 2 points lower than those in the lowest quartile (9.67 vs. 7.66). Furthermore, 17 of the 45 people (38%) in the highest PCB exposure quartile had scores that were below the 25th percentile of the distribution ([is less than] 6), whereas only 4 of 44 individuals (9%) in the lowest quartile had scores below the 25th percentile. Overall, the performance on WMS logical memory delayed recall differed by PCB exposure (Cochran-Mantel-Haenszel test, 3 degrees of freedom; p = 0.03). In contrast to this clear effect of PCB exposure on verbal delayed recall on the WMS, there was not a significant effect of PCB exposure on verbal delayed recall as measured by the CVLT. Evidence for a negative impact on verbal delayed recall would be stronger if both these measures were affected. However, the two tests differ in several important ways, and these may make the WMS a more sensitive measure of deficits in delayed recall than the CVLT. The WMS logical memory test uses coherent stories, whereas the CVLT uses a list of objects presented in a standardized order. Also, the stories are presented only once and immediate recall is assessed only once. In contrast, the word list is presented five times for immediate recall, then both free and cued recall are assessed after a short delay. Finally, both tests include a surprise delayed recall, but the WMS uses a 30-min delay, whereas the CVLT uses a shorter 20-min delay.

The positive association between DDE exposure and verbal delayed recall on the WMS is very puzzling. DDE was significant only when in the presence of PCB and two other variables, age and vocabulary score, which suggests a complex higher order interaction between these variables. There was no indication of a similar relationship on any of the other cognitive outcome measures.

California Verbal Learning Test

The strongest negative effect of PCB exposure among the CVLT variables was seen on semantic clustering. The word lists for this test consist of 16 words, four in each of four categories (e.g., tools, fruits). Semantic clustering refers to the extent to which the subject groups the words by category during recall. Use of semantic clustering requires the subject to actively organize the words by their meaning. This results in more efficient encoding to long-term memory than do other strategies such as serial order clustering (34). Immediate recall uses working memory, a transient form of memory that keeps information "online" for a short period of time while it is being actively manipulated (35), but delayed recall, which occurs after 20-30 min of unrelated testing, must rely on long-term memory (36). Thus, it is not surprising that impairments were seen on both semantic clustering and verbal delayed recall. It is unclear why PCB exposure was related to poorer verbal delayed recall on the WMS logical memory test, but not on the CVLT, but it is possible that efficient semantic clustering is more critical for memory of a coherent story than for a random list of words.

Statistical Issues

In this study multiple statistical tests were performed on multiple cognitive outcomes, and only a few significant effects of contaminant exposure were observed. This raises the possibility that the significant findings were spurious associations. We believe that this is unlikely because four chemicals, PCBs, DDE, mercury, and lead, were evaluated as exposure variables, but all of the significant negative associations were related to PCB exposure. Also, several aspects of cognitive functioning including memory and learning, executive function, and visual--spatial function were assessed, but all of the negative effects were seen on the tests of verbal memory and learning. Children exposed to PCBs in utero via maternal consumption of Great Lakes fish also showed impairments, primarily on tests involving verbal and memory function (1-4). Despite these consistencies, it would be prudent to interpret the findings with caution until they have been replicated in an independent exposure cohort.

Conclusions

This study is the first to find that adults exposed to elevated levels of PCBs may be at risk of neuropsychological impairment. The results should be important in the risk assessment arena because current fish consumption advisories focus heavily on protecting the pregnant woman and fetus. As with children exposed in utero, certain aspects of verbal memory and learning seem to be the primary targets for PCB-related impairments in older adults. Executive, visual--spatial, and motor (13) function did not appear to he affected. Furthermore, other neurotoxic contaminants in the fish including DDE, mercury, and lead did not have any negative impact on cognitive or motor function. It is possible, however, that subtle changes in other outcomes or those related to other contaminants were missed because the modest sample size restricted statistical power. It is also possible that greater exposure to PCBs and/or these other contaminants would result in additional impairments. For example, workers exposed to PCBs in capacitor manufacturing plants have been reported to have serum PCB levels 10- to 100-fold higher than the levels that are typical in Great Lakes fish eaters (3,7). These workers may be at considerably greater risk of neuropsychological impairments than are Great Lakes fish eaters, and are a group that warrants further study.

REFERENCES AND NOTES

(1.) Jacobson SW, Fein GG, Jacobson JL, Schwartz PM, Dowler JK. The effect of intrauterine PCB exposure on visual recognition memory. Child Dev 56:853-860 (1985).

(2.) Jacobson JL, Jacobson SW, Humphrey HEB. Effects of in utero exposure to polychlorinated biphenyls and related contaminants on cognitive functioning in young children. J Pediatr 116:38-45 (1990).

(3.) Jacobson JL, Jacobson SW, Padgett R, Brumitt G, Billings R. Effects of prenatal PCB exposure on cognitive processing efficiency and sustained attention. Dev Psychol 28:297-306 (1992).

(4.) Jacobson JL, Jacobson SW. Intellectual impairment in children exposed to polychlorinated biphenyls in utero. N Engl J Med 335:783-789 (1996).

(5.) Darvil T, Lonky E, Reihman J, Stewart P, Pagano J. Prenatal exposure to PCBs and infant performance on the Fagan Test of Infant Intelligence. Neurotoxicology 21:1029-1038 (2000).

(6.) Patandin S, Lanting CI, Mulder PGH, Boersma ER, Sauer PJJ, Weisglas-Kuperus N. Effects of environmental exposure to polychlorinated biphenyls and dioxins on cognitive abilities in Dutch children at 42 months of age. J Pediatr 134:33-41 (1999).

(7.) Chen YCG, Guo YL, Hsu CC, Rogan WJ. Cognitive development of Yu-Cheng (oil disease) children prenatally exposed to heat-degraded PCBs. JAMA 268:3213-3218 (1992).

(8.) Gladen B, Rogan W. Effects of perinatal polychlorinated biphenyls and dichlorodiphenyl dichloroethane on later development. J Pediatr 119:58-63 (1991).

(9.) Humphrey HEB. Chemical contaminants in the Great Lakes: the human health aspect. In: Toxic Contaminants and Ecosystem Health: A Great Lakes Focus (Evans MS, ed). New York:Wiley, 1988;153-165.

(10.) Amenta F, Zaccheo D, Collier WL. Neurotransmitters, neuroreceptors, and aging. Mech Ageing Dev 61:249-273 (1991).

(11.) Humphrey HEB. Evaluation of Humans Exposed to Waterborne Chemicals in the Great Lakes. EPA Report no. CR-807192. Lansing, MI:Michigan Department of Public Health, 1983.

(12.) Schantz SL, Sweeney AM, Gardiner JC, Humphrey HEB, McCaffrey RJ, Gasior DM, Srikanth KR, Budd ML. Neuropsychological assessment of an aging population of Great Lakes fisheaters. Toxicol Ind Health 12:403-417 (1996).

(13.) Schantz SL, Gardiner JC, Gasior DM, Sweeney AM, Humphrey HEB, McCaffrey RJ. Motor function in aging Great Lakes fisheaters. Environ Res 80:S46-S56 (1999).

(14.) Wechsler D. A standardized memory scale for clinical use. J Psychol 19:87-95 (1945).

(15.) Russell EW. A multiple scoring method for the assessment of complex memory functions. J Consult Clin Psychol 43:800-809 (1975).

(16.) Power DB, Logue PE, McCarty SM, Rosenstiel AK, Ziesat HA. Brief report: inter-rater reliability of the Russell revision of the Wechsler Memory Scale: an attempt to clarify some ambiguities in scoring. J Clin Neuropsychol 1:343-345 (1979).

(17.) Wechsler D. WAIS-R Manual. New York:The Psychological Corporation, 1981.

(18.) Heaton RK. Wisconsin Card Sorting Test. Odessa, FL:Psychological Assessment Resources, Inc., 1981.

(19.) Harris ME, Heaton RK, Tuttle K, Schinka JA. Wisconsin Card Sorting Test: Scoring Program (IBM version 4.0). Odessa, FL:Psychological Assessment Resources, Inc., 1988.

(20.) Delis DC, Kramer JH, Kaplan E, Ober BA. California Verbal Learning Test, Research Edition. San Antonio, TX:Psychological Corporation, 1987.

(21.) Fidlund AJ, Delis DC. California Verbal Learning Test, Research Edition: IBM User's Guide Version 1.2. San Antonio, TX:Psychological Corporation, 1987.

(22.) Reitan RM, Wolfson D. The Halstead-Reitan Neuropsychological Test Battery: Theory and Clinical Interpretation. Tucson, AZ:Neuropsychology Press, 1993.

(23.) Golden CJ. Stroop Color and Word Test: A Manual for Clinical and Experimental Uses. Wood Dale, IL:Stoelting Company, 1978.

(24.) Hooper HE. Hooper Visual Organization Test (VOT). Los Angeles, CA:Western Psychological Services, 1983.

(25.) Wetzel L, Boll TJ. Short Category Test, Booklet Format. Los Angeles, CA:Western Psychological Services, 1987.

(26.) Spielberger CD, Gorsuch RW, Lushene RE. State-trait Anxiety Inventory. Palo Alto, CA:Consulting Psychologists Press, 1970.

(27.) Beck AT, Ward CH, Mendelsohn M, Mock J, Erbaugh J. An Inventory For Measuring Depression. Arch Gen Psychiatry 4:561-571 (1961).

(28.) Humphrey HEB, Gardiner JC, Pandya JR, Sweeney AM, Gasior DM, McCaffrey RJ, Schantz SL. PCB Congener profile in the serum of humans consuming Great Lakes fish. Environ Health Perspect 108:167-172 (2000).

(29.) Jensen AA. Polychlorinated biphenyls (PCBs), polychlorodibenzo-p-dioxins (PCDDs) and polychlorodibenzofurans (PCDFs) in human milk, blood and adipose tissue. Sci Total Environ 64:259-293 (1987).

(30.) Cohen YC, Rubin HR, Freedman L, Mozes B. Use of a clustered model to identify factors affecting hospital length of stay. J Clin Epidemiol 52:1031-1036 (1999).

(31.) Fried LP, Kronmal RA, Newman AB, Bild DE, Mittelmark MB, Polak JF, Robbins JA, Gardin JM. Risk factors for 5-year mortality in older adults: the cardiovascular health study. JAMA 279:585-592 (1998).

(32.) Cook RD, Weisberg S. Residuals and influence in regression. New York:Chapman and Hall, 1982.

(33.) Schantz SL, Moshtaghian J, Ness DK. Spatial learning deficits in adult rats exposed to ortho-substituted PCB congeners during gestation and lactation. Fundam Appl Toxicol 26:117-126 (1995).

(34.) Craik FIM. Encoding and retrieval effects in human memory: a partial review. In: Attention and Performance, IX (Long J, Baddeley AD, eds). Hillsdale, NJ:Erlbaum, 1981.

(35.) Goldman-Rakic PS. Circuitry of Primate Prefrontal Cortex and Regulation of Behavior by Representational Memory. In: Handbook of Physiology -- The Nervous System (Plum F, Mountcastle V, eds). Bethesda, MD:American Physiological Society, 1987;373-417.

(36.) Reitman JS. Mechanisms of forgetting in short-term memory. Cognit Psychol 2:185-195 (1971).

(37.) Lawton RW, Ross MR, Feingold J, Brown JF Jr. Effects of PCB exposure on biochemical and hematological findings in capacitor workers. Environ Health Perspect 60:165-184 (1985).

Susan L. Schantz,(1) Donna M. Gasior,(2) Elena Polverejan,(2) Robert J. McCaffrey,(3) Anne M. Sweeney,(4) Harold E.B. Humphrey,(5) and Joseph C. Gardiner(2)

(1) Department of Veterinary Biosciences, University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign, Urbana, Illinois, USA; (2) Department of Epidemiology, College of Human Medicine, Michigan State University, East Lansing, Michigan, USA; (3) Department of Psychology, University at Albany, State University of New York, Albany, New York, USA; (4) School of Public Health, University of Texas Health Science Center at Houston, Houston, Texas, USA; (5) Institute of Environmental Toxicology, Michigan State University, East Lansing, Michigan, USA

Address correspondence to S.L. Schantz, Department of Veterinary Biosciences, College of Veterinary Medicine, University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign, 2001 S. Lincoln Avenue, Urbana, IL 61802 USA. Telephone: (217) 333-6230. Fax: (217) 244-1652. E-mail: schantz@uiuc.edu

This research was supported by grant 598339 from the Agency for Toxic Substances and Disease Registry (ATSDR). Its contents are solely the responsibility of the authors and do not necessarily represent the official views of the ATSDR.

Received 15 November 2000; accepted 18 December 2000.

COPYRIGHT 2001 National Institute of Environmental Health Sciences
COPYRIGHT 2004 Gale Group

联系我们|关于我们|网站声明
国家哲学社会科学文献中心版权所有