首页    期刊浏览 2024年10月06日 星期日
登录注册

文章基本信息

  • 标题:Cognitive effects of endocrine-disrupting chemicals in animals - Review
  • 作者:Susan L. Schantz
  • 期刊名称:Environmental Health Perspectives
  • 印刷版ISSN:0091-6765
  • 电子版ISSN:1552-9924
  • 出版年度:2001
  • 卷号:Dec 2001
  • 出版社:OCR Subscription Services Inc

Cognitive effects of endocrine-disrupting chemicals in animals - Review

Susan L. Schantz

A large number of chemical pollutants including phthalates, alkylphenolic compounds, polychlorinated biphenyls and polychlorinated dibenzodioxins, organochlorine pesticides, bisphenol A, and metals including lead, mercury, and cadmium have the ability to disrupt endocrine function in animals. Some of these same chemicals have been shown to alter cognitive function in animals and humans. Became hormonally mediated events play a central role in central nervous system development and function, a number of researchers have speculated that the changes in cognitive function are mediated by the endocrine-like actions of these chemicals. In this paper we review the evidence that cognitive effects of chemicals classified as environmental endocrine disruptors are mediated by changes in hormonal function. We begin by briefly reviewing the role of gonadal steroids, thyroid hormones, and glucocorticoids in brain development and brain function. We then review the endocrine changes and cognitive effects that have been reported for selected endocrine-disrupting chemicals, discuss the evidence for causal relationships between endocrine disruption and cognitive effects, and suggest directions for future research. Key words: cognitive function, endocrine disruptors, learning, memory. Environ Health Perspect 109:1197-1206 (2001). [Online 14 November 2001]

http://ehpnet1.niehs.nih.gov/docs/2001/109p1197-1206schantz/ abstract.html

**********

There is increasing concern about chemical pollutants that have the ability to act as hormone mimics. Because of a structural similarity with an endogenous hormone, an ability to interact with hormone transport proteins, or an ability to disrupt hormone metabolism, these chemicals have the potential to mimic, or in some cases block, the effects of the endogenous hormone. In either case, these chemicals serve to disrupt the normal actions of endogenous hormones and thus have become known as "endocrine disruptors." A large number of environmental pollutants including phthalates, alkylphenolic compounds, polychlorinated biphenyls (PCBs), polychlorinated dibenzodioxins, organochlorine pesticides, bisphenol A, and heavy metals including lead, mercury, and cadmium have been shown to disrupt endocrine function in animals. Because hormonally mediated events play a central role in central nervous system (CNS) development and function, there is speculation that some of the cognitive deficits that arise from developmental exposure to environmental chemicals may be the result of endocrine disruption.

For example, thyroid hormone is essential for proper neuronal proliferation, cell migration, and differentiation in the developing mammalian brain (1). Disruption of the thyroid system during development has been shown to result in permanent changes in neurochemical (2), morphologic (3,4), and neurobehavioral end points (5-7). There are a number of environmental contaminants that have been shown to affect thyroid system function (8). One example is PCBs. PCBs alter thyroid function through multiple mechanisms including direct toxic effects to the thyroid gland, induction of thyroid hormone metabolism via UDP-glucuronyl transferase, and interaction with thyroid hormone carrier proteins (9). Further complicating matters is the fact that PCBs have multiple endocrine effects, impacting not only thyroid hormones but the gonadal (10-12) and adrenal steroid systems (13) as well. PCBs are just one example of the many environmental contaminants capable of disrupting one or more endocrine systems. When one considers that human populations have body burdens of multiple contaminants capable of affecting multiple endocrine systems, the potential human health risk could be significant. Therefore, an understanding of the endocrine-disrupting potential of these chemicals and subsequent neurobehavioral changes that result is an important task facing environmental toxicologists, endocrinologists, and behaviorists.

The goals of the current review are to discuss the evidence for cognitive changes resulting from exposure of laboratory animals to chemicals classified as endocrine disruptors and to examine the extent to which these cognitive changes appear to be mediated by changes in hormonal function. The discussion is focused on the three hormonal systems--the gonadal steroids, the thyroid hormones and the glucocorticoids, for which the richest data set on endocrine disruption exists. Although other hormones, growth hormone in particular, play important roles in brain development and behavior, relatively little is known about the effects of chemical pollutants on other hormonal systems. We begin by reviewing the roles of the gonadal steroids, thyroid hormones, and glucocorticoids in brain development and function. We then review the endocrine changes and cognitive effects that have been reported for several major chemical pollutants including PCBs, dioxins, and lead, discuss the evidence for causal relationships between the endocrine disruption and cognitive effects, and conclude by highlighting important directions for further research.

Role of Hormones in Brain Development and Cognition

Estrogens and androgens. The role of gonadal steroids in the development of brain areas involved in reproduction has been recognized for many years (14). The brain is particularly sensitive to the differentiating effects of gonadal hormones during a critical period early in development. The absence of testicular hormones during this period allows the development of a female pattern of behavior and neuroendocrine function. Conversely, the presence of testicular hormones allows the development of a male pattern. In the rat, the critical period for sexual differentiation of the brain starts a few days before birth and ends approximately 10 days after birth. The brain is exquisitely sensitive to estrogens and androgens during this time. Female rats treated with testosterone during the critical period permanently lose the capacity to secrete leutinizing hormone in a cyclical fashion in response to estrogen stimulation and do not show typical female reproductive behaviors, such as lordosis. Conversely, they have the capacity to exhibit masculine sexual behaviors in response to administration of testosterone. Male rats castrated during the critical period are unable to display typical male sexual behaviors after administration of testosterone in adulthood, but will show lordosis in response to estrogen treatment.

In the rat, sexual differentiation primarily occurs through the aromatization of testosterone to estrogen locally within the brain (14). Estrogen then acts to organize neural components in the hypothalamus and preoptic area in a male-specific pattern. In the absence of testosterone, the hypothalamus develops in a female-specific pattern. The most striking sex difference in brain anatomy is present in an area within the medial preoptic area (MPOA) known as the sexually dimorphic nucleus of the preoptic area (SDN-POA). This nucleus is 5- to 7-fold larger in male rats than in females (15). The nucleus is dimorphic not only in terms of its volume, but also in terms of the neurotransmitters in the cell bodies of the neurons comprising the nucleus and in the fibers innervating it (16). Another region, the ventral medial nucleus of the hypothalamus (VMN), is also sexually dimorphic (17). The roles of these hypothalamic nuclei in reproductive behavior are not completely understood, but the VMN appears to be involved in the lordosis response in female rats (17), and the SDN-POA has been implicated in the execution of coital behavior in male rats (16). Some aspects of sexual differentiation of the brain, including the development of sexually dimorphic patterns of social play, appear to be regulated by the direct actions of androgens in the brain, rather than by the aromatization of androgens to estrogen (18). A similar process of sexual differentiation appears to occur in the brains of all mammalian species including humans. However, in other species, particularly nonhuman primates and humans, the mechanisms are not as well understood.

Recently it has become clear that early exposure to estrogens and androgens has important actions in areas of the brain that are not involved in reproduction. One of these is the hippocampus, which plays an important role in learning and memory, particularly spatial learning and memory (19). Sex differences in spatial learning have been reported by many investigators and appear to be present in humans as well as in animals (20). In general, men outperform women on tasks that require spatial skills. Male rodents also make fewer errors than females on spatial learning tasks. The work of Williams et al. (21) suggests that these differences could be due to differences in the way males and females process spatial information. Male rats appear to attend primarily to geometric cues (the shape of the environment), whereas females use a combination of landmarks and geometry, to locate a target.

As reviewed by McEwen et al. (19), sex differences in hippocampal morphology also exist. These include differences in the number of spines on the apical dendritic shafts of CA3 pyramidal cells (22), as well as differences in the number of mossy fiber synapses to these cells (23). Male rats have more spines on the apical dendrites and more mossy fiber synapses. They also have a larger and more asymmetric dentate gyrus (24). Neonatal testosterone treatment causes the female dentate gyrus to appear masculine and also improves the spatial learning ability of female rats (24). In contrast, neonatal castration of male rats results in a female pattern of spatial learning (20).

The rat hippocampus shows a transient increase in estrogen receptors for a short period during perinatal development (25). This coincides with transient expression of the aromatase that converts testosterone to estrogen (26). Thus, hippocampal estrogen receptors of male rats are exposed to locally generated estrogen during a brief period early in development. Just as in the hypothalamus, this appears to lead to sexual differentiation of hippocampal structure and function. Exposure to chemicals that perturb the delicate balance of gonadal hormones during early development could result in changes in hippocampal morphology and alter the normal pattern of male/female differences in spatial learning.

Estrogen receptors are sparsely distributed in the adult hippocampus, but recent research indicates that estrogen continues to play an important role in the hippocampus during adulthood. Morphologic studies have shown that estrogen induces cyclical changes in dendritic spine density on pyramidal cells in the CA1 region of the hippocampus in female rats (27). More recent in vitro studies have demonstrated that the estradiol-induced increase in spine density increases the sensitivity of the cells to N-methyl-D-aspartate receptor-mediated synaptic input (28). There are a number of studies suggesting that learning ability varies over the course of the estrous cycle in female rats (29-33), although several other studies have not found any changes (34,35). In addition, estrogen replacement therapy appears to preserve memory function in post-menopausal women (36,37), as well as in ovariectomized female rats (38). Thus, exposure to environmental chemicals that have estrogenic or antiestrogenic actions could also impact cognitive function during adulthood and aging.

There are two estrogen receptor subtypes (ER[alpha] and ER[beta]) that are differentially distributed throughout the CNS. Some regions--including the neurons of the olfactory bulb; supraoptic, paraventricular, suprachiasmatic, and tuberal hypothalamic nuclei; zona incerta; ventral tegmental area; and cerebellar Purkinje cells--contain only ER[beta] (39). In contrast, only ER[alpha] is found in the ventromedial hypothalamic nucleus and the subfornical organ. Neurons in many other brain regions contain both ER[alpha] and ER[beta]. However, the relative amounts of the two receptor subtypes vary by region. For example, Shughrue et al. (39) found that the cerebral cortex and hippocampus contain both ER[alpha] and ER[beta], but the relative amount of ER[beta] is much greater than ER[alpha] in these brain regions. Region-specific expression of ER[alpha] and ER[beta] may be important in determining the physiologic responses of neurons to estrogen action. Thus, if different environmental estrogens have different affinities for the two ER subtypes, they could potentially affect brain development and behavior in very different ways.

Thyroid hormones. The actions of thyroid hormones are vital for normal brain development (1). Thyroid hormones are involved in regulating many aspects of nervous system development including neuronal proliferation, cell migration, and differentiation. Neonatal hypothyroidism results in delayed myelinogenesis, alterations in cell migration, delayed or impaired neuronal differentiation and synaptogenesis, and alterations in neurotransmitter function (2-4). These morphologic and neurochemical changes are associated with permanent impairments in neurobehavioral function, including delayed reflex development, changes in motor activity and emotionality, and deficits in learning and memory (5,6,40). Although thyroid hormone imbalances during adulthood can also lead to cognitive and behavioral disturbances, these are usually completely reversible with appropriate hormone therapy.

Many of the biochemical and morphologic changes observed in the brains of neonatally hypothyroid rats appear to recover with time, but several brain regions, including the hippocampus, show persistent morphologic changes in response to early thyroid hormone manipulations (41). Early hypothyroidism results in hippocampal CA3 pyramidal cells with markedly stunted dendritic trees (42). The CA3 cells originate during the early embryonic period (43), but undergo extensive dendritic remodeling during the second and third postnatal weeks (42). The timing of these changes coincides with peak levels of thyroid hormones (44) and thyroid hormone receptor (45), which may explain the unusual sensitivity of the CA3 pyramidal cells to neonatal thyroid hormone imbalances. In contrast, hippocampal CA1 pyramidal cells do not undergo extensive dendritic restructuring during the postnatal thyroid hormone surge and appear to be less affected by neonatal thyroid hormone manipulations (42). Neonatal hypothyroidism also reduces the number of dentate gyrus granule cells (46) and impairs their dendritic arborization (42,47).

The cognitive effects of neonatal hypothyroidism reflect the fact that the hippocampus is one of the most severely damaged brain regions. Spatial learning and memory is severely impaired on the radial arm maze (5) and Morris water maze (6), as well as on other complex mazes (40). Congenitally hypothyroid children have cognitive deficits not unlike those observed in neonatally hypothyroid rats. These include impaired memory and spatial perception, as well as attentional problems (1). Subtle problems with hearing, speech, and word comprehension are also common. A large number of environmental chemicals are known or suspected of altering thyroid hormone function (8). Exposure to these chemicals during early development could potentially interfere with brain development and cause permanent deficits in cognitive function.

Glucocorticoids. Glucocorticoids also have a profound affect on brain development. In the rat, the first 2 weeks of life are characterized by low basal levels of corticosterone and hyporesponsiveness of the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal (HPA) axis to stressful stimuli (48). During this period, the brain is very sensitive to environmental or chemical manipulations. Various environmental stimuli including electric shock, heat stress, exposure to novelty, and handling of the neonate have been shown to produce long-lasting or permanent changes in brain glucocorticoid receptor expression and behavior. The best studied among these is the effect of early handling (49). Rats handled during infancy have a permanent increase in glucocorticoid receptors in the hippocampus, which results in greater hippocampal sensitivity to glucocorticoids and better regulatory control of the stress response. Less corticosterone is secreted in response to stress, and levels return to baseline more rapidly. Over the life span, this translates into lower cumulative exposure to glucocorticoids, which results in less hippocampal cell loss and less decline in memory function during aging (50). Interestingly, mothers of handled pups spend more time licking and manipulating their pups, and it is these alterations in maternal behavior that seem to mediate the handling effect on glucocorticoid function (49).

Unlike the positive effects of neonatal handling, exposure to elevated levels of glucocorticoids during the period when the HPA axis is normally quiescent can have detrimental effects on brain development (48). Cell proliferation ceases early, and axonal outgrowth, myelination, formation of dendritic spines, and synaptogenesis are retarded. Some of these effects may be reversible if the exposure to exogenous glucocorticoids ends early enough, but recovery is seldom complete. As with thyroid hormone imbalances, these changes in brain morphology are accompanied by deficits in behavioral function, including deficits in learning and altered motor activity (48).

Many studies have shown that hippocampal development and function are exquisitely sensitive to the circulating levels of glucocorticoids. Formation of the granule cells of the dentate gyrus, in particular, is closely linked to adrenal steroids (51). Immediately before birth the levels of glucocorticoids are high and the number of dentate gyrus granule cells that incorporate [sup.3]H-thymidine is low. As discussed above, after birth, the levels of adrenal steroids drop and remain low for roughly 2 weeks. During this period, the number of dentate gyrus granule cells that incorporate [sup.3]H-thymidine increases dramatically. As glucocorticoid levels rise again at the end of the hyporesponsive period, granule cell proliferation diminishes once again. In adulthood, the levels of adrenal steroids are relatively high and the rate of granule cell proliferation remains low. Injecting either developing pups or adult rats with corticosterone reduces the rate of granule cell proliferation, whereas removal of the adrenal gland increases proliferation. Paradoxically, adrenal steroids also suppress granule cell death. Throughout the life of the rat, the level of adrenal steroids correlates negatively with the number of degenerating dentate gyrus granule cells. In the adult animal, adrenalectomy results in massive granule cell death.

Normal levels of circulating adrenal steroids appear to be necessary for accurate performance on spatial tasks, which is not surprising given the role of the hippocampus in spatial learning and memory. Either increases or decreases in corticosterone impair spatial learning in the rat. Adrenalectomy has been shown to impair performance on the radial arm maze (52) and the Morris water maze (53). Conversely, restraint stress, which elevates corticosterone levels, also impairs radial arm maze performance (54). Chemicals that alter the levels of adrenal steroids or mimic or block their actions, would have the potential to alter dentate gyrus morphology and disturb spatial learning and memory. Although exposure during development is more likely to lead to permanent functional changes, cognitive deficits are possible regardless of whether exposure occurs during development or in adulthood.

Interactions between hormone systems. It is important to note that individual hormonal systems interact with each other in complex ways. Thus, the possibility exists for alterations in one specific hormonal pathway to cascade through multiple systems, producing nervous system effects that are complex and difficult to interpret (55). For example, thyroid hormones appear to be involved in mediating the effects of handling on glucocorticoid receptor expression (49). Handling activates the hypothalamic-pituitary-thyroid axis, increasing the levels of thyroid hormones. This results in increased 5-hydroxytryptamine turnover in the hippocampus, which in turn acts to permanently increase hippocampal glucocorticoid receptor expression. Direct neonatal treatment with thyroid hormone has the same effect, whereas treatment with the goitrogen propylthiouracil blocks the increase in hippocampal glucocorticoid receptor binding usually observed after handling (49).

Alterations in thyroid hormones during the critical period can also affect androgen-dependent sexual differentiation of the brain. Hyperthyroidism shortens the critical period for androgen exposure, whereas hypothyroidism prolongs it. In addition to these indirect effects, there is recent evidence that thyroid hormone may also inhibit estrogen's actions directly at the genomic level (56). Thus, agents that increase thyroid hormone bioavailability or mimic the actions of thyroid hormone might be expected to attenuate estrogen-mediated responses such as sexual differentiation of the brain, whereas agents that reduce or block thyroid hormone action would be expected to have the opposite effect.

Effects of Endocrine-Disrupting Chemicals on Cognition

A large number of synthetic chemicals have been identified as known or suspected endocrine disruptors (57). For the most part, these are compounds that have estrogenic or antiestrogenic actions (58) and/or disrupt thyroid function (8). A much smaller number of chemicals have been evaluated for effects on other endocrine systems such as androgens and adrenal steroids. Keith (59) has compiled a comparative list of environmental endocrine disruptors based on lists obtained from scientists at the U.S. Environmental Protection Agency (U.S. EPA), the Centers for Disease Control and Prevention (CDC), and the World Wildlife Fund (WWF). Although many chemicals appear on all three lists, there are also significant differences among the three lists. A total of 103 different chemicals are represented with 60, 48, and 68 chemicals appearing on the U.S. EPA, CDC, and WWF lists, respectively. The discrepancies between the three lists highlight the fact that we do not have adequate scientific data on many potential endocrine disruptors. Because of the limited scope of this review, we will limit our discussion primarily to those chemicals that all three sources identified as environmental endocrine disruptors. These fall into several broad chemical classes including phthalates, alkylphenolic compounds, organochlorine pesticides, PCBs, dioxins and furans, bisphenol A, and heavy metals (Table 1).

The lack of good scientific data on endocrine disruptors becomes even more obvious when one attempts to investigate the effects of these chemicals on cognitive function. Despite the fact that hormones play a central role in CNS development and function, few endocrine disruptors have been evaluated for cognitive effects in animal models, and few, if any, mechanistic studies directly relating changes in cognitive function to altered endocrine status have been conducted. Table 1 lists individual endocrine disruptors by chemical class, identifies some of the hormonal systems they act on, and indicates whether cognitive function has been assessed. Because few or no data exist for most of the chemicals on the list, it will be necessary to focus this discussion on a few examples for which cognitive effects have been documented.

PCBs and dioxins. PCBs and dioxins are widely dispersed, environmentally persistent organic compounds. PCBs were manufactured commercially in the United States from the 1930s through the 1970s and were widely used as dielectric fluids in capacitors and transformers (60). Dioxins are structurally similar compounds that are formed as unwanted by-products during the manufacture of certain herbicides and wood products. Dioxins are also formed during combustion of chlorinated compounds and are found in fly ash from municipal and hospital incinerators (61).

The endocrine-disrupting properties and cognitive effects of PCBs and dioxins have been extensively studied in animal models. Both have complex effects on multiple endocrine systems (10,62). PCBs and dioxins have been shown to alter thyroid function in rodents by multiple mechanisms, including direct toxic effects on the thyroid gland, induction of thyroid hormone metabolism via the UDP-glucuronyl transferases, and interactions with thyroid hormone plasma transport proteins, particularly transthyretin (9). A number of investigators have evaluated the effects of maternal PCB exposure on thyroid function of rat pups (63-65). Pup serum thyroxine ([T.sub.4]) levels are markedly reduced by PCB or dioxin exposure, but the levels of the active form of the hormone, triiodothyronine ([T.sub.3]), are generally unchanged, or only slightly reduced. A relationship between exposure to dioxins and PCBs and alterations in thyroid hormones has also been reported in human infants (66). Infants exposed to higher levels of PCBs and dioxins had lower free [T.sub.4] levels and higher thyroid-stimulating hormone levels. Thyroid hormone is transported to the brain as [T.sub.4] and then convened locally to [T.sub.3] (67). Based on this knowledge, it has been argued that the dramatic reductions in serum [T.sub.4] reported in rats after perinatal PCB exposure could place the brain at special risk for hypothyroid-related effects (68). However, recently Morse et al. (64) found that, although both serum and brain [T.sub.4] levels were reduced after fetal PCB exposure, brain [T.sub.3] levels remained normal or near normal. To complicate the situation even further, a recent report suggests that PCBs may actually act as thyroid hormone mimics in the brain (69). Exposure to the PCB mixture Aroclor 1254 caused marked reductions in circulating [T.sub.4] concentrations, yet elevations in the expression of two key thyroid-hormone responsive genes, RC3/neurogranin and myelin basic protein, were observed in the developing brain. Chemical goitrogens such as propylthiouracil and methimazole reduce the expression of these same genes (70, 71).

Commercial PCB mixtures have long been known to be estrogenic (11,12), but more recent studies focusing on individual PCB congeners have revealed a complex array of estrogenic and antiestrogenic effects (10). Certain congeners appear to act as estrogens in some assays and antiestrogens in others. Until recently, coplanar PCBs and dioxins were considered to be strictly antiestrogenic, but it now appears that coplanar PCBs can act as estrogens in some assays (72,73). PCBs and dioxins can also disrupt androgen production (74). The ability of dioxins to act as antiestrogens and antiandrogens has spawned a number of studies assessing the effects of in utero exposure on neuroendocrine function, reproductive behavior, and CNS morphology of male rats (75-77). Demasculinization and feminization of reproductive behavior, as well as feminization of neuroendocrine function, have been reported, but the only study that assessed CNS morphology did not find any evidence of altered sexual differentiation in the brain (76), so the mechanism for these effects remains uncertain. In contrast to the wealth of data available for dioxin, there are few studies that assess reproductive behavior and neuroendocrine function after in utero PCB exposure.

Early PCB exposure can alter function of the HPA axis (13), suppressing basal and stimulated corticosterone levels, but effects on this system have been less extensively studied than the estrogenic and thyrotoxic effects. High doses of dioxin alter adrenal steroid function in adult animals (78,79), but the effects of in utero exposure on functioning of the HPA axis have not been assessed. Glucocorticoid receptor binding was down-regulated in both the palate and thymus after early dioxin exposure (80,81). This suggests that there may be alterations in glucocorticoid receptor expression in other tissues, including the brain, after early dioxin exposure.

In summary, PCBs and dioxins have a number of documented endocrine-disrupting effects, which could act individually or in concert to alter CNS development and cognitive function. Neonatal hypothyroidism has profound effects on brain development and cognitive function. Many investigators have hypothesized that PCBs and dioxins alter behavioral function through their actions on thyroid hormones (1,9). Others have suggested that it is more likely that the actions of PCBs and dioxins on multiple hormone systems interact in complex ways to produce CNS effects (55).

Is there evidence to support the contention that PCBs and dioxins impair cognitive function through their endocrine-disrupting actions? It is clear from laboratory animal studies that developmental exposure to PCB mixtures or ortho-substituted PCB congeners results in long-lasting deficits in learning and memory. The evidence for learning deficits after exposure to dioxin or coplanar PCB congeners is not as clear. In fact, under some circumstances exposure to dioxin may facilitate learning. Early studies in monkeys exposed to complex mixtures of PCBs via maternal transfer during gestation and lactation found long-term deficits in spatial learning and memory (82). The monkeys were impaired on two types of spatial learning tasks: spatial discrimination-reversal learning and delayed spatial alternation. The deficit on the spatial alternation task was particularly striking. The PCB-exposed monkeys were never able to achieve control levels of performance, even after an extended period of testing. This pronounced deficit in spatial learning was observed when the monkeys were 4-6 years old, even though they had not been exposed to PCBs since they were weaned at 4 months of age. The monkeys were equally impaired at short and long delays, suggesting a deficit in learning or attentional processes rather than memory.

More recently, Rice and Hayward (83) exposed monkey infants to a mixture of PCB congeners formulated to represent the PCBs typically found in human breast milk, from birth to 20 weeks of age. Beginning at 3 years of age, the monkeys were tested on a series of learning tasks. As in the earlier study, the PCB-exposed monkeys showed a clear impairment in their ability to learn a delayed spatial alternation task. Again, the impairment in spatial alternation was interpreted as a learning decrement rather than a deficit in memory. Later, the monkeys in the Rice study were tested in several operant schedules, including a multiple fixed interval-fixed ratio schedule (84). The PCB-exposed monkeys showed retarded acquisition of the fixed interval schedule. The results of this study are particularly noteworthy because the tissue levels of PCBs after exposure were similar to the tissue levels typically observed in the human population.

In contrast to PCB-exposed monkeys, monkeys exposed to dioxin during development did not show any impairments in spatial learning (85). In fact, they did slightly better than control monkeys on both spatial discrimination-reversal learning and delayed spatial alternation (86). The dioxin-exposed monkeys were, however, impaired in their ability to learn nonspatial discrimination-reversal problems using color or shape as the relevant cues (85,86). Based on these discrepant findings in PCB- and dioxin-exposed monkeys, it has been suggested that the impaired spatial learning in the PCB-exposed monkeys could be related to the non--dioxin-like, ortho-substituted PCBs present in the mixtures (86).

Later rodent studies using individual ortho-substituted and coplanar PCB congeners support this hypothesis. Rats exposed to any of three different ortho-substituted PCB congeners (2,4,4'-trichlorobiphenyl; 2,3',4,4',5-pentachlorobiphenyl, or 2,2',4,4',5,5'-hexachlorobiphenyl) showed impaired learning on a delayed spatial alternation task (87). However, a fourth ortho-substituted congener (2,2',3,5',6-pentachlorobiphenyl) did not cause spatial learning deficits, demonstrating that not all ortho-substituted PCB congeners have the same effects (88). As in both monkey studies, the rats with spatial alternation deficits were equally impaired at short and long delays, suggesting a decrement in learning or attentional processes. The same rats showed no impairments in learning a working memory task on the eight-arm radial maze. The spatial alternation deficit was present only in female rats. Small group sizes had precluded analyzing for sex differences in the monkey studies, so this finding came as a surprise.

More recently, rats exposed to the PCB mixture Aroclor 1254 were tested on a working-reference memory task on a 12-arm radial maze and a spatial reversal learning task using operant procedures (89,90). Sex-specific deficits in spatial learning were again observed. However, in these studies using a complex PCB mixture rather than individual congeners, deficits were observed primarily in male rats. The PCB-exposed male rats showed impairments in both working and reference memory on the radial arm maze, whereas the females were not impaired on either (89). The PCB-exposed males also showed a deficit on the first reversal of the spatial reversal learning task (90). PCB-exposed female rats were not impaired on the radial arm maze task but showed a learning deficit that emerged on the later reversals of the spatial reversal learning task--a pattern very different from that seen in the males. Analyses of response patterns on the reversal learning task revealed underlying functional differences that explained the different effects in male and female rats. The first reversal deficit in the male rats was attributable to a tendency to perseverate to the previously correct response site. The female rats did not show an increased tendency to perseverate to the previously correct lever. Instead, they spent a longer period responding randomly to the two levers before finally beginning to associate the reward with the new response site. Whereas the males show a deficit early in the task and were able to overcome the deficit on later reversals, the female deficit only emerged on later reversals when the control animals were becoming proficient at performing the task.

The reason for the heightened sensitivity of female rats on some tasks and males on others is unknown. However, the discrepancies between studies could be partially explained by the fact that the animals in the earlier study were exposed to individual ortho-substituted PCB congeners, whereas those in the later studies were exposed to a complex PCB mixture. Certain effects of PCB mixtures could be either masked or unmasked when specific congeners from the mixture are given individually. Nevertheless, the sex differences in responses suggest that hormonal influences may be involved. A spatial learning deficit such as that seen in the female rats exposed to ortho-substituted PCB congeners would be consistent with a reduction in thyroid hormone. However, circulating thyroid hormones levels were assessed in litter mates of the tested animals (65), and it seems unlikely that alterations in thyroid hormones were directly mediating the learning deficit. The three PCB congeners had roughly equal effects on spatial learning but markedly different effects on thyroid hormone levels. One congener had no effect on serum [T.sub.4], one moderately reduced [T.sub.4] levels, and one dramatically reduced serum [T.sub.4] levels. Furthermore, males and females showed equal reductions in serum [T.sub.4], but only females were impaired on the learning task. The learning deficits in the latter studies would also be consistent with a reduction in thyroid hormone, but again only one sex was affected (this time males), whereas Aroclor 1254 is known to dramatically reduce circulating [T.sub.4] levels in both males and females (63). This evidence is indirect, but it argues against direct mediation of the learning deficit by reduced thyroid hormone. Koopman-Esseboom and colleagues (91-93) also failed to find a relationship between alterations in nervous system function and thyroid hormone levels in PCB-exposed human infants. As discussed above, Morse et al. (64) found that pups exposed to PCBs during fetal development had reduced serum and brain [T.sub.4] levels, but induction of type II 5'-deiodinase within the brain resulted in the maintenance of normal or near normal brain [T.sub.3] levels. Because [T.sub.3] is the active form of the hormone, these results also argue against a reduction in thyroid hormone as the mediating factor in PCB-induced learning deficits.

The extent to which changes in other hormonal systems, or interactions of altered thyroid function with other hormonal systems, may play a role in mediating PCB-induced learning deficits has not been addressed. As discussed above, thyroid hormones are involved in mediating glucocorticold receptor expression in the brain and can also influence the actions of estrogen in the brain (49,56), both directly and indirectly. As both the glucocorticoid-mediated stress response and estrogen's role in the brain are markedly sexually dimorphic, an interaction of altered thyroid hormone levels (or altered thyroid hormone action) with one or both of these systems could potentially explain the sex-specific effects that have been observed. In fact, given the complex and sexually dimorphic pattern of PCB effects on cognitive function, this seems like a reasonable scenario. If this were the case, a clear relationship between thyroid hormone concentrations and cognitive deficits would not necessarily be present. Thus, a reasonable first step to pursuing this line of research would be to determine it cotreatment of PCB-exposed animals with thyroid hormone ameliorates any of the PCB-induced cognitive deficits. If so, follow-up studies could be designed to determine if interactions of reduced thyroid hormone with the estrogen or glucocorticoid systems are involved in mediating those specific cognitive deficits. If not, follow-up studies could focus instead on determining whether particular cognitive deficits are mediated directly by changes in one of these other hormone systems. The various cognitive effects that have been reported in males and females after PCB exposure could be mediated by several different hormonal mechanisms, and sorting out the mechanisms for each behavioral effect will require a focused, stepwise approach.

In contrast to the findings for ortho-substituted PCBs, coplanar PCBs and dioxin did not impair spatial learning in rats (94). Dioxin-exposed rats did not differ from controls on delayed spatial alternation and actually made fewer errors than control rats on the radial arm maze. Although both sexes showed a trend toward better performance, the effect was more pronounced in dioxin-exposed male rats. Rats exposed to coplanar PCBs showed a similar but less striking improvement in learning. In a later study dioxin-exposed rats were tested on additional spatial and nonspatial learning tasks to determine whether the apparent facilitation in spatial learning was specific to the radial arm maze or would generalize to other tasks (95). The improved learning on the radial arm maze was replicated, but was found to be specific to the radial arm maze. It did not generalize to other spatial learning tasks, including the Morris water maze, which like the radial arm maze is primarily hippocampally-mediated. In addition, the dioxin-exposed rats showed a deficit in nonspatial, cue-based discrimination-reversal learning. This is similar to what was observed previously in dioxin-exposed monkeys (86). Another study compared the performance of dioxin-exposed litter mates on two different radial arm maze tasks. The first was the original 8-arm radial maze task in which all 8 arms were baited, and the second was a 12-arm radial maze task in which only 8 of the 12 arms were baited (96). Dioxin exposure improved performance on the 8-arm maze task in which all arms were baited, but not on the 12-arm maze task in which only a subset of the arms were baited, further highlighting the specificity of this effect.

Recently Rice and Hayward (97,98) tested rats developmentally exposed to a coplanar PCB congener on a series of learning tasks. The coplanar PCB-exposed rats did not differ from controls on delayed spatial alternation (97), visual-spatial or sustained attention (98) or fixed interval, fixed ratio, progressive ratio, and differential reinforcement of low rate operant tasks (99,100). These findings reinforce the fact that the cognitive effects of dioxin and coplanar PCBs are limited in scope, with the primary effect being an improvement in working memory, which is seen only in specific radial arm maze tasks.

It is not clear whether the cognitive changes observed after perinatal dioxin exposure are hormone mediated. However, dioxin has been shown to alter functioning of the HPA axis in adult animals (78,79) and to down-regulate glucocorticoid receptor expression in several tissues during development (80,81). Previous studies have shown that manipulations of circulating corticosteroid levels (101) or hippocampal glucocorticoid receptor expression (102) can result in improved spatial learning. Thus, it is conceivable that early dioxin exposure facilitates spatial learning on the radial arm maze by permanently altering hippocampal glucocorticoid receptor expression. This hypothesis could be tested by measuring glucocorticoid receptor expression in the hippocampus after developmental exposure to dioxin, and correlating receptor expression with spatial learning on the radial arm maze.

In summary, despite speculation by many investigators (1,9,56), there is currently no direct evidence mechanistically linking either PCB- or dioxin-induced changes in cognitive function to altered endocrine function. It is important that future studies directly assess whether there are mechanistic relationships between altered endocrine function and altered cognitive function after early exposure to these ubiquitous and persistent chemicals.

Lead. Lead exposure early in development has been shown to disrupt multiple endocrine systems, including the gonadal steroids (103), adrenal steroids (104,105), and thyroid hormones (105). The effects of developmental lead exposure on gonadal function are complex and appear to involve multiple sites of action. In utero exposure has been reported to reduce circulating estradiol and luteinizing hormone levels, delay the onset of puberty and produce irregular estrous cycling in female rats, and reduce testosterone levels, sperm counts, and masculine sexual behavior in male rats (103,106). The volume of the SDN-POA was also reduced in male rats (106). The effects of developmental lead exposure on the sexually dimorphic pattern of testosterone metabolism are variable. A partially demasculinizing (20-40%) decrease in adult CYP2C11-dependent 2-[alpha] and 16-[alpha]. hydroxylation and CYP2C11 apoprotein expression have been observed, along with a delay in the development of the sexually dimorphic pattern of hepatic P450 and sulfotransferase enzymes at puberty (107). The findings in both male and female rats are consistent with dual sites of action at the level of the hypothalamus-pituitary and directly on gonadal steroidogenesis (103).

The effects of lead exposure on adrenal steroids and thyroid hormones have been less extensively studied, but both adult (105) and developmental (104) exposure has been shown to elevate plasma corticosterone levels. The evidence for altered thyroid function is mixed. Some investigators report changes in circulating thyroid hormones after lead exposure, whereas others do not (8). Although growth hormone is not a focus of this report, it is important to note that early lead exposure also disrupts growth hormone (107), and it is possible that some of the cognitive effects of early lead exposure could be related to disruption of the growth hormone system (108).

Few, if any, developmental neurotoxicants have been more extensively evaluated for cognitive effects than lead. A large number of independent investigators have reported cognitive impairments in developmentally lead-exposed rodents and primates, and a wealth of epidemiologic data points to cognitive deficits in lead-exposed children as well (109). The effects in primates and rodents include deficits in reversal learning (110-112), delayed spatial alternation (113-115), and schedule controlled behavior, particularly fixed interval and delayed reinforcement of low rates (116-119). A leading hypothesis to explain the deficits exhibited by lead-exposed animals on many of these tasks is that the animals continue to respond to a previously correct response site when the reward contingencies change (perseveration), and/or to respond excessively and inappropriately.

Developmentally lead-exposed monkeys were impaired on both spatial and nonspatial discrimination-reversal learning (110,120,121). In general, they could learn the initial discrimination problem but made more errors when the reward contingencies changed on the reversals. Deficits were especially pronounced on the first reversal (110). Lead-exposed rats showed a similar pattern. They could learn an initial olfactory discrimination but were impaired on the subsequent reversals (112). The discrimination-reversal deficit in the rats was not due to perseveration. Analyses of the animals' response patterns indicated that the lead-exposed animals spent longer responding randomly to the two levers before finally beginning to associate the reward with the new cue. They did not perseverate to the previously correct cue. On spatial alternation tasks, both lead-exposed monkeys and rats showed deficits in percent correct responses that were constant across a series of delays (113,115). As discussed above, this pattern suggests a deficit in learning or attentional processes rather than in memory. On fixed interval operant schedules, which require the animal to wait a fixed period of time for reinforcement, lead-exposed monkeys and rats showed higher response rates and shorter inter-response times (116,118). Similarly, on a delayed reinforcement of low rate schedule, lead-exposed monkeys were slower to learn to withhold their responding to the low rate necessary for reinforcement (119). In summary, the nature of the cognitive deficits on a number of different tasks suggests that cognitive processes controlled by the prefrontal cortex including selective attention and the ability to inhibit inappropriate responding appear to be particularly sensitive to lead exposure. However, the deficit on discrimination-reversal learning appears to be the result of a decreased ability to learn new contingencies (i.e., an associative deficit), rather than a deficit in inhibitory control (112).

Although lead has been shown to disrupt multiple endocrine systems, little attention has been paid to the role changes in endocrine function might play in mediating the cognitive effects of developmental lead exposure. Interestingly, lead has recently been found to impair choroid plexus transthyretin production (122). Because transthyretin is responsible for transport of thyroid hormones into the brain, it has been suggested that lead could impair brain development by depriving the CNS of thyroid hormones. However, there do not appear to be any studies measuring thyroid hormone levels in brain after lead exposure or relating such changes to cognitive deficits. This could prove to be an important area for future research. Developmental lead exposure also increases circulating corticosterone levels, which could potentially alter glucocorticoid receptor expression in the brain. Finally, as discussed earlier, developmental lead exposure decreases circulating testosterone levels and demasculinizes the pre-optic area in male rats. Thus, lead could potentially alter hippocampal morphology and spatial learning in male rats. However, the later mechanism is perhaps not as likely since the cognitive effects of early lead exposure do not appear to be sexually dimorphic.

Organochlorine pesticides. As indicated in Table 1, many of the persistent organochlorine pesticides including DDT (and its breakdown product DDE), Alachlor, chlordane, chlordecone, dieldrin, endosulfan, heptachlor, and lindane have been identified as endocrine disruptors (8,59). Most are weakly estrogenic and some also alter thyroid or adrenal function. Some organochlorine pesticides have been evaluated for cognitive effects, but the majority of the studies involve adult exposures. The question as to whether there are cognitive effects in developing organisms is largely unanswered.

Dieldrin is a persistent chlorinated hydrocarbon pesticide that was used as a broad range insecticide until the U.S. EPA restricted its use in 1974. Although no longer in use, dieldrin can remain undegraded in soil for many years. It is lipophilic and readily bioaccumulates in animals and humans. Dieldrin exposure during adulthood resulted in deficits in visual discrimination-reversal learning in both sheep (123) and squirrel monkeys (124) and caused rats to make more errors on a zig-zag maze (125). In contrast, one study of perinatal exposure to dieldrin reported facilitated retention of learning on a symmetrical maze (126).

Studies of other organochlorine insecticides have been limited almost exclusively to acute exposures followed by testing in simple active or passive avoidance paradigms and have yielded mixed results. Lindane is a chlorinated hydrocarbon insecticide as well as a human and veterinary ectoparasiticide, which continues to be prescribed for the treatment of body lice in humans. It is a powerful neurostimulant capable of causing convulsions and electroencephalogram disturbances. Acute exposure to lindane in the early postnatal period resulted in an apparent facilitation of acquisition on a passive avoidance task (127). However, the lindane-exposed animals also showed significant reductions in spontaneous motor activity, so these data must be interpreted with caution. Changes in locomotor activity can influence avoidance behavior, with hypoactivity favoring correct responding in passive avoidance paradigms and hyperactivity favoring correct responding in active avoidance paradigms. Acute exposure to lindane during adulthood did not alter acquisition of a passive avoidance task, but did cause significant deficits in retention when animals were retested 7 days after the original training (128). In contrast to the passive avoidance task, lindane-exposed rats did show deficits in acquisition of an active avoidance task. Responding between trials was not significantly different between groups, suggesting that the effect of lindane on active avoidance learning was not due to a reduction in locomotor activity.

Endosulfan is an insecticide in current use. Chronic exposure of either immature or adult rats to endosulfan resulted in learning and memory deficits in an active avoidance task where the rats were required to jump to a pole suspended from the ceiling of the chamber in order to avoid shock (129-131). Chlordecone, or kepone, is a polycyclic chlorinated hydrocarbon that was used primarily as an insecticide. Studies assessing the cognitive effects of chlordecone have yielded mixed results. Tilson et al. (132) reported that acute exposure to chlordecone in the early postnatal period did not result in any deficits in learning of a two-choice visual discrimination-reversal task. Larger doses of chlordecone given to adult rats also did not result in any deficits in acquisition or retention of a step-through passive avoidance task (128). Acute exposure of preweanling rats to chlordecone did not lead to deficits in acquisition of passive avoidance, but when the rats were retested 6 days after the original training, a memory deficit was observed (133). In contrast, pups were impaired on both learning and retention of active avoidance tasks (134). DDT, once widely used in the United States as an insecticide, has been banned from use since 1973. However, it continues to be used in other parts of the world and continues to present a health hazard. Tilson et al. (128) found few effects of DDT on the ability of rats to learn active and passive avoidance tasks. However, other researchers have reported impaired acquisition on active avoidance tasks (135,136), as well as impaired retention on passive avoidance tasks (135-137).

In summary, the data on cognitive effects of organochlorine pesticides are sparse, and in most cases, the tests that have been used to measure cognition are simplistic. The results do suggest that many of the organochlorine pesticides have the ability to interfere with the acquisition and use of new information. However, the literature on both the endocrine and cognitive effects of organochlorine pesticides remains too sketchy to form useful hypotheses about the possible endocrine mediation of cognitive deficits.

Other chemicals. A number of other chemicals including compounds such as phthalates and bisphenol A, which are used in the manufacture of plastics, and alkylphenols, which are breakdown products of chemicals used in detergents, have been identified as endocrine disruptors. Most of these were first identified as having estrogenic activity (59), but some were later found to disrupt thyroid function as well (8). At this time, the data on cognitive effects from any of these compounds is sparse (138).

Concluding Remarks

Although it is reasonable to hypothesize that central nervous system effects of endocrine-disrupting chemicals are mediated by interference with hormone action, mechanistic studies establishing causal relationships between the hormonal actions of environmental chemicals and their cognitive effects have not been conducted. At present, the only study we are aware of that establishes a direct link between hormone disruption by an environmental chemical and nervous system dysfunction is a study by Goldey and Crofton (139), which showed that PCB-induced hearing loss closely resembles the hearing loss seen after propylthiouracil treatment and can be prevented by cotreatment of PCB-exposed pups with thyroid hormone. Our own research has demonstrated that if a relationship between alterations in thyroid hormones and PCB-induced cognitive dysfunction exists, it is likely to be considerably more complex. The complexity of this issue and the science needed to address it should not keep us from moving forward.

Mechanistic studies directly addressing the relationship between endocrine disruption and the documented cognitive deficits caused by chemical pollutants such as PCBs and lead are desperately needed. For example, thyroid hormone replacement studies similar to those used to investigate the relationship between PCB-induced reductions in circulating thyroid hormone concentrations and hearing loss would be a first step toward determining the role, if any, of reductions in thyroid hormones in mediating specific PCB-related cognitive deficits. However, as we embark on such studies it is important to keep in mind that it is unlikely that all of the cognitive effects of a particular compound such as PCBs will be mediated by a single mechanism. It is more likely that the multiple endocrine effects caused by PCBs interact in complex ways to produce the various cognitive effects that have been reported. Sorting out these interactions will require a focused, stepwise approach. It is also important to keep in mind that typical animal models in which high concentrations of the chemical are given during a narrow window of development may not be relevant to the human condition in which exposures are much lower and occur over an extended period of time. In future studies it will be important to use animal models that are more relevant to the human situation.

An important caveat is that endocrine disruptors such as PCBs and lead can also have direct effects on the nervous system, and these direct actions undoubtedly contribute to the cognitive deficits induced by these compounds. For example, PCBs have been shown to interact directly with ryanodine-sensitive calcium release channels, altering calcium signaling in neurons (140), and lead has been shown to act as an antagonist at the N-methyl-D-aspartate receptor (141). The relative importance of direct mechanisms versus indirect endocrine-related mechanisms in mediating the cognitive deficits induced by these compounds remains to be determined.

Finally, more extensive studies of the cognitive effects of other endocrine-disrupting chemicals such as organochlorine pesticides that are still in active use, components of plastics and cosmetics such as phthalates and bisphenol A, and alkylphenol breakdown products from detergents are desperately needed. These studies should be designed with the goal of determining mechanisms, not just screening for cognitive effects. It is only through the active and continued pursuit of this challenging research area that we will gain the knowledge we need to protect the health of future generations.

Table 1. Effects of synthetic chemicals on endocrine and cognitive
function.

                          Estrogen/
Compound                  androgen    Thyroid

Industrial chemicals
  Bisphenol A             A-; E+      ?

PCBs, dioxins, and
    furans
  Dioxins                 A-; E-      Mixed ([down arrow] [T.sub.4];
                                        unchanged or
                                        [down arrow] [T.sub.3];
                                        unchanged
                                        or [up arrow] TSH)
  PCBs                    E+/-; A-    Mixed ([down arrow] [T.sub.4];
                                        unchanged or
                                        [down arrow] [T.sub.3];
                                        unchanged
                                        or [up arrow] TSH)
  PCDFs                   E-          [down arrow] [T.sub.4];
                                        [up arrow] TSH
  Pentachlorophenol       E+; A-      G; [down arrow] [T.sub.4]

Phthalates
  Butylbenzylphthalate    A-; E+      ?
  Diethylhexylphthalate   ?           [down arrow] [T.sub.4]
  Di-n-Butylphthalate     E+          ?

Alkylphenols
  p-Nonylphenol           A+; E+      ?

Organochlorine
    pesticides
  Alachlor                E+          [down arrow] [T.sub.4];
                                        [up arrow] [T.sub.3];
                                        [up arrow] TSH; G
  Chlordane               A-          G
  Chlordecone (Kepone)    E+          ?
  DDT                     A-; E+      [up arrow] [T.sub.3];
                                        [down arrow] PBI; G
  DDE                     A-; E+      G; [down arrow] I uptake
  Dieldrin                A-; E+      G; [down arrow] PBI
  Endosulfan              E+          [up arrow] [T.sub.4];
                                        [down arrow] [T.sub.3]; G
  Heptachlor              A-          ?
  Lindane                 E+/-        [down arrow] [T.sub.4];
                                        [down arrow] [T.sub.3];
                                        [down arrow] TSH;
                                        [down arrow] PBI; G
  Oxychlordane            ?           G

Other pesticides/
    herbicides
  2,4-D                   A-          [down arrow] PBI;
                                        I uptake
  Atrazine                A-; E-      Mixed ([up arrow] [T.sub.3];
                                        [up arrow] [T.sub.4];
                                        [down arrow] [T.sub.3])
Heavy metals
  Cadmium                 E-          [down arrow] [T.sub.4];
                                        [down arrow] [T.sub.3]; G
  Mercury                 A-; E-      [down arrow] [T.sub.3];
                                        [down arrow] I uptake
  Lead                    A-; E-      Mixed

                                                      Alters
                                                     cognitive
Compound                  Glucocorticoids            function?

Industrial chemicals
  Bisphenol A             ?                              ?

PCBs, dioxins, and
    furans
  Dioxins                 Mixed                         Yes
                            ([up arrow] C;
                            [down arrow] C;
                            unchanged or
                            [up arrow] ACTH)
  PCBs                    [down arrow] C                Yes
  PCDFs                   ?                              ?
  Pentachlorophenol       ?                              ?

Phthalates
  Butylbenzylphthalate    ?                              ?
  Diethylhexylphthalate   ?                              ?
  Di-n-Butylphthalate     ?                              ?

Alkylphenols
  p-Nonylphenol           ?                              ?

Organochlorine
    pesticides
  Alachlor                ?                              ?
  Chlordane               [down arrow] C (females)      Yes
                            [up arrow] C (males)
  Chlordecone (Kepone)    Mixed ([down arrow] C or      Yes
                            no change)
  DDT                     [down arrow] C;               Yes
                            [down arrow] response
                            to ACTH
  DDE                     [down arrow] C;                ?
                            [down arrow] response
                            to ACTH
  Dieldrin                ?                             Yes
  Endosulfan              ?                             Yes
  Heptachlor              [down arrow] C                 ?
  Lindane                 ?                             Yes
  Oxychlordane            ?                              ?

Other pesticides/
    herbicides
  2,4-D                   ?                             Yes
  Atrazine                ?                             Yes

Heavy metals
  Cadmium                 Mixed ([up arrow] C;          Yes
                            [down arrow] C;
                            or no change)
  Mercury                 Mixed ([down arrow]           Yes
                            response to ACTH;
                            or no change)
  Lead                    [up arrow] C                  Yes

Compound                   References

Industrial chemicals
  Bisphenol A

PCBs, dioxins, and
    furans
  Dioxins                 (85,86,94,95)
  PCBs                    (82-84,87,88)
  PCDFs
  Pentachlorophenol

Phthalates
  Butylbenzylphthalate
  Diethylhexylphthalate
  Di-n-Butylphthalate

Alkylphenols
  p-Nonylphenol

Organochlorine
    pesticides
  Alachlor
  Chlordane                   (142)
  Chlordecone (Kepone)    (128,132-134)
  DDT                     (128,135-137)
  DDE
  Dieldrin                  (123-126)
  Endosulfan                (129-131)
  Heptachlor
  Lindane                 (127,128,143)
  Oxychlordane

Other pesticides/
    herbicides
  2,4-D                       (144)
  Atrazine                    (145)

Heavy metals
  Cadmium                   (146,147)
  Mercury                   (148,149)
  Lead                      (110-121)

Abbreviations: A+, androgenic; A-, antiandrogenic; ACTH,
adrenocorticotropic hormone; C, corticosterone; 2,4-D,
2,4-dichlorophenoxyacetic acid; E+, estrogenic; E-, antiestrogenic;
G, goiter; I, iodine; PBI, protein-bound iodine; PCDF, polychlorinated
dibenzofurans; TSH, thyroid-stimulating hormone; ?, unknown.

REFERENCES AND NOTES

(1.) Porterfield SP, Hendry LB. Impact of PCBs on thyroid hormone directed brain development. Toxicol Ind Health 14:103-120 (1996).

(2.) Vacarri A. Teratogenic mechanisms of dysthyroidism in the central nervous system. Prog Brain Res 73:71-86 (1968).

(3.) Nunez J. Effects of thyroid hormones during brain differentiation. Mol Cell Endocrinol 37:125-132 (1984).

(4.) Timiras PS. Thyroid hormones and the developing brain. in: Handbook of Human Growth and Developmental Biology, Vol 1, Part C (Meisami E, Timiras PS, eds). Boca Raton, FL:CRC Press, 1988;59-82.

(5.) Akaike MH, Kat H, Ohno H, Kobayashi T. Hyperactivity and spatial maze learning impairment of adult rats with temporary neonatal hypothyroidism. Neurotoxicol Teratol 13:317-322 (1991).

(6.) Stein SA, Adams PM, Shanklin DR, Mihailoff GA. Palnitkar MB. Thyroid hormone control of brain and motor development: molecular, neuroanatomical, and behavioral studies. In: Advances in Perinatology and Thyroidology (Bercu BB, Shulman DI, eds). New York:Plenum Press, 1991;47-105.

(7.) Goldey ES, Kehn LS, Rehnberg GL, Crofton KM. Effects of developmental hypothyroidism on auditory and motor function in the rat. Toxicol Appl Pharmacol 135:67-76 (1995).

(8.) Brucker-Davis F. Effects of environmental synthetic chemicals on thyroid function. Thyroid 8:827-856 (1998).

(9.) Brouwer A. Morse DC, Lans MC, Schuur AG, Murk AJ, Klasson-Wehler E, Bergman A. Visser TJ. Interactions of persistent environmental organohalogens with the thyroid hormone system: mechanisms and possible consequences for animal end human health. Toxicol Ind Health 14:59-84 (1998).

(10.) Hansen LG. Stepping backward to improve assessment of PCB congener toxicities. Environ Health Perspect 106(suppl 1):171-189 (1998).

(11.) Bitman J, Cecil HC. Estrogenic activity of DDT analogs and polychlorinated biphenyls. J Agric Food Chem 18:1108-1112 (1972).

(12.) Ecobichon DJ, MacKenzie DO. The uterotropic activity of commercial and isomerically-pure chlorobiphenyls in the rat. Res Commun Chem Pathol Pharmacol 9:85-95 (1974).

(13.) Meserve LA, Murray BA, Landis JA. Influence of maternal ingestion of Aroclor 1254 (PCB) or Firemaster BP-6 (PBB) on unstimulated and stimulated corticosterone levels in young rats. Bull Environ Contam Toxicol 48:715-720 (1992).

(14.) MacLusky NJ, Naftolin F. Sexual differentiation of the central nervous system. Science 211:1294-1303 (1981).

(15.) Gorski RA. Sexual differentiation of brain structure in rodents. In: Sexual Differentiation: Basic and Clinical Aspects (Serio M, ed). New York:Raven Press, 1984;65-77.

(16.) Jarzab B, Kokocinska D, Kaminski M, Gubala E, Achtelik W, Wagiel J, Dohler KD. Influence of neurotransmitters on sexual differentiation of the brain: relationship between volume of the SDN-POA and functional characteristics. Comp Physiol 8:41-50 (1990).

(17.) McEwen BS, Coirini H, Westlind-Danielsson A, Frankfurt M, Gould E, Schumacher M, Woolley C. Steroid hormones as mediators of neural plasticity. J Steroid Biochem Mol Biol 39:223-232 (1991).

(18.) Meaney MJ, Stewart J, Poulin P, McEwen BS. Sexual differentiation of social play in rat pups is mediated by the neonatal androgen-receptor system. Neuroendocrinology 37:85-90 (1983).

(19.) McEwen BS, Gould E, Orchinik M, Weiland NG, Woolley CS. Oestrogens and the structural and functional plasticity of neurons: implications for memory, aging end neurodegenerative processes. Ciba Found Symp 191:52-73 (1905).

(20.) Williams CL, Meck WH. The organizational effects of gonadal steroids on sexually dimorphic spatial ability. Psychoneuroendocrinology 16:155-176 (1991).

(21.) Williams CL, Barnett AM, Meck WH. Organizational effects of early gonadal secretions on sexual differentiation of spatial memory. Behav Neurosci 104:84-97 (1990).

(22.) Gould E, Westlind-Daneilsson A, Frankfurt M, McEwen BS. Sex differences and thyroid hormone sensitivity of hippocampal pyramidal cells. J Neurosci 10:996-1003 (1990).

(23.) Parducz A, Garcia-Segura LM. Sexual differences in the synaptic connectivity in the rat dentate gyrus. Neurosci Lett 161:53-50 (1993).

(24.) Roof RL, Havens MD. Testosterone improves maze performance and induces development of a male hippocampus in females. Brain Res 572:310-313 (1992).

(25.) O'Keefe JA, Handa RJ. Transient elevation of estrogen receptors in the neonatal rat hippocampus. Dev Brain Res 57:119-27 (1990).

(26.) MacLusky NJ, Clark AS, Naftolin F, Goldman-Rakic PS. Estrogen formation in the mammalian brain: possible role of aromatase in sexual differentiation of the hippocampus and neocortex. Steroids 50:459-474 (1987).

(27.) Woolley CS, McEwen BS. Roles of estradiol and progesterone in regulation of hippocampal dendritic spine density during the estrous cycle in the rat. J Comp Neurol 336:293-306 (1993).

(28.) Woolley CS, Weiland NG, McEwen BS, Schwartzkroin PA. Estradiol increases the sensitivity of hippocampal CA1 pyramidal cells to NMDA receptor-mediated synaptic input: correlation with dendritic spine density. J Neurosci 17:848-859 (1997).

(29.) Warren SG, Juraska JM. Spatial and nonspatial learning across the rat estrous cycle. Behav Neurosci 111:259-266 (1997).

(30.) Rissanen A, Puolivali J, van Groen T, Riekkinen P Jr. In mice tonic estrogen replacement therapy improves nonspatial and spatial memory in a water maze task. Neuroreport 10:1369-1372 (1999).

(31.) Shors TJ, Lewczyk C, Pacynski M, Mathew PR, Pickett J. Stages of estrous mediate the stress-induced impairment of associative learning in the female rat. Neuroreport 9:419-423 (1998).

(32.) Diaz-Veliz G, Soto V, Dussaubat N, Mora S. Influence of the estrous cycle, ovariectomy and estradiol replacement upon the acquisition of conditioned avoidance responses in rats. Physiol Behav 46:397-401 (1989).

(33.) Healy SD, Braham SR, Braithwaite VA. Spatial working memory in rats: no differences between the sexes. Proc R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 266:2303-2308 (1999).

(34.) Stackman RW, Blasberg ME, Langan CJ, Clark AS. Stability of spatial working memory across the estrous cycle of Long-Evans rats. Neurobiol Learn Mem 67:167-171 (1997).

(35.) Berry B, McMahan R, Gallagher M. Spatial learning and memory at defined points of the estrous cycle: effects on performance of a hippocampal-dependent task. Behav Neurosci 11:267-274 (1997).

(36.) Sherwin BB. Estrogen effects on cognition in menopausal women. Neurology 48(suppl 2):S21-S26 (1997).

(37.) Maki PM, Zonderman AB, Resnick SM. Enhanced verbal memory in nondemented elderly women receiving hormone-replacement therapy. Am J Psychiatry 158:227-233 (2001).

(38.) O'Neal MF, Means LW, Poole MC, Hamm RJ. Estrogen affects performance of ovariectomized rats in a two-choice water-escape working memory task. Psychoneuroendocrinology 21:51-65 (f996).

(39.) Shughrue PJ, Lane MV, Merchenthaler I. Comparative distribution of estrogen receptor-[alpha] and -[beta] mRNA in the rat central nervous system. J Comp Neurol 388:507-525 (1997).

(40.) Davenport J. Environmental therapy in hypothyroidism and other disadvantaged animal populations. In: Environments as Therapies for Brain Dysfunction (Walsh RN, Greenough WT, eds). New York:Plenum Press, 1976;71-114.

(41.) Gould E, Woolley CS, McEwen BS. The hippocampal formation: morphological changes induced by thyroid, gonadal and adrenal hormones. Psychoneuroendocrinology 16:67-84 (1991)

(42.) Rami A, Patel AJ, Rabie A. Thyroid hormone and development of the rat hippocampus: morphological alterations in granule and pyramidal cells. Neuroscience 19:1217-1226 (1986).

(43.) Bayer SA. Development of the hippocampal region in the rat I. Morphogenesis during embryonic and early postnatal life. J Comp Neurol 190:115-134 (1980).

(44.) Vigouroux J, Clos J, Legrand J. Uptake and metabolism of exogenous and endogenous thyroxine in the brain of young rats. Horm Meta Res 11:228-232 (1979).

(45.) Valcana T, Timiras PS. Nuclear triiodothyronine receptors in the developing brain. Mol Cell Endocrin 11:31-41 (1978).

(46.) Rami A, Rabie A, Patel AJ. Thyroid hormone and development of the rat hippocampus: cell acquisition in the dentate gyrus. Neuroscience 19:1207-1216 (1986).

(47.) Madeira MD, Paula-Barbosa MM, Cadete-Leite A, Tavares MA. Unbiased estimate of hippocampal granule cell numbers in hypothyroid and in sex-age-matched controls. J Hirnforsch 29:643-650 (1988).

(48.) de Kloet ER, Rosenfield P, Van Eekelen AM, Sutanto W, Levin S. Stress, glucocorticoids and development Prog Brain Res 73:101-120 (1988).

(49.) Francis D, Diorio J, LaPlante P, Weaver S, Seckl JR, Meaney MJ. The role of early environmental events in regulating neuroendocrine development: moms, pups, stress and glucocorticoid receptors. Ann Natl Acad Sci 704:136-152 (1996).

(50.) Meaney MJ, Aitken DH, Van Berkel C, Bhatnagar S, Sapolski RM. Effect of neonatal handling on age-related impairments associated with the hippocampus. Science 239:766-768 (1988).

(51.) Gould E, Cameron HA. Regulation of neuronal birth, migration and death in the rat dentate gyrus. Dev Neurosci 18:22-35 (1996).

(52.) Vaher PR, Luine VN, Gould E, McEwen BS. Effects of adrenalectomy on spatial memory performance and dentate gyrus morphology. Brain Res 656:71-78 (1994).

(53.) Conrad CD, Roy EJ. Selective loss of hippocampal granule cells following adrenalectomy: implications for spatial memory. J Neurosci 13:2982-2590 (1993).

(54.) Luine V, Villegas M, Martinez C, McEwen B. Repeated stress causes reversible impairments of spatial memory performance. Brain Res 639:167-170 (1994).

(55.) MacLusky NJ, Brown TJ, Schantz SL, Seo BW, Peterson RE. Hormonal interactions in the effects of halogenated aromatic hydrocarbons on the developing brain. Toxicol Ind Health 14:185-208 (1998).

(58.) Zhu YS, Yen PM, Chin WW, Pfaff DW. Estrogen and thyroid hormone interaction on regulation of gene expression. Proc Natl Acad Sci 93:12587-12592 (1996).

(57.) Keith LH. Environmental Endocrine Disrupters: A Handbook of Physical Data. New York:John Wiley and Sons, 1997.

(58.) Sonnenschein C, Soto AM. An updated review of environmental estrogen and androgen mimics and antagonists. J Steroid Biochem Mol Biol 65:143-150 (1998).

(59.) Keith LH. Environmental Endocrine Disruptors: An Overview of the Analytical Challenge. Presented at the 13th Annual Waste Testing and Quality Assurance Symposium, 6-9 July 1997, Arlington, VA.

(60.) Broadhurst MG. Use and replacability of polychlorinated biphenyls. Environ Health Perspect 2:81-102 (1972).

(61.) Ahlborg U, Brouwer A, Fingerhut M, Jacobson J, Jacobson S, Kennedy S, Kettrup A, Koeman J, Poiger C, Rappe C, et al. Impact of polychlorinated dibenzo-p-dioxins, dibenzofurans, and biphenyls on human and environmental health, with special emphasis on application of the toxic equivalency factor concept. Eur J Pharmacol 228:179-199 (1992).

(62.) Whitlock JP. The aromatic hydrocarbon receptor, dioxin action, and endocrine homeostatis. Trends Endocrinol Metab 5:183-168 (1994).

(63.) Goldey ES, Kehn LS, Lau C, Rehnberg GL, Crofton KM. Developmental exposure to polychlorinated biphenyls (Aroclor 1254) reduces circulating thyroid hormone concentrations and causes hearing deficits in rats. Toxicol Appl Pharmacol 135:77-88 (1995).

(64.) Morse DC, Wehler EK, Wesseling W, Koeman JH, Brouwer A. Alterations in rat brain thyroid hormone status following pre- and postnatal exposure to polychlorinated biphenyls (Aroclor 1254). Toxicol Appl Pharmacol 136:269-279 (1996).

(65.) Ness DK, Schantz SL, Moshtaghian J, Hansen LG. Effects of perinatal exposure to specific PCB congeners on thyroid hormone concentrations and thyroid histology in the rat. Toxicol Lett 68:311-323 (1993).

(68.) Koopman-Esseboom C, Morse DC, Weisglas-Kuperus N, Lutkeschipholt IJ, van der Paauw CG, Tuinstra LGM, Brouwer A, Sauer PJJ. Effects of dioxins and polychlorinated biphenyls on thyroid hormone status of pregnant women and their infants. Pediatr Res 36:468-473 (1994).

(67.) Silva JE, Matthews PS. Production rates and turnover of triiodothyronine in rat developing cerebral cortex and cerebellum. J Clin Invest 74:1035-1049 (1984).

(68.) Porterfield SP. Vulnerability of the developing brain to thyroid abnormalities: environmental insults to the thyroid system. Environ Health Perspect 102(suppl 2):125-130 (1994).

(69.) Zoeller RT, Dowling ALS, Vas AA. Developmental exposure to polychlorinated biphenyls exerts thyroid hormone-like effects on the expression of RC3/neurogranin and myelin basic protein messenger ribonucleic acids in the developing rat brain. Endocrinology 141:181-189 (2000).

(70.) Ibarrola N, Rodriguez-Pena A. Hypothyroidism coordinately and transiently affects myelin protein gene expression in most rat brain regions during postnatal development. Brain Res 752:285-293 (1997).

(71.) Iniguez MA, DeLecea L, Guadano-Ferraz A, Morte B, Gerendasy D, Sutcliffe JG, Bernal J. Cell specific effects of thyroid hormone on RC3/neurogranin expression in rat brain. Endocrinology 137:1032-1041 (1996).

(72.) Nesaretnam K, Corcoran D, Dils RR, Darbre P. 3,4,3',4'-tetrachlorobiphenyl acts as an estrogen in vitro and in vivo. Mol Endocrinol 10:923-936 (1996).

(73.) Seegal RF, Gierthy JF, Arcaro KF, Brosch KO. Neurochemical and neuroendocrine effects of non-coplanar (NCP) and coplanar (CP) PCBs. Toxicologist 36:332 (1997).

(74.) Peterson RE, Theobald HM, Kimmel GL. Developmental and reproductive toxicity of dioxins and related compounds: cross-species comparisons. Crit Rev Toxicol 23:283-335 (1993).

(75.) Mably TA, Moore RW, Goy RW, Peterson RE. In utero and lactational exposure of male rats to 2,3,7,8-TCDD (effects on sexual behavior and the regulation of lutenizing hormone secretion in adulthood). Toxicol Appl Pharmacol 114:118-126 (1992).

(76.) Bjerke DL, Brown TJ, MacLusky NJ, Hochberg RB, Peterson RE. Partial demasculinization and feminization of sex behavior by in utero and lactational exposure to 2,3,7,8-TCDD is not associated with alterations in estrogen receptor binding or volumes of sexually differentiated nuclei. Toxicol Appl Pharmacol 127:258-267 (1994).

(77.) Gray LE, Kelce WR, Manoson E, Ostby JS, Birnbaum LS. Exposure to TCDD during development permanently alters reproductive function ia male Long Evans rats and hamsters: reduced ejaculated and epididymal sperm numbers and sex accessory gland weights in offspring with normal androgenic status. Toxicol Appl Pharmacol 131:108-118 (1995).

(78.) Bestervelt LL Cai Y, Piper DW, Nolan CJ, Pitt JA, Piper WN. TCDD alters pituitary-adrenal function I: adrenal responsiveness to exogenous ACTH. Neurotoxicol Teratol 15:365-370 (1993).

(79.) Bestervelt LL, Pitt JA, Nolan CJ, Piper WN. TCDD alters pituitary-adrenal function II: evidence for decreased bioavailability of ACTH. Neurotoxicol Teratol 15:371-376 (1993).

(80.) Abbott BD, Perdew GH, Buckalew AR, Birnbaum LS. Interactive regulation of Ah and glucocorticoid receptors in the synergistic induction of cleft palate by 2,3,7,8-tetrachlorodibenzo-p-dioxin and hydrocortisone. Toxicol Appl Pharmacol 128:138-150 (1994).

(81.) Csaba G, Mag O, Inczefi-Gonda A, Szeberenyi S. Persistent influence of neonatal 2,3,7,8-tetrachlorodibenzo-p-dioxin (TCDD) treatment on glucocorticoid receptors and on the microsomal enzyme system. J Dev Physiol 15:337-340 (1991).

(82.) Schantz SL, Levin ED, Bowman RE. Long-term neurobehavioral effects of perinatal polychlorinated biphenyl (PCB) exposure in monkeys. Environ Toxicol Chem 10:747-756 (1991).

(83.) Rice DC, Hayward S. Effects of postnatal exposure to a PCB mixture in monkeys on nonspatial discrimination reversal learning and delayed spatial alternation. Neurotoxicology 18:479-494 (1997).

(84.) Rice DC. Effect of postnatal exposure to a PCB mixture in monkeys on multiple fixed interval-fixed ratio performance. Neurotoxicol Teratol 19:429-434 (1997).

(85.) Schantz SL, Bowman RE. Learning in monkeys exposed to 2,3,7,8-tetrachlorodibenzo-p-dioxin (TCDD). Neurotoxicol Teratol 11:13-19 (1989).

(86.) Seegal RF, Schantz SL. Neurochemical and behavioral sequalae of exposure to dioxins and PCBs. In: Dioxins and Health (Schecter A. ed) New York:Plenum Press, 1994;409-447.

(87.) Schantz SL, Moshtaghian J, Ness DK. Spatial learning deficits in adult rats exposed to ortho-substituted PCB congeners during gestation and lactation. Fundam Appl Toxicol 26:117-126 (1995).

(88.) Schantz SL, Seo BW, Wong PW, Pessah IN. Long-term effects of developmental exposure to 2,2',3,5',6-pentachlorobiphenyl (PCB 95) on locomotor activity, spatial learning and memory and brain ryanodine binding. Neurotoxicology 18:457-468 (1997).

(89.) Roegge CS, Seo BW, Crofton KM, Schantz SL. Gestational-lactational exposure to Aroclor 1254 impairs radial-arm maze performance in male rats. Toxicol Sci 57:121-130 (2000).

(90.) Widholm JJ, Clarkson GB, Strupp BJ, Crofton KM, Seegal RF, Schantz SL. Spatial reversal learning in Aroclor 1254-exposed rats: sex-specific deficits in associative ability and inhibitory control. Toxicol Appl Pharmacol 174:188-198 (2001).

(91.) Koopman-Esseboom C, Weisglas-Kuperus N, de Ridder MAJ, van der Paauw CG, Tuinstra LGM, Sauer PJJ. Effects of polychlorinated biphenyl/dioxin exposure and feeding type on infants' mental and psychomotor development. Pediatrics 97:700-706 (1996).

(92.) Huisman M, Koopman-Esseboom C, Fidler V, Hadders-Algra M, van der Paauw CG, Tuinstra LG, Weisglas-Kuperus N, Sauer PJJ, Touwen BC, Boersma ER. Perinatal exposure to polychlorinated biphenyls and dioxins and its effect on neonatal neurological development. Early Hum Dev 41:111-127 (1995).

(93.) Huisman M, Koopman-Esseboom C, Lanting CI, van der Paauw CG, Tuinstra LG, Fidler V, Weisglas-Kuperus N, Sauer PJJ, Boersma ER, Touwen BC. Neurological condition in 18-month-old children perinatally exposed to polychlorinated biphenyls and dioxins. Early Hum Dev 43:165-176 (1995).

(94.) Schantz SL, Seo BW, Moshtaghian J, Peterson RE, Moore RW. Effects of gestational and lactational exposure to TCDD or coplanar PCBs on spatial learning. Neurotoxicol Teratol 18:305-313 (1996).

(95.) Seo BW, Sparks AJ, Medora K, Amin S, Schantz SL. Learning and memory in rats gestationally and lactationally exposed to 2,3,7,8-tetrachlorodibenzo-p-dioxin (TCDD). Neurotoxicol Teratol 21:231-239 (1999).

(96.) Seo BW, Powers BE, Widholm JJ, Schantz SL. Radial arm maze performance in rats following gestational and lactational exposure to 2,3,7,8-tetrachlorodibenzo-p-dioxin (TCDD). Neurotoxicol Teratol 22:511-519 (2000).

(97.) Rice DC, Hayward S. Effects of exposure to 3,3',4,4',5-pentachlorobiphenyl (PCB 126) throughout gestation and lactation on behavior (concurrent random interval-random interval and progressive ratio)in rats Neurotoxicol Teratol 21:679-687 (1999).

(98.) Rice DC, Hayward S. Lack of effect of 3,3',4,4',5-pentachlorobiphenyl (PCB 126) throughout gestation and lactation on multiple fixed interval-fixed ratio and DRL performance in rats. Neurotoxicol Teratol 20:645-650 (1996)

(99.) Rice DC. Effect of exposure to 3,3',4,4',5-pentachlorobiphenyl (PCB 126) throughout gestation and lactation on development and spatial delayed alternation performance in rats. Neurotoxicol Teratol 21:59-69 (1999).

(100.) Bushnell PJ, Rice DC. Behavioral assessments of learning and attention in rats exposed perinatally to 3,3',4,4',5-pentachlorobiphenyl (PCB 126). Neurotoxicol Teratol 21:381-392 (1999)

(101) Luine V, Matinez C, Villegas M, Magarinos A, McEwen B. Restraint stress reversibly enhances spatial memory performance. Physiol Behav 59:27-32 (1996).

(102) Yau J, Olsson T, Morris G, Meaney M, Secki J. Glucocorticoids, hippocampal corticosteroid receptor gene expression and antidepressant treatment: relationship with spatial learning in young and aged rats. Neuroscience 66:571-581 (1995)

(103.) Ronis MJJ, Badger TM, Shema SJ, Roberson PK, Shaikh F. Reproductive toxicity and growth effects in rats exposed to lead at different periods during development. Toxicol Appl Pharmacol 38:361-371 (1996).

(104.) Vyskocil A, Fiala Z, Ettlerova E, Tenjnorova I. Influence of chronic lead exposure on hormone levels in developing rats. J Appl Toxicol 10:301-302 (1990).

(105.) Der R, Yousef M, Fahim Z, Fahim M. Effects of lead and cadmium on adrenal and thyroid function in rats. Res Comm Chem Pethol Pharmacol 17:237-253 (1977).

(106.) McGivern RF, Sokol RZ, Berman NG. Prenatal lead exposure in the rat during the third week of gestation: long-term behavioral, physiological, and anatomical effects associated with reproduction. Toxicol Appl Pharmacol 110:206-215 (1991).

(107.) Ronis MJJ, Badger TM, Shema SJ, Roberson PK, Templer L, Ringer D, Thomas PE. Endocrine mechanisms underlying the growth effects of developmental lead exposure in the rat. J Toxicol Environ Health 54:101-120 (1998).

(108.) Sartorio A, Conti A, Molinari E, Riva G, Morabito F, Faglia G. Growth, growth hormone and cognitive functions. Horm Res 45:23-29 (1996).

(109.) Bellinger DC. Interpreting the literature on lead and child development: the neglected role of the experimental system. Neurotoxicol Teratol 17:201-212 (1995).

(110.) Bushnell PJ, Bowman RE. Reversal learning deficits in young monkeys exposed to lead. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 10:733-742 (1979).

(111.) Rice DC. Lead-induced behavioral impairment on a spatial discrimination reversal task in monkeys exposed during different periods of development. Toxicol Appl Pharmacol 108:327-333 (1990).

(112.) Hilson JA, Strupp BJ. Analyses of response patterns clarity lead effects in olfactory reversal and extradimensional shift tasks: assessment of inhibitory control, associative ability and memory. Behav Neurosci 111:532-542 (1997).

(113.) Alber SA, Strupp BJ. An in-depth analysis of lead effects in a delayed spatial alternation task: assessment of mnemonic effects, side bias, and proactive interference. Neurotoxicol Teratol 18:3-15 (1996).

(114.) Rice DC, Gilbert SG. Lack of sensitive period for lead-induced behavioral impairment on a spatial delayed alternation task in monkeys. Toxicol Appl Pharmacol 103:364-373 (1990).

(115.) Levin ED, Bowman RE. Long-term lead effects on the Hamilton search task and delayed spatial alternation in monkeys. Neurobehav Toxicol Teratol 8:219-224 (1986).

(116.) Cory-Slechta DA, Weiss B, Cox C. Performance and exposure indices of rats exposed to low concentrations of lead. Toxicol Appl Pharmacol 78:291-299 (1985).

(117.) Mele PC, Bushnell PJ, Bowman RE. Prolonged behavioral effects of early postnatal lead exposure in rhesus monkeys: fixed-interval responding and interactions with scopolamine and pentobarbital. Neurobehav Toxicol Teratol 6:129-135 (1984).

(118.) Rice DC. Effect of lead on schedule-controlled behavior in monkeys. In: Behavioral Pharmacology: The Current Status (Seiden LS, Balster RL, eds). New York:Alan R Liss Inc., 1985;473-486.

(119.) Rice DC, Gilbert SG. Low lead exposure from birth produces behavioral toxicity (DRL) in monkeys. Toxicol Appl Pharmacol 80:421-426 (1985).

(120.) Rice DC. Chronic low-lead exposure from birth produces deficits in discrimination-reversal in monkeys. Toxicol Appl Pharmacol 77:201-210 (1985).

(121.) Gilbert SG, Rice DC. Low-level lifetime lead exposure produces behavioral toxicity (spatial discrimination-reversal) in adult monkeys. Toxicol Appl Pharmacol 91:484-490 (1987).

(122.) Zheng W, Shen H, Blaner WS, Zhao Q, Ren X, Graziano JH. Chronic lead exposure alters transthyretin concentration in rat cerebrospinal fluid: the role of the choroid plexus Toxicol Appl Pharmacol 139:445-450 (1996).

(123.) Van Gelder GA. Behavioral toxicologic studies of dieldrin, DDT, and ruelene in sheep. In: Behavioral Toxicology (Weiss B, Laties VG, ads). New York:Plenum Press, 1975;217-239.

(124.) Smith RM, Cunningham WL, Van Gelder GA. Dieldrin toxicity and successive discrimination reversal in squirrel monkeys. J Toxicol Environ Health 1:737-747 (1976).

(125.) Burt GS. Use of behavioral techniques in the assessment of environmental contaminants. In: Behavioral Toxicology (Weiss B, Laties VG, eds). New York:Plenum Press, 1975;241-263.

(126.) Olson KL, Boush GM, Matsumura F. Pre- and postnatal exposure to dieldrin: persistent stimulatory and behavioral effects. Pestic Biochem Pharmacol 13:20-33 (1980).

(127.) Rivera S, Rosa R, Marinez E, Sunol C, Serrano MT, Vendrell M, Rodriguez-Farre E, Sanfeliu C. Behavioral and monoaminergic changes after lindane exposure in developing rats. Neurotoxicol Pharmacol 20:155-160 (1998).

(128.) Tilson HA, Shaw S, McLamb RL. The effects of lindane, DOT and chlordecone on avoidance responding and seizure activity. Toxicol Appl Pharmacol 88:57-65 (1987).

(129.) Paul V, Balasubramaniam E, Sheela S, Krishnamoorthy MS. Effects of endosulfan and aldrin on muscle coordination and conditioned avoidance response in rats. Pharmacol Toxicol 71:254-257 (1992).

(130.) Paul V, Balasubramaniam E, Kazi M. The neurobehavioral toxicity of endosulfan in rats: a serotonergic involvement in learning impairment. Eur J Pharmacol 270:1-7 (1994).

(131.) Paul V, Balasubramaniam E, Jayakumar AR, Kazi M. A sex-related difference in the neurobehavioral and hepatic effects following chronic endosulfan treatment in rats. Eur J Pharmacol 293:355-360 (1995).

(132.) Tilson HA, Squibb RE, Burne TA. Neurobehavioral effects following a single dose of chlordecone (Kepone) administered neonatally to rats. Neurotoxicology 3:45-52 (1982).

(133.) Mactutus CF, Unger KL, Tilson HA. Neonatal chlordecone exposure impairs early learning and memory in the rat on a multiple measure passive avoidance task. Neurotoxicology 3:27-44 (1982).

(134.) Mactutus CF, Tilson HA. Neonatal chlordecone exposure impairs early learning and retention of active avoidance in the rat. Neurobehav Toxicol Teratol 6:75-83 (1984).

(135.) Uppal RP, Garg BD, Ahmad A. Effect of malathion and DDT on the action of some tranquilizers on learning and memory traces in rats. Indian J Exp Biol 21:617-619 (1983).

(136.) Uppal RP, Garg BD, Ahmad A. Effect of malathion and DOT on the action of some chlorpromazine and diazepam with reference to conditioned avoidance response in rats. Indian J Exp Biol 21:254-257 (1983).

(137.) Sobotka TJ. Behavioral effects of low doses of DDT. Proc Soc Exp Biol Med 137:952-955 (1971).

(138.) Carr RL, Bertasi FR, Betancourt AM. Effects of developmental bispenol A exposure on cognition in rats. Toxicologist 60(1):113 (2001).

(139.) Goldey ES, Crofton KM. Thyroxine replacement attenuates hypothyroxinemia, hearing loss and motor deficits following developmental exposure to Aroclor 1254 in rats. Toxicol Sci 45:94-105 (1998).

(140.) Pessah IN, Wong PW. Etiology of PCB neurotoxicity: from molecules to cellular dysfunction. In: Recent Advances in the Environmental Toxicology and Health Effects of PCBs (Robertson L, Hansen L, eds). Lexington, KY:University of Kentucky Press (in press).

(141.) Guilarte, TR, Miceli, RC, Jett, DA. Biochemical evidence of an interaction of lead at the zinc allosteric sites of the NMDA receptor complex: effects of neuronal development. Neurotoxicology 16:63-72 (1995).

(142.) Al-Hachim GM, Al-Baker A. Effects of chlordane on conditioned avoidance response, brain seizure threshold and open-field performance of prenatally treated mice. Br J Pharmacol 49:311-315 (1973).

(143.) Desi I. Neurotoxological effect of small quantities of lindane. Animal studies. Int Arch Arbeitsmed 33(2):153-162 (1974).

(144.) Evangelista de Duffard AM, Orta C, Duffard R. Behavioral changes in rats fed a diet containing 2,4-dichlorophenoxy-acetic butyl ester. Neurotoxicology 11:563-572 (1990).

(145.) Peruzovic M, Kniewald J, Capkun V, Milkovic K. Effect of atrazine ingested prior to mating on rat females and their offspring. Acta Physiol Hung 83:79-89 (1995).

(146.) Lehotzky K, Ungvary G, Polinak D, Kiss A. Behavioral deficits due to prenatal exposure to cadmium chloride in CFY rat pups. Neurotoxicol Teratol 12:169-172 (1990).

(147.) Newland MC, Ng WW, Baggs RB, Gentry GD, Weiss B, Miller RK. Operant behavior in transition reflects neonatal exposure to cadmium. Teratology 34:231-241 (1986).

(148.) Gilbert SG, Grant-Webster KS. Neurobehavioral effects of developmental methylmercury exposure. Environ Health Perspect 103(suppl 6):135-142 (1995).

(149.) Rice DC. Sensory and cognitive effects of developmental methylmercury exposure in monkeys and a comparison to effects in rodents. Neurotoxicology 17:139-154 (1996).

Address correspondence to S.L. Schantz, Department of Veterinary Biosciences, College of Veterinary Medicine, University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign, 2001 S. Lincoln Avenue, Urbana, IL 61802 USA. Telephone: (217) 333-6230. Fax: (217) 244-1652. E-mail: schantz@uiuc.edu

We thank Geoff B. Clarkson for his help with the review and revision of this manuscript.

Received 20 July 1999: accepted 4 May 2001.

COPYRIGHT 2001 National Institute of Environmental Health Sciences
COPYRIGHT 2004 Gale Group

联系我们|关于我们|网站声明
国家哲学社会科学文献中心版权所有